CULTURE AND WELL-BEING: EAST AND WEST Heine, Lehman, Markus, & Kitayama (1999) claim that there is no universal need for positive self-regard, based on their analysis of Japanese and American selves. “Self-esteem is made possible by the development of cultural worldviews, which provide a stable and meaningful conception of the universe, social roles, and specific prescriptions for behaviors that are deemed valuable, and the promise of safety and immortality for those who satisfy those prescriptions. Self-esteem is therefore a cultural contrivance consisting of two compotents: a meaningful conception of a universe combined with the perception that one is meeting the standards for virtue within that culturally contrived reality (Solomon et al., 1991). There may be many ways of fulfilling your role as a good member of your society. Selfways are defined as communities’ ideas about being a person and the social practices, situations, and institutions of everyday life that represent and foster these ideas. Selfways include core cultural ideas and values, including understandings of what a person is and a sense of how to be a “good” or “moral” or “appropriate” person (Markus, Mullaly, & Kitayama, 1997). The construction of a Japanese self usually involves the validation of a person’s social relationships by seeking to identify and confirm shared expectations and norms, whereas the goal in American life is to turn the dependent child into an independent person with distinct preferences and unique attributes. Self-criticism is a defining characteristic of Japanese. Children are encouraged to search for their own weaknesses and inadequacies, to refine themselves to become “rashii”, prototypical of the ideal qualities of the group. Self-discipline and self-effacement are important tools in this process. Caudill & Weinstein (1969) found a strong positive correlation between the frequency of American mothers chatting with their babies and their infants’ “happy vocals” and no correlation with the babies’ “unhappy vocals”. The American mother appears to elicit and reinforce her baby’s “happy vocalizations”. By contrast, they found Japanese mothers chatting to soothe their babies, rather than reinforce happy vocals. Japanese ideals are more about being emotionally balanced than unambiguously happy. Distance between self-ideal was larger in Japan than Canada BUT these differences were less related to depression. Heine (1996) found that Canadians rated happiness 1st or 2nd in terms of the desirability of 20 traits in life, whereas Japanese ranked it 18th. 80% of Americans claim to be happy, whereas this percentage is far lower in Japan and Taiwan. So how important is happiness? How central is self esteem? It is difficult to replicate studies of dissonance reduction in Asia. In recent formulations of dissonance theory, dissonance in created by behaviour inconsistent with the self view. For example, Americans showed post-decision dissonance after choosing from two close alternatives, whereas Japanese did not (Heine & Lehman, 1997). Rationalization of the decision was especially pronounced when negative personality feedback was given to Americans (thereby jeopardizing selfaffirmation), but no effect was found for Japanese. Americans compensated for negative selfrelevant feedback by inflating their assessments in another domain (Baumeister & Jones, 1978). Canadians exhibited no differences in their evaluations of unrelated traits following negative feedback on their creativity, while Japanese decreased their selfevaluations (Heine, Kitayama, & Lehman, 1999). Whereas Canadians discounted negative feedback more, Japanese tended to discount positive feedback more. Japanese reported failure situations as more relevant to their self-esteem than success situations, whereas Americans reported the opposite pattern. Japanese persist longer at a task when given false failure feedback whereas Canadians persist longer when given false positive feedback. Japanese are far more sensitive to information that they are doing worse than others than information they are doing better than others. Self-enhancement is a typical pattern of social interaction in the USA, and self-criticism is typical for Japan. Self-critical tendencies are adaptive for Japanese because they are embedded in a culturally shared and sustained practice of self-improvement. These tendencies are premised on the individual’s acceptance of group standards. Self is not constant, it is embedded in a network of social relations. Self is seen through the eyes of others, face is maintained. An interpersonal sympathy script is highly developed among Japanese, so one may experience warm feelings from others when the self is found to be wanting. Sympathy is correlated with both receiving and giving help in Japan, but only with receiving help in the US. General good feelings are highly correlated with self-esteem in the US (disengagement), but better predicted by frequency of receiving help in Japan (interpersonal engagement). Good feelings in Japan are not owned by the self but shared in a relationship. Well-being is a collaborative project, and to be happy is to be attuned to others. Americans self-report a far greater proportion of positive compared to negative feelings, whereas for Japanese it is about equal. Diener & Diener (1995) suggest that “life satisfaction may be based more on positive feelings in individualistic nations, for example, feelings about the self. Conversely, in collectivist nations life satisfaction might be influenced by a more prevalent negative focus.” Suh, Diener, Oishi, & Triandis (1998) found the relation between life satisfaction and positive feelings to be correlated with individualism across nations, whereas collectivism is correlated with the relation between life satisfaction and adherence to norms. Kitayama & Markus (2000), summarizing data showing an antagonistic relationship between positive and negative emotions in the US (e.g., a single dimension of valence), compared to positive correlations in Japan, and the overwhelming experience of positive emotions in the US compared to Japan, state that “East Asian subjective well-being may be moderate by its very nature.” A PANCULTURAL VIEW Kitayama & Markus are fond of saying North Americans are like this and Japanese (or East Asians) are like that, but could it be a mixture of both? According to Kwan, Bond, & Singelis (1997), relationship harmony and self-esteem are two separable and independent variables that have an additive effect in predicting life satisfaction. They found that self-esteem was more important than relationship harmony in the US, but the two were equally good predictors of life satisfaction in Hong Kong. “Harmony is a concept that refers to the balance achieved in relationships. The major focus of this concept is on the relationship, rather than the satisfaction of the constituent individuals or the support derived from the individual from that relationship.” The effect of an independent self-construal (on life satisfaction) is mediated through selfesteem, whereas the effect of an interdependent self-construal is mediated through relationship harmony. They also were able to mediate most of the effects of the FFM personality indictors on life satisfaction (except for direct paths from neuroticism and extraversion—indicators of positive and negative affect). The two variables exerted the same mediational effects in both Hong Kong and the USA. How about in New Zealand? How would we characterize Maori, NZ Europeans, and Asians? insert graphs on the models insert data from NZL insert graph on SE and acculturation figures from K&M 2000 – enhancement + models