Social Identity & Performance, page 1 Identity matters: Identity as a proximal determinant of employee performance Steven L. Blader New York University Stern School of Business, Management Department 40 West 4th Street Room 7-18 New York, NY 10012 Tel: (212) 998-0431 sblader@stern.nyu.edu Tom R. Tyler New York University Department of Psychology 6 Washington Place, 5th Floor New York, NY 10003 Tel: (212) 998-7816 tom.tyler@nyu.edu Social Identity & Performance, page 2 Identity matters: Identity as a proximal determinant of employee performance Abstract Two field studies test and extend the group engagement model (Tyler & Blader, 2000, 2003) by applying the model to the issue of employee performance. Consistent with the group engagement model’s predictions, results of these studies indicate that the social identities employees form around their work groups and their organizations are a critical determinant of their work performance, insofar as social identity not only directly shapes their performance but also explains the impact of other factors that have been shown to shape performance. In particular, the findings indicate that social identity mediates the effect of procedural justice judgments and economic outcomes on supervisor ratings of performance. Overall, these studies provide strong indication that social identity is critical in determining people’s behavior within their work organizations and provide strong support for the application of the group engagement model in organizational settings. Keywords: Social identity, Procedural justice, Economic outcomes, Employee performance Social Identity & Performance, page 3 The integration of social identity theory (Tajfel & Turner, 1979) into organizational research represents one of the most successful recent examples of how basic psychological research can enhance our understanding of people in organizational contexts. Since Ashforth & Mael’s (1989) seminal piece, which advocated the utilization of social identity theory to clarify and extend our understanding of how people use their organizations as a basis for self-definition, interest in the application of social identity theory to understand organizational phenomena has flourished. Indeed, the insights of social identity theory, which at their core emphasize the importance people place on group membership for understanding both themselves and the world around them, have been embraced by many organizational researchers and the theory has emerged as an important framework for analyzing the psychology of the individual in the organization (e.g., Haslam, 2004). Some theorists have even gone so far as to argue that social identity provides an essential approach for understanding organizational life and, moreover, makes organizational life possible (Haslam, Postmes, & Ellemers, 2003). One recent theoretical model that shares this perspective and places social identity centerstage is the group engagement model (Tyler & Blader, 2000; 2003), which argues that social identity is a critical ingredient for understanding the psychological basis of people’s engagement with their groups, organizations, and societies. Attesting to the widespread interest in the application of social identity to organizational phenomena, the group engagement model has already been integrated into much organizational research and theorizing (e.g., Boezeman & Ellemers, in press-a, in press-b; Fuller, et al., 2006; Haslam & Reicher, 2006; Olkkonen & Lipponen, 2006; Sparrowe, Soetjipto, & Kraimer, 2006 ). However, interest in the model has developed despite the fact that empirical tests and theoretical extensions of the model are still Social Identity & Performance, page 4 quite limited, with the only comprehensive test of the model to date (Tyler & Blader, 2000) relying exclusively on self-report measures of engagement. In response, the two studies presented below were conducted to further test the group engagement model. In particular, they examine a key hypothesis derived from the model, namely that social identity plays a critical role in shaping employee job performance. Support for this hypothesis can help further validate the arguments of the group engagement model and can also provide broader support for the argument that identity is important to the study of behavior in work settings. These studies also use the group engagement model as a basis for examining the interrelationship between social identity and two other factors that have been found to have strong linkages with employee performance: procedural justice judgments and economic outcome evaluations. In addition, they extend earlier work on identity in organizational contexts by adopting a conceptualization of social identity that is broader than that used in previous research but which more directly reflects the original thinking about this construct in the social psychological literature, and by focusing on two different identity targets regularly implicated by organizational membership, i.e., identity connected to one’s work group and identity connected to one’s work organization. The Group Engagement Model: Social Identity and Performance A core principle of the group engagement model is that a person’s orientation towards the collectives to which he or she belongs (broadly referred to as their groups) is primarily shaped by the role the group plays in his or her self definition. That is, the model argues that a person’s orientation towards their group—as manifested in his or her attitudes and behaviors within the group—is determined by the role the group plays in how he or she thinks and feels about himself or herself. This impact of group membership on someone’s self-definition is known as the social Social Identity & Performance, page 5 identity (Tajfel & Turner, 1979) they form around the group, and the perspective that social identity is a primary driver of people’s orientation towards their groups is based on the underlying argument that a critical element of what people seek from their groups relates to their social or collective selves, i.e., that aspect of the self-concept that is focused on the self as it relates to groups (Brewer & Gardner, 1996). In particular, people seek a sense of connectedness and membership from their groups—especially when that connectedness is of positive valence. People are thus likely to socially identify with groups that positively fulfill their desire for connections with a collective, since such groups enable people to satisfy needs linked to their collective selves and, thus, impact people’s feelings of well-being and self-worth (Hogg & Abrams, 1988). Based on the role social identity plays in serving such fundamental psychological needs, the group engagement model predicts that more positive attitudes and greater behavioral effort in support of group goals will emerge among those with social identities focused on a particular group. These positive attitudinal and behavioral implications are likely precipitated by social identity because such attitudes and behaviors facilitate the viability of the group and can enhance the individual’s acceptance in the group (Haslam & Ellemers, 2005; Klein, Spears & Reicher, 2007; Kramer, Hanna, Su, & Wei, 2001; Tajfel, 1978; Tyler & Blader, 2000, 2003; van Knippenberg & Ellemers, 2003). People who draw more of their social identity from a group will be more strongly concerned with ensuring the group’s success, as well as ensuring their inclusion in the group. One consequence of such social identity-motivated engagement in organizational contexts will likely be increased levels of employee performance. Past research supporting the fundamental basis of this argument has shown important effects of social identity on people’s efforts on behalf of their groups. For instance, social Social Identity & Performance, page 6 identity has been