MINDFULNESS AND CHILDREN WITH ADHD Mindfulness-Based Intervention and Cognitive Function of Children with Attention DeficitHyperactivity Disorder Melissa K. Craft Towson, University Research Methods in Psychology PSYC.314, Spring 2013 1 MINDFULNESS AND CHILDREN WITH ADHD 2 Abstract The research community has increasingly been investigating the impact of mindfulness on overall physical and mental health. Much of the research has demonstrated an association between mindfulness and cognitive processes of attention and mood regulation. With Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder being characterized by problems of cognition, particularly related to attention and learning, the effectiveness of Mindfulness-based interventions on improving cognition in this population is in question. The current study assessed the impact of a 16-week Mindfulness-based intervention program on the spatial learning in the Virtual Morris Water Maze task of 25 children with diagnosed ADHD and 25 children with no ADHD, ranging from ages 8-9 years. Relative to age-matched no treatment control groups, the children with ADHD and the children with no ADHD responded differently to the program. There was a crossover interaction between the mindfulness treatment and the children’s level of cognitive function, as the children with ADHD demonstrated significantly improved learning performance and the children with no ADHD demonstrated significantly impaired learning performance following the 16-week intervention. The findings provide evidence that mindfulness-based interventions are associated with improvements in spatial-cognitive learning in children with ADHD, but with learning impairments in children with no ADHD. Keywords: mindfulness, adhd, learning MINDFULNESS AND CHILDREN WITH ADHD 3 Mindfulness-Based Intervention and Cognitive Function of Children with Attention DeficitHyperactivity Disorder Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is a condition primarily diagnosed in childhood and marked by cognitive deficits associated with difficulty in sustained attention and self-regulation (Baron, 2007; Rutledge, Bos, McClure, & Schweitzer, 2012). These deficits often manifest from symptoms such as over-activity, difficulty following directions, poor academic performance, difficulty finishing tasks, difficulty holding memories, difficulty avoiding distraction, difficulty organizing or planning, and difficulty controlling impulses (Baron, 2007; Daley & Birchwood, 2010; Ek, Westerlund, Holmberg, & Fernell, 2011). Primary brain structures associated with dysfunction in the case of ADHD have been noted in areas of the brain including the prefrontal and cingulate cortices (Castellanos & Proal, 2012). In addition, significant widespread thinning across parts of cortices, including the frontal, medial and left lateral parietal, occipital and temporal cortices, have been observed in direct relation to individuals with ADHD (Narr, 2009). This cortical thinning has been associated with the cognitive dysfunctions related to this condition. Currently, many children with ADHD are often treated pharmacologically and/or with cognitive behavioral interventions in order to improve cognitive and behavioral deficits (DuPaul, Eckert, Vilardo, & Brigid, 2012; Rutledge et al., 2012). However, upcoming research has shed light on an alternative method of treatment that has been associated with cognitive and behavioral improvements among both cognitively normal and impaired populations. Within recent years, a growing body of research has surfaced exploring the impact of mindfulness on cognition and its implications in regards to potential treatment programs in some MINDFULNESS AND CHILDREN WITH ADHD 4 cases of cognitive dysfunction (Chiesa, Calati, & Serretti, 2011). Mindfulness practice has been linked to improvements in emotional regulation, mood, cognitive functioning, and even immune functioning (Davidson et al., 2003; Josefsson & Broberg, 2011; Kozhevnikov, Louchakova, Josipovic, & Motes, 2009; Lykins & Bear, 2009; Miller, Fletcher, & Kabat-Zinn, 1995; Witkiewitz, Bowen, Douglas, & Hsu, 2013). Mindfulness can be defined as a meditative process of purposefully and fully attending to the present moment, allowing for awareness of all components of the immediate experience while remaining free of judgment (Bishop et al., 2004; Kabat-Zinn, 1994). Historically, mindfulness has been used as part of a daily religious practice (Bishop et al., 2004). It was used frequently among Buddhist monks who practiced mindfulness as part of a daily spiritual regimen. However, modern practices of mindfulness meditation have been applied clinically and into everyday life without the connection between practice and any religious affiliation (Brown, Marquis, & Guiffrida, 2013). In regards to the ways in which mindfulness practice has been used for intervention purposes, most research has focused on its implications for improving symptoms associated with anxiety, depression, chronic pain and emotional regulation (Biegel, Shapiro, Brown, & Schubert, 2009; Brown et al., 2013; Miller et al., 1995; Wiltkiewitz et al., 2013). Many of these improvements seem to be associated with the nature of the overall goal of mindfulness, which is to shift from a generally negative to positive thinking style and to adopt a positive and nonjudgmental approach toward experiences that occur during the present moment (Bishop et al., 2004; Kabat-Zinn, 1994). Specific evidence of emotional and mood improvements following a mindfulness-based intervention have been noted in studies observing a variety of different conditions. One study found that a mindfulness intervention program was efficient in reducing substance abuse relapse MINDFULNESS AND CHILDREN WITH ADHD 5 in individuals who were going through alcohol dependency recovery (Wiltkiewitz et al., 2013). Another study noted the long-term outcome of a mindfulness meditation intervention on a group of people with various anxiety disorders (Miller et al., 1995). In this population, a significant decrease