Comment on Rowe & Healy - Open Research Exeter (ORE)

advertisement
1
How and why are some species so smart? A comment on Rowe & Healy
2
Alex Thornton*
3
4
*
5
Penryn Campus, Treliever Road, Penryn TR10 9FE, UK
6
Address correspondence to A. Thornton. E-mail: alex.thornton@exeter.ac.uk
Centre for Ecology and Conservation, Department of Biosciences, University of Exeter,
7
8
Examining how individual cognitive variation relates to fitness holds the promise of
9
providing a richer understanding of how cognitive traits evolve (Thornton and Lukas 2012).
10
However, as Rowe and Healy (2014) point out (see also Thornton, Isden, and Madden 2014
11
in this issue), this endeavour is rife with pitfalls, and requires more psychological rigour than
12
has been employed in most studies to date. I agree wholeheartedly with them that arbitrary
13
“problem-solving” tasks should be abandoned in favour of psychologically-grounded tests
14
targeting specific cognitive mechanisms (Thornton, Isden, and Madden 2014) and hope that
15
future work will heed their advice in designing tests and accounting for non-cognitive
16
influences. Here I want to focus on two additional, interrelated issues: which species should
17
we focus on, and what cognitive mechanisms should we examine?
18
Choices of study species will largely be determined by practical considerations such as the
19
need for an adequate sample size. Abundant, tractable, yet seemingly cognitively
20
unsophisticated model systems may prove useful in developing methods and testing general
21
principles. Such animals have long been a cornerstone of experimental psychology and could
22
be tested in the field to illuminate general principles including trade-offs in cognitive
23
processing (e.g. speed vs. accuracy; Sih and Del Giudice 2012) or between cognition and
24
other traits (e.g. (Mery and Kawecki 2003) in the face of natural selective pressures. I would
25
argue that yet greater insights may be gained by examining the fitness consequences of
26
cognitive variation in animals whose cognitive abilities appear in some way remarkable.
27
Given the costs of investment in neural tissue, what are the fitness benefits that generate
28
selection for these abilities? Food-storing birds provide the paradigmatic example, as there is
29
strong evidence linking the challenges of retrieving stored food with elevated hippocampal
30
volume and long-term memory retention (see Rowe and Healy 2014). Spatial memory tests
31
may offer similar opportunities in other animals, such as female brood parasites, which must
32
remember the locations of potential hosts’ nests (Reboreda, Clayton, and Kacelnik 1996),
33
though the relevant facets of spatial memory are yet to be determined. Here, individual-based
34
studies can help pinpoint which, if any, aspects of spatial memory confer fitness advantages
35
and specify what those advantages may be.
36
37
Arguably, an even greater mystery is the striking performance of some species not only in a
38
single, ecologically predictable cognitive domain, but across domains. For instance, meta-
39
analyses show that some primate species consistently outperform others across different
40
laboratory tasks (Deaner, van Schaik, and Johnson 2006). Among birds, corvids may also
41
exhibit similar cross-context cognitive prowess, with evidence for remarkable abilities in
42
memory, planning, rule learning, inferential reasoning and aspects of physical and social
43
cognition (Seed, Emery, and Clayton 2009). One fruitful avenue of research may be to test
44
the performance of wild individuals across batteries of tasks targeting different cognitive
45
processes (c.f. Isden et al. 2013). While it is difficult to generate straightforward predictions,
46
such work may help elucidate the mechanistic structure of cognition (e.g. the extent to which
47
general mechanisms explain individual performance across different tasks) and understand
48
whether selection acts on cognitive mechanisms as discrete traits or part of an inter-related
49
complex.
50
As Rowe and Healy (2014) remind us, it would be naïve to suppose that individual cognitive
51
performance will necessarily correlate positively with fitness. However, exploring the
52
relationships between suites of cognitive measures and multiple fitness components may help
53
build a picture of the costs and benefits of cognitive traits. We have argued that there is much
54
to be learned from psychometrics, a branch of psychology specifically concerned with
55
quantifying individual cognitive differences (Thornton, Isden, and Madden 2014). In
56
particular, psychometric tests aim to reduce the influence of non-cognitive factors and
57
generate continuous measures of cognitive performance. These tests typically specify clear
58
criteria as to what constitutes a “correct” outcome, but apparent mistakes will also be
59
informative, for example in revealing individual differences in sampling strategies (Seed et
60
al. 2012). By uniting psychometric approaches with conceptual and methodological tools
61
from behavioural ecology, we can start to hone in on trade-offs and understand whether and
62
how selection acts on cognitive variation.
63
64
65
Funding
66
A.T. was supported by a BBSRC David Phillips Fellowship (BB/H021817/1).
67
68
Acknowledgements
69
I am grateful to J. Madden for helpful comments on previous drafts of the paper.
70
71
References
72
Deaner RO, van Schaik CP, Johnson V. 2006. Do some taxa have better domain-general
73
cognition than others ? A meta- analysis of nonhuman primate studies. Evol. Psychol. 4:149–
74
196.
75
Isden J, Panayi C, Dingle C, Madden J. 2013. Performance in cognitive and problem-solving
76
tasks in male spotted bowerbirds does not correlate with mating success. Anim Behav. 86:
77
829-838.
78
Mery F, Kawecki TJ. 2003. A fitness cost of learning ability in Drosophila melanogaster.
79
Proc. R Soc. B 270:2465–2469.
80
Reboreda JC, Clayton NS, Kacelnik A. 1996. Species and sex differences in hippocampus
81
size in parasitic and non-parasitic cowbirds. Neuroreport 7:505–508.
82
Rowe, C, Healy SD 2014 Measuring variation in cognition. Behav. Ecol. In Press.
83
Seed AM, Emery NJ, Clayton NS. 2009. Intelligence in corvids and apes: a case of
84
convergent evolution? Ethology 115:401–420.
85
Seed AM, Seddon E, Greene B, Call J. 2012. Chimpanzee “folk physics”: bringing failures
86
into focus. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 367: 2743-2752.
87
Thornton A, Isden J, Madden JR. 2014 Towards wild psychometrics: linking individual
88
cognitive differences to fitness. Behav. Ecol. In press
89
Thornton A, Lukas D. 2012. Individual variation in cognitive performance: developmental
90
and evolutionary perspectives. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 367:2773–2783.
91
Download