linked to ingroup-favoritism (Brewer, 1979; Hinkle & Brown, 1990; Tajfel, Billig, Bundy, & Flament, 1971), coordination of efforts with other group members to advance group goals (Kelly & Kelly, 1994; LaLonde & Silverman, 1994), monetary donations to one’s group (Mael & Ashforth, 1992), and loyalty to the group (Abrams, Ando & Hinkle, 1998; Ellemers, Spears, & Doosje, 1997) (for reviews, see Haslam, 2004; Turner & Reynolds, 2001). These empirical demonstrations are complemented by theories that argue that social identity shapes behaviors that advance group goals (e.g., Ashforth & Mael, 1989; Brewer & Gardner, 1996; Dukerich et al., 2002; Dutton et al., 1994; Ellemers et al., 2004; Kramer et al., 2001). While this work suggests that social identity may influence employee performance, not all actions geared toward promoting the in-group are created equal. Indeed, employee performance is a highly complex behavior with a wide variety of determinants (e.g., Kanfer, 1990; Roe, 1999), greater costs to the self (e.g., effort), and potentially greater benefits than many other types of behaviors that have been linked to social identity (Ouwerkerk, Ellemers, & de Gilder, 1999). As a result, the threshold for detecting an impact of social identity on employee performance is likely more stringent than it might be for other efforts to advance group goals, which makes direct examination of the social identity-employee performance link critical. Toward this end, past experimental research has found that social identity increases the level of effort that participants put forth on behalf of their group (Worchel et al., 1998) and reduces social loafing (Karau & Williams, 1993; Williams, Karau, & Bourgeois, 1993). Similar effects have been found in naturalistic work settings, where social identity impacted self-reported internal motivation (van Knippenberg & van Schie, 2000), self-reported work performance (Tyler & Blader, 2000), and supervisor ratings of work effort among a select group of employees participating in a community outreach program (Bartel, 2001). These findings are consistent with Social Identity & Performance, page 7 the argument that social identity shapes employee performance. However, the reliance on experimental paradigms that may not replicate to real-world settings, the use of self-report measures as proxies of performance, and the focus on restricted samples of employees that may not be representative of the general population makes it difficult to draw definitive conclusions about the social identity-employee performance link from these findings. Given the prominence of both identity and employee performance in the organizational literature, there is a need for stronger examination of the relationship between these constructs. Doing so may not only shed empirical light on the relationship between two important constructs, but may also contribute to our theoretical understanding of the bases of behavior in organizations. Moreover, examining the performance implications of social identity provides a much-needed test of social identity’s motivational consequences, filling a gap in identity research within organizational contexts created by more rapid theory development than theory testing (Corley et al., 2006; Dukerich, Golden, & Shortell, 2002; Foreman & Whetten, 2002). In response, our current studies test the following hypothesis: Hypothesis 1: Employee social identity will shape employee performance. The Group Engagement Model: Procedural justice and economic outcome evaluations A critical element of the group engagement model is that it not only emphasizes the importance of social identity for analyzing people’s engagement in their groups, but it also tries to facilitate a broader understanding by emphasizing the importance of locating identity in a framework that includes other antecedents of engagement. To do this, the group engagement model draws on a key distinction in the psychology literature between two different bases of people’s reactions to their encounters with groups and organizations (Brockner & Wiesenfeld, 1996). Each of these bases has been shown to be a critical precursor of people’s attitudes and Social Identity & Performance, page 8 behaviors in groups. One basis for people’s reactions is the processes and treatment they experience while in a group -- or those elements of the group that determine how the group functions and, as a result, shape the nature of one’s experience as a group member. Research has shown that a primary dimension along which people evaluate the processes and treatment they encounter is with regards to their fairness (Tyler, 2000, Tyler and Blader, 2000). Thus, this basis of people’s reactions to their groups is embodied in their judgments of procedural fairness. The second basis of people’s reactions to encounters with their groups are the economic outcomes they attain as a result of their membership in the group. This basis is embodied in various judgments they make about the economic outcomes they receive from the group. Together these two bases characterize a great deal about the experience of group membership for the individual, insofar as they capture elements of what life is like within the group and what the implications of group membership are for the individual. The importance of considering each of these bases of people’s reactions is underscored by the extensive research that has been conducted on them within organizational contexts, much of it focused on the impact of processes and outcomes on employee performance. The influence of procedural justice on employee performance is supported by a rich set of findings from both psychology and organizational research that collectively indicate that employee evaluations of the fairness of decision-making processes and quality of treatment (referred to by some researchers as interactional justice, cf. Blader & Tyler, 2003a, 2003b) shape the level and nature of people’s behavior in groups (Cohen-Charash & Spector, 2001; Colquitt, Conlon, Wesson, Porter, & Ng, 2001). People become more engaged in groups when they regard those groups as operating in procedurally fair ways (Tyler, 1999; Tyler & Blader, 2000, 2003), and in the workplace employee performance represents one form that such increased engagement may take Social Identity & Performance, page 9 (Blader & Tyler, 2005; Cohen-Charash & Spector, 2001; Colquitt, Conlon, Wesson, Porter, & Ng, 2001; Tyler & Blader, 2000). The group engagement model argues that the impact of procedural justice on performance (and, more generally, on attitudes and behavior) can be attributed to the social identity implications that follow from assessments of procedural justice. The basis of this argument is rooted in past research on the relational models of procedural justice (Lind & Tyler, 1988; Tyler & Lind, 1992), which emphasize that the fairness of decision making processes and treatment experienced in a group provide a cue people use to evaluate the quality of their relationship with their group. In particular, people use procedural justice to evaluate whether or not a particular group is one in which they can safely invest their social identities (Tyler & Blader, 2002, 2003) since it communicates to them whether or not they are respected members of the group and whether or not they can be proud of the group (Tyler, Degoey, & Smith, 1996). As such, procedural justice judgments are an antecedent of whether people come to socially identify with a particular group. In sum, the group engagement model takes the findings that a) procedural justice shapes group members’ attitudes and behaviors, and b) procedural justice shapes social identity and uses them to make the argument that the impact of procedural justice on people’s attitudes and behaviors in groups is mediated by social identity. This argument is again based