in trait anxiety was observed as a result of a mindfulness program even after a threeyear follow up. Furthermore, one other study observed significant decreases in self- ratings of depression and somatic distress accompanied by improvements in self-esteem in adolescent psychiatric outpatients who had completed a mindfulness-based intervention program (Biegel et al., 2009). With these results in mind, the potential efficacy of a mindfulness approach potentially treating a wide variety of mental health disorders seems widely supported. Although the most widespread uses for mindfulness-based intervention approaches have been used with a focus on mood dysfunction and emotional regulation, particularly noteworthy evidence has been emerging in more recent research demonstrating its association with improved cognitive functioning (Chan & Woollacott, 2007; Kozhevnikov et al., 2009; Lykins & Baer, 2009). Using both brain imaging techniques and behavioral methods of measurement, researchers continue to discover additional ways in which mindfulness practice may benefit cognition. These experiments have involved the observation of Buddhist monks, experienced mindfulness practitioners and those with only short-term mindfulness experienced by way of treatment program (Chan & Woollacott, 2007; Kozhevnikov et al., 2009; Lykins & Baer, 2009; Zeidan, Johnson, Diamond, David, & Goolkasian, 2010). Regardless of the length of experience, significant cognitive and behavioral improvements have been noted in response to mindfulness practice (Zeidan et al., 2010). In regard to cognition, mindfulness has been linked to improvements in sustained attention, executive attention, self-regulation, and spatial memory (Chambers, Lo, & Allen, MINDFULNESS AND CHILDREN WITH ADHD 6 2008; Chan & Woollacott, 2007; Kozhevnikov et al., 2009; Zeidan et al., 2010). One study observed the impact of deity yoga meditation, which involves sustaining one’s attention towards a specific personal object of deity, on spatial processing (Kozhevnikov et al., 2009). This study found that when tested after meditation, those participants involved in deity yoga meditation demonstrated significant improvements on tasks of mental rotation and spatial memory when compared to control groups of both non-meditators and experienced meditators who did not meditate prior to the test. Another study found that daily mindfulness meditation was significantly associated with less interference on the Stroop task, which is a measure of executive attention (Chan & Woollacott, 2007). Similarly, one other study found that when compared to a control group of no mindfulness meditation, participants who took part in a brief mindfulness meditation program demonstrated significant improvements on cognitive tasks of working memory and verbal fluency (Zeidan et al., 2010). These mindfulness participants also demonstrated improved self-ratings of fatigue and anxiety. These results have been only part of the recent movement to discover how these cognitive improvements come about and what specific parts of the brain are impacted by mindfulness meditation. For this reason, an abundance of studies have risen to the forefront demonstrating the measurable impact of mindfulness meditation on the brain. Various brain imaging techniques have been used to measure cortical thickness as a result of mindfulness training and also measure activation of particular cortical areas during the mindfulness meditation (Cahn & Polich, 2009; Engstrom, Pihlsgard, Lundberg, & Soderfeldt, 2010; Grant et al., 2013; Holzel et al., 2011; Moore, Gruber, Derose, & Malinowski, 2012). One study, in particular, evaluated the cortical thickness of the entire brain of experienced mindfulness meditators and a control group of nonmeditators (Grant et al., 2013). Their results showed that the experienced meditators reported a MINDFULNESS AND CHILDREN WITH ADHD 7 tendency to became more absorbed in mindfulness practice and scored higher on levels of mindfulness in general. These results were also positively correlated with significant increases in grey matter thickness measured in the cingulo-frontal-parietal network of attention of the experienced meditators. Similarly, one other study also noted significant increases in grey matter concentration in the left hippocampus, cerebellum, posterior cingulated cortex, and the temporoparietal junction (Holzel, 2011). These increases were observed in participants of an 8-week mindfulness intervention program when compared to a no mindfulness control group. Furthermore, another study used an EEG recording to measure Evoked-Related Potentials in different cortical areas of individuals who participated in a 16-week mindfulness program when compared to a no mindfulness control group (Moore et al., 2012). They measured ERPs of these individuals during a computerized Stroop task of attention and compared the data to the pattern of cortical activation related to the task. These researchers found, when compared to controls, those who participated in the mindfulness program allocated more efficient attentional resources to cortical areas specifically associated with the assigned task. This was demonstrated by an increase of activation in task relevant cortical areas in the presence of congruent stimuli and less activation in the presence of incongruent stimuli. The no meditation control group demonstrated the opposite effect. Likewise, a similar study found that experienced mindfulness practitioners demonstrated a decrease in ERP activation in response to distracting stimuli and an increase in such activation in response to the target stimuli during active mindfulness meditation (Cahn & Polich, 2009). These results were found to be significantly different from participants in a control group who engaged in neutral thinking rather than mindfulness meditation. Comparatively, the control group demonstrated an increased ERP response to the distracter stimuli and responded with weaker activation to the target response. These studies provide MINDFULNESS AND CHILDREN