on the key premise that social identity is a fundamental concern people have in their relations with their groups, and as such, it explains the impact of other factors that also seem to affect how people relate to their groups. The current studies directly test this proposition by examining the following hypothesis: Social Identity & Performance, page 10 Hypothesis 2: The effect of procedural justice on employee performance will be mediated by employee social identity. Extensive theorizing and research indicate that judgments of the economic outcomes one receives from their group or organization will also have a major impact on employee performance. These judgments include evaluations of pay, benefits, and opportunities for promotion, as well as other short- and long-term financial benefits that may be provided by the organization. It would be difficult to overstate the extent to which laymen, organizational practitioners, and academic researchers believe that such judgments impact employee performance. It is generally accepted that people respond to favorable economic outcome evaluations with high levels of work performance, especially when those outcomes are seen as linked to their behavior (Lawler, 1990, 2000; Rynes, Gerhart & Park, 2005). The influence of economic outcomes on employee behavior is central to many applications of exchange theory (which emphasizes that the receipt of valued outcomes begets an obligation to reciprocate, often in the form of high levels of work performance) as well as expectancy theory (which emphasizes that people are motivated to engage in behaviors that are likely to facilitate the receipt of valued outcomes and will continue to engage in those behaviors if doing so perpetuates the receipt of those outcomes). This in turn has led to a major focus on ideal ways of designing and leveraging economic outcomes (such as compensation) in ways that most strongly encourage high performance (e.g., Milkovich & Newman, 1999). Underlying the link between economic outcomes and performance is the view that people are fundamentally resource-seeking actors. This viewpoint dominates many theories within the social sciences and, thus, the organizational sciences as well. However, this perspective has not received uniform support, and considerable evidence demonstrates digressions from resource- Social Identity & Performance, page 11 maximizing behavior in organizations as well as in broader contexts (e.g., see Gintis, Bowles, Boyd, & Fehr, 2005; Haslam & Ellemers, 2005; Kohn, 1999; Miller & Ratner, 1998; Pearce, 1987). These findings suggest that, to the extent people do react to the economic outcomes they receive from their work organizations, those reactions may be rooted in part in alternate concerns that are unrelated to resource-maximization goals. One such alternate concern is suggested by findings that economic outcomes carry important symbolic—as opposed to material—value for evaluations of self-worth, success, relationships, and status, among other things (Furnham & Argyle, 1998; Goldberg & Lewis, 1978; Mitchell & Mickel, 1999; Opsahl & Dunnette, 1966; Porter, Bigley, & Steers, 1996). When economic outcomes from the group are regarded positively, they communicate a positive message to people about their contributions and, more generally, about what the group and its authorities think of them. Thus, people’s reactions to their economic outcomes may be explained by the symbolic message these economic outcomes convey, rather than by their material value alone. The group engagement model (Tyler & Blader, 2000, 2003) builds on the insight that economic outcomes carry symbolic value and specifically argues that economic outcomes may carry symbolic social identity-relevant value. In particular, the group engagement model states that economic outcomes can convey a message to group members that resembles the message conveyed by procedural justice – insofar as both cues let group members know whether or not they are respected by the group, whether or not the group is one that they can be proud of, and more generally, whether or not they can safely invest their social identities with the group. In other words, people may often use economic outcomes to evaluate their standing within their group, the status of the group, and whether or not their social identity needs will be met by the Social Identity & Performance, page 12 group. These identity relevant inferences may actually explain the impact of economic outcome judgments on employee performance. That is, economic outcomes may impact employee performance because of their social identity implications. To examine this, the current studies test the following hypothesis: Hypothesis 3: The effect of economic outcome evaluations on employee performance will be mediated by employee social identity. Hypotheses two and three test the group engagement model’s argument that social identity is the proximal determinant of employee performance among the three antecedents of performance considered here. They propose that social identity is the psychological intermediary through which employee perceptions of these two critical aspects of the workplace shape performance. We focus on these aspects because they are primary characteristics of people’s encounters with their work organizations and because they have been shown in previous research to be potent predictors of performance. By predicting that social identity mediates their effects on performance, these studies not only test the argument that social identity plays an especially central role as a performance antecedent but more generally provide an empirical test of the perspective that social identity and the collective self are among people’s most fundamental concerns (Brewer & Roccas, 2001; Brewer & Weber, 1994; Ellemers, Spears, & Doosje, 2002; Haslam & Ellemers, 2005; Hogg, 2001). While our focus is on the group engagement model and its predictions, it is important to note that the underlying rationale is consistent with the general framework articulated in the uncertainty management model of justice (Van den bos and Lind, 2002). That model also suggests that people use information to understand their social environments and, like earlier theorists (Brockner & Wiesenfeld, 1996; Thibaut & Walker, 1975), that model emphasizes that Social Identity & Performance, page 13 people are especially likely to focus on information about processes and outcomes. The group engagement model, however, goes further and argues that people use information about processes and outcomes to shape their social identities. As such, the model advances the point of view that these pieces of social information are valued because they are identity-relevant. Conceptualizing Social Identity Importantly, the group engagement model conceptualizes social identity much as it was understood in original social psychological theorizing: as a multi-dimensional construct with various elements that work in coordination to produce the implications of group membership on the self concept (e.g., Bergami & Bagozzi, 2000; Haslam, 2004; Tajfel, 1978, 1982). This conceptualization characterizes social identity as impacting people’s thoughts and feelings about themselves. One key component of this conceptualization is the cognitive component, i.e., an employee’s sense of “oneness” with the organization (known as their identification with the organization). This component reflects what is typically meant by identity in organizational research and is consistent with self-categorization theory’s emphasis (Turner, 1985; Turner, Oakes, Haslam & McGarty, 1994) on the cognitive processes that underlie a sense of group membership. The cognitive aspect of identity captures the extent to which group membership is relevant and self-defining and thus determines the implications group membership for how people think of themselves. A second component of social identity is the evaluative component. This component embodies the value connotations people place on group membership, and thus, determines the implications group membership has for how people feel about themselves. Although not typically examined in organizational research on identity, the importance of this component was noted in some of the very earliest theorizing on social identity (Tajfel, 1982) and is critical to Social Identity & Performance, page 14 social identity theorists’ arguments that a social identity has greater fit and impact to the extent that it fosters positive self-definition and self-esteem (Ellemers, 1993; Ellemers et al., 1997; Haslam et al., 2000; Hogg & Abrams, 1990; Mael & Ashforth, 1992; Tajfel & Turner, 1979). Even outside of social identity theory itself, it has been noted that evaluative aspects of group membership are critical for understanding the nature of identity and for predicting reactions to identity (Ashmore, Deaux, & McLaughlin-Volpe, 2004; Dutton et al., 1994; March and Simon, 1958). As such, the group engagement model includes an evaluative component and in so doing, tests a broader conceptualization of identity than that examined in most organizational research. The cognitive and evaluative components of social identity are assessed in the studies that follow by measuring respondents’ 1) cognitive representation of the connection between the group and the self (i.e., their identification) and 2) evaluative judgments of pride and respect (Tyler, Degoey, & Smith, 1996). Pride refers to evaluations of the standing of one’s group overall; it is an intergroup evaluative judgment. Respect, on the other hand, refers to evaluations of one’s standing and acceptance within his or her group; it is an intragroup evaluative judgment. Prior research confirms that both pride and respect are critical judgments for understanding the impact of group membership on the self (e.g., Baumeister & Leary, 1995; Ellemers, Spears, & Doosje, 1997; Smith & Tyler, 1997; Tyler & Blader, 2000, 2001, 2003; Tyler, Degoey, & Smith, 1996). Such intergroup and intragroup evaluative judgments will likely be especially important in contexts such as work organizations, where group boundaries are permeable, membership is voluntary, and numerous structures are in place to accentuate both intergroup and intragroup comparisons. In addition, the studies presented below advance previous research on the group engagement model by recognizing that different social identity targets may be implicated by Social Identity & Performance, page 15 organizational membership (Johnson et al., 2006; Pratt & Foreman, 2000). These social identity targets include social identity connected to one’s work group and social identity connected to one’s work organization. Both targets represent groups that accompany affiliation to most work organizations, and both represent social identities that will prompt an impact of organizational membership on the self-concept (Ashforth & Johnson, 2001; Johnson et al., 2006). Perhaps most important for the current studies, social identity related to each of these targets is likely to have expression through employee performance, since performance may benefit the work group, the organization, or both. In study one we examine social identity vis-à-vis the work group, an extension of previous research on the group engagement model that has focused on social identity vis-à-vis the organization. In study two, we return to a focus on social identity vis-à-vis the organization. By examining different social identity targets across the two studies, we hope to more fully explore the robust nature of the social identity-performance relationship. Current Research The two field studies presented below test three hypotheses that emerge from the group engagement model and which examine the implications of social identity for employee performance. They test the proposition that social identity shapes employee performance, and test the additional propositions that social identity accounts for the effect both procedural justice and economic outcome judgments on performance. These tests use a multi-faceted conceptualization of social identity that examines social identity as it relates to both the work group (study 1) and organizational (study 2) levels. Overall, the studies test whether social identity is the proximal determinant of performance in a model that includes two workplace conditions that have themselves demonstrated strong linkages to performance. The theoretical Social Identity & Performance, page 16 model that follows from these proposals—and which represents the key ideas of the group engagement model—is presented in Figure 1. The first study presented below was conducted in a major financial services organization and represents an effort to test these hypotheses in a context where financial incentives and economic concerns are especially prominent. As a result, this context provides a particularly stringent test of the impact of identity. The second study, which sought to provide a broader test, was conducted on sample of workers heterogeneous with regard to location, industry, job, income, etc. STUDY ONE Method Participants and procedure. Five hundred and forty (540) U.S. employees of a single division of an international financial services organization responded to a survey that included the measures described below (a total of 1,350 employees, representing the entire U.S. division, received the survey, and thus there was a 40% response rate). Employees received the questionnaires via interoffice mail and were asked to complete them while at work and to return them directly to the investigators using enclosed business-reply envelopes. They were assured confidentiality by both the investigators and directly by the organization’s management. Supervisor surveys were also distributed to the immediate supervisors of all 1,350 employees in the division. Four hundred and fifteen (415) of these surveys were returned (a 31% response rate). Of these, the overlap with the employee respondent pool led to a final employeesupervisor matched sample of 112 employees. Among this subsample of matched employee-supervisor respondents (n = 112), 52% were female and 46% had pursued some post-bachelor’s education. The mean tenure with the Social Identity & Performance, page 17 firm was 13.7 years and the mean age was 44.47 years. The sample was only moderately heterogeneous with respect to race, with 82% Caucasian, 13% Latino, 4% African American, and 2% Asian. Importantly, these demographics closely resembled the sample of employee respondents (n =540), in which 53% were female, 48% had pursued some post-bachelor education, the mean tenure was 13.67 years, the mean age was 42.9 years, and race was distributed such that there were 74% Caucasian, 11% Latino, 5% African American, and 5% Asian. The demographics among the full population of employees in the division were quite similar, among whom 59% were female, the mean tenure was 13 years, the mean age was 43 years, and in which race was distributed such that there were 63% Caucasian, 17% Latino, 11% African American, and 8% Asian. Reliable education data were unavailable for employees in the division overall. Measures. Social identity vis-à-vis one’s work group, economic outcome judgments, and procedural justice were all assessed via measures included in the employee survey. Assessments of employee performance were included on the supervisor survey. Responses were on a scale of 1 to 6 (strongly disagree to strongly agree, unless otherwise noted). The items composing the scales were adapted from previous studies (Tyler & Blader, 2000, 2001, 2002; except where otherwise noted) and all scales demonstrated strong reliability (coefficient alphas are