WITH ADHD 8 support for the potential benefits of mindfulness practice in regard to improving allocation of attentional resources. The present study will focus on the potential implications for a mindfulness based intervention program for improving cognitive function in children diagnosed with ADHD. As noted previously, ADHD is largely characterized by significant decreases in cortical gray matter in the prefrontal cortex (Narr et al., 2009). Subsequently, it has been associated with diminished ability to execute resources of attention properly and in turn individuals with ADHD will often display weakened learning performance (Baron, 2007; Daley & Birchwood, 2010; Ek et al., 2011). With Mindfulness-based meditation seeming to directly impact these areas of cognitive function in the brain and based on its noted success in improving conditions in cases of other types of cognitive dysfunction, whether or not it could successfully improve cognition in children with ADHD is currently an intriguing question worthy of further investigation. Previous studies and reviews have evaluated the potential connections between mindfulness and ADHD and how mindfulness-based interventions may improve cognitive function and performance in children diagnosed with ADHD. One study investigated the mindfulness skills of adults with ADHD compared to those without ADHD (Smalley, Loo, Hale, Shrestha, & McGough, 2009). They found through self-report measures that adults with ADHD reported lower trait mindfulness, particularly in regard to attention measures, than individuals without ADHD. These results suggest a negative correlation between ADHD and trait mindfulness. Additionally, noteworthy literature has highlighted the associations between mindfulness and ADHD (Grant et al., 2013; Burke, 2010). They have emphasized the importance of future research to explore if these associations are true indicators of any measurable outcomes of improvement in individuals with the disorder as a result of mindfulness-based interventions. MINDFULNESS AND CHILDREN WITH ADHD 9 Few studies have actually explored the measurable cognitive impact of a mindfulnessbased intervention on children with ADHD. One study did measure parent ratings of their child’s behavior following a mindfulness-based intervention program for both the parents and the child (Oord, Bogels, & Peijnenburg, 2012). Results indicated that parents noted significant improvement in ADHD behaviors following the treatment. However, this study did not investigate specific cognitive functions as a result of the treatment and therefore true improvements in cognitive performance could not be examined. Another study investigated the efficacy of a 16-week mindfulness-based intervention program for adolescents with ADHD (Weijer-Bergsma, Formsma, Bruin, & Bogels, 2012). In this study, the self-reports and parent and tutor reports revealed significantly improved executive functioning and behavior in the adolescents following the intervention. Significant improvements in attention were also demonstrated on measurable tests of attention performance. This study, however, only investigated the impact of such a treatment on adolescents ranging from ages 11-15 years. There is still very little information in regards to the efficacy of a mindfulness-based intervention program for children younger than 10 years. Consequently, we believe the next step in evaluating the efficacy of a mindfulness-based intervention program on improving cognitive performance of children with ADHD is to begin by highlighting a specific cognitive task related to the typical cognitive dysfunction associated with the condition. Because areas associated with executive function, such as the frontal cortices, are particularly impaired in children with ADHD, an intervention approach designed to improve this function would be desirable. According to Baddeley and Hitch’s (1974) model of working memory, the central executive (executive function center) of the brain is responsible for controlling the flow of information that travels in and out of two specific centers of short-term MINDFULNESS AND CHILDREN WITH ADHD 10 memory storage (Baddeley & Hitch, 1974). These two systems are the phonological loop, responsible for storing verbal information, and the visuo-spatial loop, responsible for storing visual and spatial information. This model could suggest that dysfunction in the central executive could be a primary contributor to impaired performance in learning and memory. Based on this model, if executive function can be improved, then attention may be allocated more efficiently to specific systems of the brain responsible for learning, memory and behavior. Potentially, such an improvement could lead to more generalized improvements in academic performance and daily cognitive tasks. The present study is going to focus particularly on the impact that a mindfulness-based intervention program may have on spatial learning in children with ADHD. It has been demonstrated that such learning may require the proper functioning of the central executive portion of the brain and therefore improvements of this system should lead to improvements in learning (Purser et al., 2012). This study will consist of four groups with two of the groups comprised of children with diagnosed ADHD and two groups of normal functioning children. One ADHD group and one normal functioning group of children will participate in a 16-week mindfulness-based intervention program as well as their parents, while the other two groups will participate in a control program not affiliated with any type of mindfulness practice. We will measure each group’s spatial-cognitive learning over three different time points through the use of a virtual version of the Morris Water Maze (Morris, 1981). It is hypothesized that the group of children with diagnosed ADHD will demonstrate improved spatial-cognitive learning as an outcome of the Mindfulness-Based Program, and may achieve acquisition performance on par with their normal functioning same age peers. In