listed in Table 1). Social identity vis-à-vis the work group was measured using scales that assessed identification, pride, and respect, which were all loaded onto a common latent factor in the structural equation models presented below. Identification with the work group was measured using a five-item scale. Sample items included “When I talk about my work group, I usually say “we” rather than they” and “I have a sense that I personally belong in my work group.” Pride in Social Identity & Performance, page 18 the work group was measured using a 4-item scale, with sample items such as “I feel proud to be a part of my work group” and “My work group is highly respected within the company.” Respect in the work group was measured using a six-item scale that included items such as “My manager values me as a member of my work group” and “My work group manager respects my workrelated ideas.” Economic outcome judgments were measured using scales that assessed evaluations of current pay, expectations of future economic outcomes, outcome fairness, and incentives, which were all loaded onto a common latent factor in the structural equation models presented below. Instructions asked respondents to consider these statements with respect to the job they currently hold within their work group. Evaluations of pay were assessed by four items that asked respondents to indicate their agreement with such items as “I am satisfied with my pay” and “Overall, I receive excellent pay at (company name).” Expectations of future economic outcomes were assessed with three items such as “I have good opportunities for promotion.” Outcome fairness was measured by asking respondents three items, such as “How fairly are resources allocated among employees where you work?” (1—not at all fairly; 6—very fairly). Finally, incentives were measured by assessing agreement with six items, such as “If you do your job well, how much does that improve your pay and benefits?” (1—very little, 6—a lot). Procedural justice was measured by a five-item scale with items such as “How often do you feel that decisions are made in fair ways at your job?” (1—rarely, 6—very often). Again respondents were instructed to consider these statements with respect to the job they currently hold within their work group. Employee performance was assessed via three measures on the supervisor survey, based on scales by Williams & Anderson (1991) and Smith, Organ, & Near (1983). There were Social Identity & Performance, page 19 measures of inrole behavior (three items, e.g., “How often does this employee fulfill their job responsibilities?”), extrarole behavior (three items, e.g., “How often does this employee volunteer to do helpful things not required by their job description?”), and an overall assessment of performance (four items, e.g., “Overall, how would you rate this employee’s performance?”). These three performance measures were loaded onto a common latent factor in the structural equation models presented below. Results Table 1 presents the means, standard deviations, and a correlation matrix for all measures. Three control variables were included in all analyses: age, tenure, and gender. One potential concern regarding the sample is that it represents a fairly small segment (n = 112) of the total pool of respondents (n = 540). Although response rates to the employee and supervisor surveys (as noted in the method section above) were independent from one another and within traditional ranges for field research in organizational settings—and thus there are no apparent systematic methodological issues that indicate the sample is biased—the overlap between them nevertheless yielded a somewhat small sample of respondents. Thus, as an additional check, we examined the equivalence between the subsample of matched employeeemployer respondents and the remaining pool of employee respondents on the mean levels of all variables collected in the employee survey. The results indicate that there were no major differences between the subsample of employees included in the analyses below and the broader employee respondent group, i.e., means on all variables (except for age) were not significantly different from one another between the two groups (all Fs < 3.30, ps > .05). While the mean age of respondents in the subsample was somewhat higher than that in the group of remaining respondents (44.5 vs 42.9 years old), the effect size indicates that this difference is relatively Social Identity & Performance, page 20 minor (² = .009) and it is not clear that this age difference would impact interpretation of the results below. Thus, there are both methodological and empirical reasons to believe that the subsample of matched employee-supervisor respondents represents a random, non-biased sample from the larger pool of employee respondents. Structural equation modeling (via AMOS) was used to test the conceptual model presented in Figure 1 and, more specifically, to test the predicted impact of social identity on employee performance as well as the predicted mediating role of social identity in explaining the effect of procedural justice and economic outcomes on employee performance. The results of these structural equation models are presented in Table 2. Prior to these analyses, however, the fit of the proposed measurement model was examined via a confirmatory factor analysis that included the three social identity measures (identification, pride, and respect) loaded onto a common latent factor (“social identity”), the four outcome judgments (pay evaluations, expectations of future economic outcomes, outcome fairness, and incentives) loaded onto a common latent factor (“economic outcomes”), and procedural justice. This analysis confirmed that the proposed measurement model fit the data well (2 = 44.5, df = 18, CFI = .98, IFI = .98) and moreover, was better fitting than a model that loaded all the variables on a single factor (2 = 37.9, df = 2, p > .01). Hypothesis one predicted that social identity (in this study, regarding one’s work group) would significantly influence employee performance. A structural equation model testing this prediction, presented in Table 2 (model 1), indicates strong support for this hypothesis, since social identity was significantly related to performance ( = .40, p < .001) and explained a considerable portion of the variance in supervisor’s ratings of performance (R2 = 17%). This result confirms the prediction that social identity is an important antecedent of performance. Social Identity & Performance, page 21 Hypothesis two predicted that social identity would mediate the effects of procedural justice on performance. Before testing this hypothesis, an analysis was conducted to determine if procedural justice significantly influenced employee performance in this dataset. That analysis (Table 2, model 2) confirms that procedural justice does shape performance ( = .25, p < .01; R2 = 7%). Consistent with previous research (Colquitt et al., 2001; Tyler & Blader, 2000, 2001, 2003), employee perceptions of process fairness in their work place was associated with stronger engagement in the organization -- in this case, in the form of higher levels of work performance. Most critical to the current analysis, however, is whether or not work group social identity explains the effects of procedural justice on performance. Analyses examining this question, presented in Table 2 (model 3), provide strong support for this hypothesis. That analysis shows that all of the conditions for mediation (Baron & Kenny, 1996) are met in this data: procedural justice is related to social identity ( = .67, p < .001), social identity is related to performance ( = .55, p < .01), and most importantly, the effect of procedural justice on performance becomes non-significant ( = -.11, ns) when work group social identity is included