addition, we expect to observe improvements in MINDFULNESS AND CHILDREN WITH ADHD 11 the normal cognitive functioning children who participate in the Mindfulness-Based Program when compared to those who did not participate. Method Participants The participants gathered for this study consisted of 50 children with diagnosed Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder and 50 children with no identifiable cognitive impairments. Children ranged from 8-9 years-of-age, were all in the third grade, and were English-speaking. Apart from ADHD, none of the children in any of the groups had any associated disabilities that may have interfered with performance in this study. All children were also diverse in race, socioeconomic status, and gender. The children with ADHD were recruited from The Center for ADHD in Cincinnati, Ohio, one of the largest centers in the United States devoted to the research and care of individuals diagnosed with ADHD. For recruitment, a 50 slot sign-up sheet was made available to parents in the center informing them of the study and briefly what it involved. Because the nature of the study required that children not take medications during the experiment, for it very well may have confounded the results of the Mindfulness-based intervention, parents of newly diagnosed children and parents who were seeking a naturalistic approach to treatment were among the majority who signed up. The 50 third grade children with normal cognitive abilities were recruited from three of the surrounding elementary schools including Maple Dale Elementary School, Hoffman Elementary School, and Lincoln Heights Elementary School. A brief description of the study was sent out to children’s homes in the schools’ monthly newsletters and information regarding the sign up procedure was included in the explanation. Parents from both The Center for ADHD MINDFULNESS AND CHILDREN WITH ADHD 12 and the elementary schools were assured that there were no health-risks associated with the experiment and were compensated with $100.00 cash for participation. Materials The Virtual Morris Water Maze task was executed on a computerized program designed to replicate the environment and apparatus of the original Morris Water Maze. The program used was the NeuroInvestigations Virtual Navigation Software Morris Water Task Version 1.2 and was displayed on a Sony VAIO laptop model VGN-A260. Participants received audio feedback via Sony MDR-ZX100 Stereo Headphones and used the arrow keys on the keyboard to navigate around the water maze. For reference, Hamilton and Sutherland (1999) provided a detailed description of the environment within the computerized program. In summary, the virtual environment consisted of a circle shaped pool with white pool walls. The pool walls had no distinguishable features and looked the same at every angle. The room in which the pool was in had 4 walls, each the same length and width and each containing one distinguishable cue. These cues served as distal cues, however were placed in such a way that the target could not be reached by directly approaching one specific cue head on. The target platform was placed in the center of the Northeast quadrant of the circular pool and took up approximately 2% of the surface area of the pool. The target remained invisible until it was reached, at which time it rose from the water and was visible to participant. The UP arrow key allowed the participant to move forward in the maze, while the RIGHT and LEFT arrow keys enabled turning directions in the maze. The DOWN arrow key was not used, as the participant could not move backwards in the maze. Procedure MINDFULNESS AND CHILDREN WITH ADHD 13 This study used a 2x2 factorial design consisting of a total of 4 groups. The 50 children with ADHD were assigned to two groups of 25 by flipping a coin, one group receiving the mindfulness intervention treatment and the other receiving no treatment. The 50 children with normal cognitive functioning were assigned to two groups of 25 as well by flipping a coin, one receiving the mindfulness treatment and the other receiving no treatment. Children completed the virtual spatial-cognitive learning task at three different time points: before the 16-week intervention, after the 9th week, and after the 16th week. Children in the no treatment group completed the task at these time points as well, just did not participate in the mindfulness intervention program. So as to not deny some children treatment, parents were informed before agreeing to allow their children to participate in the study that if their child was placed in the no treatment group, they would be given the opportunity to receive the mindfulness treatment intervention free of charge if they so chose to after the study was completed. The mindfulness intervention treatment was a 16-week program that required the parents and children to attend an hour-long initial mindfulness introduction session the first week followed by 15 hour-long sessions that were held once a week. Twelve of the children in the ADHD treatment group met on Monday nights at 6:00 pm and the other 13 children in the ADHD treatment group met on Tuesday nights at 6:00 pm. Twelve of the children in the normal functioning treatment group met on Wednesday nights at 6:00 pm and the remaining 13 children in the normal function treatment group met on Thursday nights at 6:00 pm. During the initial session, two professional mindfulness instructors who both had over 10 years of experience studying and practicing mindfulness trained parents and children. Children and parents during this time were given an introduction to mindfulness as a practice and were informed of what the following 15 weeks were to entail. Children were instructed to keep a daily MINDFULNESS AND CHILDREN WITH ADHD 14 journal of their experience with the exercises taught each week. Parents were instructed to practice the exercise taught each week with the child twice every day. The session ended with a 5 minute guided meditation for both the parents and the