in the analysis. Hypothesis three predicted that social identity (again, with one’s work group) would mediate the effects of economic outcomes on employee performance. To test this, we first verified that economic outcome judgments had an impact on performance in this data, as would be predicted by the extensive literature that advocates an influence of economic concerns on employee behavior. That analysis (Table 2, model 4) confirms that economic outcome evaluations shape performance ( = .34, p < .001; R2=12%), such that more positive evaluations of issues such as pay, future economic outcomes, outcome fairness, and incentives foster higher levels of employee performance. Next, we tested hypothesis three’s prediction that social identity mediates this impact of economic outcome evaluations on performance (Table 2, model 5). This Social Identity & Performance, page 22 analysis provided strong confirmation of hypothesis three. It shows that the conditions for mediation (Baron & Kenny, 1996) are all met in this data: economic outcome evaluations are related to social identity ( = .67, p < .001), social identity is related to performance ( = .45, p < .05), and the effect of economic outcome evaluations on performance becomes non-significant ( = .05, ns) when social identity is included in the analysis. Model 6 (Table 2) simultaneously examines the effects of social identity, procedural justice, and economic outcome evaluations. As might be expected based on Models 3 and 5, the results indicate an effect of social identity on performance ( = .52, p < .001), but no effect of economic outcome evaluations ( = .19, ns) or procedural justice ( = -.23, ns) when accounting for social identity. Interestingly, this analysis revealed effects for both procedural justice ( = .39, p < .001) and economic outcome evaluations ( = .36, p < .01) on social identity (R2 = 50%), indicating that procedural justice and economic outcomes each have unique implications for work group social identity. Discussion Study one indicates strong support for the three key hypotheses examined in the current research and, more generally, for application of the group engagement model to the issue of employee performance. First, strong performance implications were found for the nature of employees’ social identities vis-à-vis their work group. This finding corroborates the value of social identity in organizational contexts and highlights the importance of examining social identity with the work group as the target. Moreover, this study shows the value of adopting a multi-faceted conceptualization of social identity and advocates moving beyond a sole focus on the cognitive elements of social identity, as has been the norm in organizational research to date. Social Identity & Performance, page 23 In addition to demonstrating that work group social identity matters, these findings go a step further to show how social identity relates to two key elements of the workplace that have been shown to impact performance. Namely, the results indicate that work group social identity accounted for the impact of procedural justice judgments and evaluations of the economic outcomes employees experience at work. As such, it appears that evaluations of these workplace conditions shape performance in this data because they impact the social identities employees form around their work group. Many researchers have examined and discussed the reasons people react to their process fairness and economic outcome judgments. These results contribute to that conversation and highlight that the social identity implications of process fairness and economic outcomes should also be a focus of those efforts. Overall, these results confirm the group engagement model’s argument that social identity may be a psychological integrator that provides the route through which at least some features of the workplace impact employee performance. While compelling, the results of study one require further examination. The sample was drawn from a single organization, which means that the effects for work group social identity in these data are possibly an artifact of the specific organization or its context. Therefore, it is important to test the hypotheses using a sample that is more heterogeneous with regard to context, occupation, industry, etc. Moreover, the sample size in study one was somewhat small -particularly given the complexity of some of the models examined (e.g., model 6). As such, it is desirable to replicate these results on a larger sample. Finally, while study one examines the social identities that employees form around their work groups, it is also critical to examine whether similar results emerge for the social identities employees form around their Social Identity & Performance, page 24 organizations overall. Study two directly addresses these issues by examining a large (n=831) and heterogeneous panel of employees from throughout the United States. STUDY TWO Method Participants and procedure. Participants in study two were a random sample drawn from a national panel of respondents designed to be representative of the entire US population. Members of this national panel, which is maintained by a for-profit private organization (Knowledge Networks, Inc.), are initially contacted for their participation via random digit dialing. An incentive of free internet access via WebTV is provided to all those that join the panel, and all surveys administered to the panel are conducted over WebTV (for more details on the panel, please contact the authors). All potential respondents for the current study were screened to ensure that they worked at least 20 hours a week, had a primary supervisor, and had worked at their current employer for at least 3 months. Respondents meeting these criteria completed the survey in two parts, one week apart. In total, there were 4,430 employee respondents in the sample. Each of these respondents was asked if they would be willing to have their supervisor complete and return a confidential questionnaire. Financial incentives were offered to employees and supervisors for each completed supervisor questionnaire received. 2,020 (46%) employee respondents allowed their supervisor to be contacted, and of these, 831 supervisors (41%) actually completed and returned the questionnaire they were sent. The data presented below are based on this subsample of 831 matched employee-employer respondents. Employees in the subsample of matched employee-supervisor respondents came from a variety of organizations. Twenty-six percent (26%) worked for small businesses, nineteen Social Identity & Performance, page 25 percent (19%) for large companies in one location, thirty-six percent (36%) for large multi-city American companies, and seventeen percent (17%) for multinational companies. The sample was evenly split by gender (48% female). The sample’s average tenure at their current job was 4.49 years, 75% had at least some college experience (only 9% had earned a post-bachelor degree), and the mean age was 44 years old. Measures. Constructs and measures generally paralleled those in study one, although a few scales were adjusted in an effort to examine broader or different conceptualizations of certain constructs. In particular, in study two we focused on social identity linked to the organization (rather than the work group), and thus the social identity measures were adjusted accordingly. Also, instructions for the questionnaire asked respondents to consider the questions in the survey with regard to their work organization (as compared to study 1 instructions which focused them on their current job in their work group). In addition, the procedural and distributive justice scales were expanded (to ten- and eight-items, respectively) to not only capture assessments of the fairness of the culture of the overall organization (Naumann & Bennett, 2000) as in study one, but to also index the perceived fairness directly experienced by individuals (including items such as “Decisions that affect me are usually made in fair ways at my company”). The performance scales on the supervisor survey were also expanded to inquire about a broader range of inrole (e.g., “How often does this employee complete their work in a timely, effective manner?”) and extrarole (e.g., “How often does this employee share their knowledge with others even when they will not receive credit?”) behaviors. Incentives were not measured in study two due to space constraints on the employee survey as a result of expansion of these other scales. All scales demonstrated strong reliability (coefficient alphas are listed in Table 3). Social Identity & Performance, page 26 Results Means, standard deviations, and the interscale correlation matrix for all measures are presented in Table 3. Table 3 also includes three control variables that were included in all analyses: age, tenure with the organization, and gender. As in study one, mean ratings on each of the variables collected from employees were compared between the subsample of matched employee-employer respondents and the remaining pool of employee respondents (for whom no supervisor ratings were available). The results of this comparison again suggest that differences between the two groups were inconsequential. Due to the large sample sizes involved in this study, statistical significance does not provide a meaningful indicator of the equivalence of the groups because even negligible differences will likely have statistical significance. However, investigation of the effect sizes of these differences indicates that there were no sizable differences in the means of any of the variables between the two respondent groups (all ² < .02), which indicates that the subsample of matched employeesupervisor respondents is likely equivalent to the larger pool of employees in our study. We again used structural equation modeling (via AMOS) to test the model presented in Figure 1 and, in particular, to test the interrelationship of social identity, procedural justice, economic outcome evaluations, and employee performance. The results of these structural equation models are presented in Table 4. Prior to testing these models, however, we used confirmatory factor analysis to examine the fit of the proposed measurement model, which includes the three social identity measures (identification, pride, and respect) loaded onto a common latent factor (“social identity”), the three economic outcome judgments (evaluations of pay, expectations of future economic outcomes, and outcome fairness) loaded onto a common latent factor (“economic outcomes”), and procedural justice. This analysis confirmed that the Social Identity & Performance, page 27 proposed measurement model fit the data well (2 = 190.9, df = 12, CFI = .99, IFI = .99) and that it fit significantly better than a simpler one-factor model (2 = 132.5, df =2, p < .001). The results presented in Table 4 provide strong confirmation of our hypotheses. The results presented in model 1 confirm hypothesis 1, since they indicate a strong impact of social identity on supervisor ratings of employee performance ( = .26, p < .001). Hypothesis 2, which predicted that social identity would mediate the effects of procedural justice on performance, was also confirmed by our results. As shown in model 2, the effect of procedural justice on performance was replicated in this dataset ( = .21, p < .001), which again corroborates the robust finding in the organizational behavior literature that process fairness shapes employee engagement (Colquitt et al., 2001; Tyler & Blader, 2000). More importantly, model 3 indicates that the effect of procedural justice on performance is fully explained by social identity, consistent with the prediction set forth by hypothesis two. When social identity is included in the analysis, procedural justice is significantly related to social identity ( = .79, p < .001), but no longer has a significant influence on performance ( = .01, ns) while social identity does influence performance ( = .26, p < .001). Furthermore, the results of models 4 and 5 confirm hypothesis 3. First, we found that economic outcome evaluations shape performance ( = .15, p < .001) -- more positive evaluations of economic outcomes are associated with higher levels of performance. Second, adding social identity to this analysis (model 5) reduces the effect of economic outcome evaluations to nonsignificance ( = -.14, ns), but does reveal a strong effect of social identity ( = .34, p < .01). Similarly, there was a significant effect of economic outcome evaluations on social identity ( = .83, p < .001). Social Identity & Performance, page 28 Once again, we conducted a comprehensive test of the model presented in Figure 1 (model 6, Table 4). The results indicate that social identity impacts performance ( = .32, p < .01), but that when accounting for this effect procedural justice only has a marginal impact ( = .18, p < .10) and economic outcomes actually have a significant but negative direct effect on performance ( = -.27, p < .01). This analysis again confirms that both procedural justice ( = .31, p < .001) and economic outcomes ( = .55, p < .001) have independent effects on social identity (R2 = 71%). Discussion Study two directly confirms the results of study one in verifying several key propositions of the group engagement model and in demonstrating the important role of social identity in shaping employee performance. The results of study two indicate that the social identity that an employee forms around their work organization is an important determinant of their work performance. Beyond mere demonstration that social identity matters, study two also confirms that social identity can explain the impact of other factors (i.e., procedural justice and economic outcome evaluations) that are themselves often investigated for their influence on performance. The consistency in findings with study one is striking and considerably increases confidence in the robust nature of these results. This is reinforced by the fact that study two was conducted on a large and extremely heterogeneous sample of employees. Respondents were drawn from throughout the United States, worked in a wide variety of occupations, industries, and organizations, and came from a diverse range of levels within their organization’s structure. Moreover, study two focused on a different social identity target than study one (organizational social identity rather than work group social identity), yet the overall pattern of results replicated that found in study one. This suggests that since employee performance impacts both sub- and Social Identity & Performance, page 29 super-ordinate group goals and success, it may be a form of behavioral engagement that is linked to the various levels of social identity targets that may are relevant in organizational life. One difference with the results of study one was that a significant but negative effect of economic outcome evaluations on employee performance was detected in the overall analysis that simultaneously examined all the variables under consideration. This result is intriguing because it suggests a complex interrelationship between economic outcomes, procedural justice, and social identity. In particular, it suggests that extrinsic issues such as economic outcomes may actually have detrimental effects on performance once their symbolic or intrinsic value has been accounted for. These results may be related to the classic finding that extrinsic rewards can have detrimental effects on behaviors that are otherwise intrinsically motivated (Deci, Koestner, & Ryan, 1999). Additional work is needed to investigate this