children. During the following 8 weeks, parents and children were taught techniques and exercises to improve attention and focus. One mindfulness concept and exercise was taught each week. The concepts included awareness of an object, awareness of self in the environment, attending to the senses, awareness of movement, meditation on the breath, attending to the thinking process, meditation on the bubble, and finally visualization meditation. A more detailed description of each of the concepts and related exercises was provided by Hooker and Fodor (2008). These exercises were taught and explained to each child and the child, trainer and parent would all practice the exercise together. Each of these 8 sessions concluded with a 5 minute guided meditation. The sessions during the last 7 weeks were based mostly around group discussion and sharing of experiences with mindfulness. Children would talk about their experience of mindfulness with the other children. Each child in the group discussion was given an opportunity to share what they liked, disliked, what was difficult, what was easy, how they felt when they were mindful and how they have been practicing. The parents shared their experiences with mindfulness as well and discussed what exercises were the most challenging, what was the most helpful, and any improvements they had seen in their children. Parents were instructed to continue their practice of different exercises with their child twice a day and children were to continue their daily journals of their experiences. Each of these sessions concluded with a 5 minute guided meditation. MINDFULNESS AND CHILDREN WITH ADHD 15 For the Spatial-Cognitive Learning Task, there were two testing rooms with five computers in each room. Ten children could complete the task at a time and the entire process took approximately 20-30 minutes per child. Testing at each time period for all children took place on one day. There was one experimenter for each room that gave a specific set of directions to each group of children regarding how to complete the task. The children were instructed to use the arrow keys as noted above to maneuver around the water maze. The task consisted of 3 main phases: practice trials, place-learning trials, and a probe trial. For the first phase, there were four practice trials, during which time the platform was visible and the participants were told to swim to the platform. For each trial the participants had an opportunity to practice swimming from each of the four potential starting points (North, East, South, West). This was to get each participant accustomed to the nature of the task and have them practice using the arrow keys to swim. Next, there were 20 place-learning trials, during which time participants were told that their goal was to swim to a hidden platform in the water pool. Once the goal was approached, the platform would rise and the participant would be given a five second time interval to look around and observe the environment. If the target was not approached within a 60 second time frame, the platform became visible and the participant was notified by a beep and visual instruction that the platform was visible and to swim to it. The target platform remained in a stable location in the center of the N/E quadrant for all of the place-learning trials, but the starting location of the participant changed from trial to trial between the four possible start points. Each participant was informed of the platform location constancy prior to beginning the task. During this time, escape latency (time in seconds that it took from the point in which the participant began moving to the time the platform was approached) was calculated in order to measure place-learning. MINDFULNESS AND CHILDREN WITH ADHD 16 Finally, there was one 45 second probe trial, in which the platform was removed from the pool and the participants were pseudo-randomly placed at a one of the two furthest starting points from the target area in the place-learning phase. At this point, duration of time in seconds spent in each quadrant was calculated in order to measure place-learning. Results Using a 2x2 (Cognitive Function x Mindfulness Treatment) factorial analysis of variance (ANOVA), we examined whether an escape latency change on the Virtual Morris Water Maze Task would occur following a 16-week mindfulness training program in children with ADHD and in an age-matched control group without ADHD. Escape latency measures for each group are displayed in Figure 1. There was no significant main effect of the cognitive function level of the children [F (1,36) = 1.073, p > .05]. Likewise, there was no significant main effect of the Mindfulness Treatment manipulation [F (1,36) = .800, p > .05]. However, the results showed that there was a significant interaction between cognitive function level and the meditation treatment manipulation [F (1,36) = 12.262, p < .01]. As reflected in Figure 1, there was a crossover interaction between the mindfulness treatment and the children’s level of cognitive function. The children with no ADHD escaped slower in response to the mindfulness treatment as opposed to when there was no treatment. The opposite occurred in children with ADHD, however, as they escaped faster following the MINDFULNESS AND CHILDREN WITH ADHD 17 mindfulness treatment in comparison to a condition in which there was no treatment. Figure 1: Mean escape latencies for control and treatment groups among children with ADHD and children with No ADHD. Discussion There is a growing body of research supporting that mindfulness-based interventions may have positive implications for individuals suffering from a wide variety of mental health problems (Chan & Woollacott, 2007; Chiesa et al., 2011; Kabat-Zinn, 1995; Kozhevnikov et al., 2009; Miller et al., 1995). This research has been overwhelmingly limited to adolescents or adults, however, and much less has been evaluated in regards to children’s responses to mindfulness. Our analysis of the performance of children with ADHD on a spatial-cognitive learning task following a mindfulness intervention supports the hypothesis that children with ADHD may benefit cognitively from practicing mindfulness. Following a 16-week mindfulness intervention, children diagnosed with ADHD did, in fact, demonstrate improved learning performance on the Virtual Morris Water Maze task in comparison to those children with ADHD who did not participate in the treatment. Part of what we hypothesized, however, was that the children with no ADHD would benefit from the mindfulness treatment as well. Contrary to this MINDFULNESS AND CHILDREN WITH ADHD 18 prediction, our findings suggest that the mindfulness treatment actually impaired performance on the Virtual Morris Water Maze task in normal cognitive functioning children. As suggested by our results, it seems that a mindfulness intervention may be a useful treatment tool for improving learning in children with ADHD, but may be detrimental to the spatial-cognitive learning performance of children with no diagnosed cognitive dysfunction. Although past mindfulness research that has specifically involved child participants with ADHD is limited, there has been some research to suggest that mindfulness practice could very well lead to cognitive and behavioral improvements among this population. The improvements observed in the learning performance of children with ADHD as a response to the mindfulness based treatment during our study are consistent with past studies, and provide useful information to add to this developing collection of research. For example, Oord et al. (2012) also found improvements in children with ADHD following an 8-week mindfulness program. Specifically, these researchers found that the treatment reduced the children’s parent-rated ADHD behavior and parental stress related to the child’s ADHD. Likewise, Singh et al. (2010) observed improved compliance of children with ADHD following a 12-session mindfulness training program for both the child with ADHD and mother. These improvements were most robust in cases in which the training was given to both the children and the mother, rather than just the mother. This further suggests that the child, in fact, benefitted from the training itself and not just from the mother’s reaction to the training. It should be noted, as well, that during the course of the mindfulness treatment, the children involved in the study were slowly stripped off of their medication for ADHD without any recognizable adverse effects. By the same token, the specific improvements observed in the ADHD participants in our study also are consistent with the past research regarding the impact of mindfulness on specific MINDFULNESS AND CHILDREN WITH ADHD 19 cognitive processes, although not specifically in children. Kozhevnikov et al. (2009) also found evidence that practicing mindfulness meditation may directly impact an individual’s performance on tasks of mental rotation and spatial memory in a positive way. Likewise, our results support the findings of Chan and Woollacott (2007), whose evidence suggested that mindfulness practice was associated with an improvement in the allocation of attentional resources during a test of executive function. This finding may provide some insight as to why a mindfulness-based intervention was so beneficial to the children with ADHD in our study. The Virtual Morris Water Maze task would require a level of executive function that children with ADHD generally lack. Therefore, perhaps as a function of regular mindfulness practice, their ability to allocate their attentional resources efficiently during the Virtual Water Maze task improved, thus leading to some of the improvements demonstrated in our results. We did find, however, that children functioning at a normal cognitive level did not benefit from the mindfulness-based intervention like the children with ADHD did. In fact, these children were actually impaired by the treatment. This finding was unexpected, as it does not so smoothly fall in line with the past research regarding mindfulness. Nonetheless, these results are important findings that provide us with more insight as to how some children may react to mindfulness-based treatments. Grant et al. (2013) found evidence to suggest that elevated selfratings of mindfulness practice absorption were positively correlated with increased grey matter thickness in areas of the brain responsible for attention processes. This may provide us with a potential explanation as to why a lack of improvement occurred among this population. It is possible that the parents of the normal functioning children were less motivated during the treatment than the parents of children with ADHD to practice consistently with their children. Therefore, there may have been less home practice and less absorption of mindfulness. This MINDFULNESS AND CHILDREN WITH ADHD 20 possibility, however, does not provide a sufficient explanation as to why their performance was so greatly impaired following the treatment rather than just remaining unchanged. Perhaps these children may have responded in a negative way to the mindfulness treatment due to an inability to grasp the concept of the mindfulness practice. These children may have become frustrated with the training and this, in turn, may have negatively impacted their performance on the Virtual Water Maze task. Why this effect was not observed among the children with ADHD remains rather ambiguous, however, and should be evaluated further in future studies. It is important to address some of the potential downfalls of the study and how they may have impacted the results. As mentioned above, the motivation of both the parents of the children with ADHD and the parents of the children with no ADHD could have been more heavily controlled for. Whereas the parents of the children with ADHD were highly motivated to hopefully observe improvements in their children following the treatment, the parents of the normal functioning children had less of a reason to desire to see these types of changes. Parents were informed during the introductory session of the many positive outcomes associated with mindfulness practice, however, which should have promoted some level of motivation within all parents involved. In addition, regulation of the parent and child’s practice at home could not be completely controlled. It was impossible to be sure that each parent was consistent with practicing with the child over the full 16-week period. Therefore, while some children may have experienced extensive practice in mindfulness, other children may not have had much more practice than what was done during the weekly sessions. Ultimately, for the main purposes of this study, we found much of the evidence we intended to find. The mindfulness-based intervention did seem to improve the spatial-cognitive MINDFULNESS AND CHILDREN WITH ADHD 21 learning performance of children with ADHD. For a population of children generally characterized by their impaired learning ability and poor academic performance, this evidence provides much promise in regard to the development of new, safe treatment alternatives for children with ADHD. This potential is crucial, as the majority of mothers rate behavioral treatments as a more acceptable form of treatment than stimulant medication for their children (Johnston, Hommersen, & Seipp, 2008). Still, further investigation of the specific impact of mindfulness treatments on this population is necessary before they should be regarded as an appropriate equal alternative to medication. Perhaps future research could explore possible academic improvements in this population in response to a mindfulness intervention. These studies could follow children with ADHD throughout the course of a school year and investigate the impact of a school-based mindfulness program on their grades and teacher-ratings of academic performance. Furthermore, it would also be necessary to continue investigating the impact of mindfulness on normal functioning children, and why it might impair their performance on various levels of cognitive functioning. Perhaps these studies could focus on how well the children are able to grasp the concepts taught in the interventions and track their feelings and frustrations in regards to their practice. It might also be beneficial to explore what might be going on at the neurophysiological level in normal functioning children compared to children with ADHD over the course of a mindfulness-based intervention program. In conclusion, because there is so much yet to discover related to the potential benefits of mindfulness on children with ADHD, surely there are an abundance of avenues for future investigation on the topic. Indeed, while this study might have brought us one step closer to understanding the effects of mindfulness on the brain and its processes, there is still so much more information about this incredible practice to unearth and explore. MINDFULNESS AND CHILDREN WITH ADHD 22 Figure Captions Figure 1: Average escape latencies on Virtual Morris Water Maze are demonstrated in response to either the Treatment condition or the control condition for both the ADHD group and the No ADHD group. Error bars reflect the standard errors of the means. MINDFULNESS AND CHILDREN WITH ADHD 23 References Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 8, 47-89. doi: 10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60452-1 Baron, I. S. (2007). Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity disorder: New challenges for definition, diagnosis, and treatment. Neuropsychology Review, 17, 1-3. doi: 10.1007/s11065-0069016-4 Biegel, G. M., Brown, K. W., Shapiro, S. L., & Schubert, C. M. (2009). Mindfulness-based stress reduction for the treatment of adolescent psychiatric outpatients: A randomized clinical trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 77(5), 855-866. doi: 10.1037/a0016241 Bishop, S.R., Lau, M., Anderson, N. D., Segal, Z. V., Abbey, S., Devins, G., . . . Velting, D. (2004). Mindfulness: A proposed operational definition. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 11(3), 230-241. doi: 10.1093/clipsy/bph077 Brown, A. P., Marquis, A., & Guiffrida, D. A. (2013). Mindfulness-based interventions in counseling. Journal of Counseling & Development, 91, 96-104. Burke, C. (2010). Mindfulness-based approaches with children and adolescents: A preliminary review of current research in an emergent field. Journal of Child & Family Studies, 19(2), 133-144. doi: 10.1007/s10826-009-9282-x Cahn, B. R., & Polich, J. (2009). Meditation (vipassana) and the P3 event-related brain potential. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 72, 51-60. doi: MINDFULNESS AND CHILDREN WITH ADHD 24 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.03.013 Castellanos, F. X., & Proal, E. (2012). Large-scale brain systems in ADHD: Beyond the prefrontal–striatal model. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(1), 17-26. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.007 Chambers, R., Lo, B. C. Y., & Allen, N.B. (2008). The impact of intensive mindfulness training on attentional control, cognitive style, and affect. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 32(3), 303-322. doi: 10.1007/s10608-007-9119-0 Chan, D., & Woollacott, M. (2007). Effects of level of meditation experience on attentional focus: Is the efficiency of executive or orientation networks improved? The Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 13(6), 651-657. doi: 10.1089/acm.2007.7022 Chiesa, A., Calati, R., & Serretti, A. 2011. Does mindfulness training improve cognitive abilities? A systematic review of neuropsychological findings. Clinical Psychology Review, 31, 449-464. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.11.003 Daley, D., & Birchwood, J. (2010). ADHD and academic performance: Why does ADHD impact on academic performance and what can be done to support ADHD children in the classroom? Child: Care, Health & Development, 36(4), 455-464. doi: 10.1111/j.13652214.2009.01046.x Davidson, R. J., Kabat-Zinn, J., Schumacher, J., Rosenkrnz, M., Muller, D., Santorelli, S. F., . . . Sheridan, J. F. (2003). Alterations in brain and immune function produced by mindfulness meditation. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65(4), 564-570. MINDFULNESS AND CHILDREN WITH ADHD 25 DuPaul, Eckert G. J., Vilardo T L., & Brigid. (2012). The effects of school-based interventions for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analysis 1996-2010. School Psychology Review, 41(4), 387. Ek, U., Westerlund, J., Holmberg, K., & Fernell, E. (2011). Academic performance of adolescents with ADHD and other behavioural and learning problems -a populationbased longitudinal study. Acta Paediatrica, 100(3), 402-406. doi: 10.1111/j.16512227.2010.02048.x Engström, M., Pihlsgård, J., Lundberg, P., & Söderfeldt, B. (2010). Functional magnetic resonance imaging of hippocampal activation during silent mantra meditation. The Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 16(12), 1253-1258. doi: 10.1089/acm.2009.0706 Grant, J. A., Duerden, E. G., Courtemanche, J., Cherkasova, M., Duncan, G. H., & Rainville, P. (2013). Cortical thickness, mental absorption and meditative practice: Possible implications for disorders of attention. Biological Psychology, 92, 275-281. Hamilton, D. A., & Sutherland, R. J. (1999). Blocking in human place learning: Evidence from virtual navigation. Psychobiology, 27, 453-461. Holzel, B. K., Carmody, J., Vangel, M., Congleton, C., Yerramsetti, S. M., Gard, T., & Lazar, S. W. (2011). Mindfulness practice leads to increases in regional brain gray matter density. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 191, 36-43. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.08.006 Hooker, K. E., & Foder, I. E. (2008). Teaching mindfulness to children. Gestalt Review, 12(1), 75-91. MINDFULNESS AND CHILDREN WITH ADHD 26 Johnston, C., Hommersen, P., & Seipp, C. (2008). Acceptability of behavioral and pharmacological treatments for Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity disorder: Relations to child and parent characteristics. Behavior Therapy, 39(1), 22-32. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2007.04.002 Josefsson, T., & Broberg, A. (2011). Meditators and non-meditators on sustained and executive attentional performance. Mental Health, Religion and Culture, 14(3), 291-309. doi:10.1080/13674670903578621 Kabat-Zinn, J. (1994). Wherever you go, there you are: Mindfulness meditation in everyday life / jon kabat-zinn New York : Hyperion, c1994; 1st ed. Kozhevnikov, M., Louchakova, O., Josipovic, Z., & Motes, M. A. (2009). The enhancement of visuospatial processing efficiency through buddhist deity meditation. Psychological Science, 20(5), 645-653. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02345.x Lykins, E. L. B., & Baer, R. A. (2009). Psychological functioning in a sample of long-term practitioners of mindfulness meditation. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy: An International Quarterly, 23(3), 226-241. doi: 10.1891/0889-8391.23.3.226 Miller, J. J., Fletcher, K., & Kabat-Zinn, J. (1995). Three-year follow-up and clinical implications of a mindfulness meditation-based stress reduction intervention in the treatment of anxiety disorders. General Hospital Psychiatry, 17, 192-200. Moore, A., Gruber, T., Derose, J., & Malinowski, P. (2012). Regular, brief mindfulness meditation practice improves electrophysiological markers of attentional control. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6(18) doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00018 MINDFULNESS AND CHILDREN WITH ADHD 27 Morris, R. G. M. (1981). Spatial localization does not require the presence of Local cues . Learning and Motivation, 12, 239-260. Narr, K. L., Woods, R. P., Lin, J., Kim, J., Phillips, O. R., Del'homme, M., . . . Levitt, J. G. (2009). Widespread cortical thinning is a robust anatomical marker for attentionDeficit/Hyperactivity disorder. American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 48(10), 1014-1022. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181b395c0 Oord, S., Bögels, S., & Peijnenburg, D. (2012). The effectiveness of mindfulness training for children with ADHD and mindful parenting for their parents. Journal of Child & Family Studies, 21(1), 139-147. doi: 10.1007/s10826-011-9457-0 Purser, H. R. M., Farran, E. K., Courbois, Y., Lemahieu, A., Mellier, D., Sockeel, P., & Blades, M. (2012). Short-term memory, executive control, and children's route learning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113(2), 273-285. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.06.005 Rutledge, K. J., Bos, W., McClure, S. M., & Schweitzer, J. B. (2012). Training cognition in ADHD: Current findings, borrowed concepts, and future directions. Neurotherapeutics, 9, 542-558. doi: 10.1007/s13311-012-0134-9 Singh, N., Singh, A., Lancioni, G., Singh, J., Winton, A., & Adkins, A. (2010). Mindfulness training for parents and their children with ADHD increases the Children’s compliance. Journal of Child & Family Studies, 19(2), 157-166. doi:10.1007/s10826-009-9272-z Smalley, S. L., Loo, S. K., Hale, T. S., Shrestha, A., & McGough, J. (2009). Mindfulness and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 65(10), 10871098. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20618 MINDFULNESS AND CHILDREN WITH ADHD Weijer-Bergsma, E., Formsma, A., Bruin, E., & Bogels, S. (2012). The effectiveness of mindfulness training on behavioral problems and attentional functioning in adolescents with ADHD. Journal of Child & Family Studies, 21(5), 775. doi: 10.1007/s10826-0119531-7 Witkiewitz, K., Bowen, S., Douglas, H., & Hsu, S. H. (2013). Mindfulness-based relapse prevention for substance craving. Addictive Behaviors, doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2012.04.001 Zeidan, F., Johnson, S. K., Diamond, B. J., David, Z., & Goolkasian, P. (2010). Mindfulness meditation improves cognition: Evidence of brief mental training. Consciousness and Cognition, 19, 597-605. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2010.03.014 28