possibility, particularly since this result did not emerge in study one. Nevertheless, the results of the current study do indicate that the observed positive effect of economic outcomes on performance is explained by social identity. GENERAL DISCUSSION These studies make an important contribution to research on the group engagement model and to our theoretical understanding of the motivational consequences of social identity in organizational contexts. They do so by finding that social identity plays an important role in shaping employee performance, and by demonstrating that social identity explains the performance implications of two key facets of people’s experiences with their work organizations (i.e., the procedural justice they experience and the economic outcomes they receive). These findings are consistent with the group engagement model’s emphasis on social identity as the basis of people’s engagement in their groups, and on the importance of locating Social Identity & Performance, page 30 social identity within the landscape of other factors that shape engagement. These studies further contribute to research on social identity in organizations by 1) highlighting the value of a multifaceted conceptualization of identity that is rooted in principles of social identity theory, and 2) by applying this conceptualization to two focal social identity targets (i.e., the work group and the organization). The research presented here advances previous work with regard to each of these issues and makes clear that cognitive and evaluative components of social identity—with regard to both the work group and the organization—can act in coordination to shape performance. Importantly, these studies show that social identity explains a comparatively favorable proportion of variance in performance relative to other predictors that have been examined in other research (e.g., perceived organizational support (Rhoades & Eisenberger, 2002); psychological well being (Wright & Cropanzano, 2000); job satisfaction (Judge et al., 2001); commitment (e.g., Becker et al., 1996); and incentives (Jenkins, Mitra, Gupta & Shaw, 1998)). Further, they show these potent effects of social identity among employees working in a wide variety of organizations, occupations, hierarchical levels, regions, etc. Further, by going beyond mere demonstration that social identity matters, the results reveal that social identity is a proximal determinant of performance insofar as it explains the impact of two organizational conditions that have received a great deal of attention for their relation to performance: 1) perceptions of the procedural justice experienced at work and 2) evaluations of the economic outcomes received from work. Determining that social identity provides the mechanism by which these organizational conditions influence organizational behavior provides critical insight into understanding how, when, and why efforts to shape the context of employees’ work experiences will impact their behavior. It also contributes to the Social Identity & Performance, page 31 growing acknowledgement within psychology that the collective self—the aspect of the self most closely linked to social identity—plays a fundamental role in shaping behavior (Hogg, 2001). The results for economic outcomes in these studies may seem somewhat surprising. Indeed, it is traditionally believed that pay, benefits, expectations of promotion (and thus hopes for greater pay), and the like impact behavior because of their economic or instrumental value. However, studies investigating the utility of these economic outcome-based approaches raise questions about their actual impact (e.g., Jenkins et al., 1998; Kerr, 1995; Kohn, 1993; Pearce, 1987) and psychological research suggests that such extrinsic factors can have a detrimental effect on people’s intrinsic drives and motivations (Deci, Koestner, & Ryan, 1999). The current findings may provide some potential insight into the equivocal effect of outcome concerns on performance. In particular, they suggest that a primary way in which outcomes may impact performance is through their impact on social identity, which explains earlier observations that identity can trigger many of the same effects as economic incentives (Dukerich, Golden, & Shortell, 2002). One important implication of this finding is that economic outcomes are most likely to impact performance when they lead employees to regard their relationship with the organization positively and when they lead employees to integrate the organization into their self concept. When the economic outcomes an employee experiences fail to prompt these positive evaluations—such as when they are dispensed in an environment marked by threats or guided by obligations—it becomes less likely that economic outcomes will have a positive impact on performance. On a more general level, these findings suggest that the basis of employees’ relationships with their organizations is primarily linked to the role the organization plays in determining how employees think and feel about themselves. Of course, there are likely boundary conditions to Social Identity & Performance, page 32 our findings. For employees at lower socioeconomic levels, for whom resources are literally critical, the impact of economic outcomes on behavior may be more direct. Similarly, social identity may not be likely to shape behavior among employees who have tenuous links to the organization such as temporary and contract workers, nor perhaps among workers that do not share demographic group memberships or values with the organization. Still, the results of the current and related (Tyler & Blader, 2000) research clearly suggest the potential value, under many circumstances, of a social identity-based understanding of employees’ attachments to their organizations and of social identity-based ways of stimulating higher levels of employee performance. Limitations and future research A note of caution is necessary regarding the causal directions advocated in our conceptual model. While our research avoids the problem of common method variance by linking employee judgments to supervisor ratings of behavior, it cannot provide definitive evidence regarding the causal relations we have proposed. Consistent with the orientation adopted in the psychology and management literatures, we advocate the perspective that behavior flows from identity. However, it is possible that supervisor judgments that an employee is high-performing actually lead employees to identify with the group through a social exchange process. While plausible, it is not clear what the theoretical basis would be for expecting that employees reciprocate their supervisors’ regard for them as high performers by integrating the group into the self (i.e., it is not clear why employees would use such a psychological currency in their exchange). Ultimately, alternate research methods that can help further tease apart causal relationships are needed to provide more definitive support for the causal relations we propose. Social Identity & Performance, page 33 A variety of issues raised by this research require further investigation. First, it is important to investigate other organizational conditions that shape performance -- aside from procedural justice and economic outcomes -- in order to better understand the relationship between social identity and these other factors. Indeed, a myriad of factors have been linked to performance, and the current contribution is only one small step toward truly developing a map of performance antecedents and their relationships with one another. Second, additional work is needed to understand factors that may determine the dynamics between social identity and performance. Relations between the various foci of social identity (work group, organization, etc.), the salience of a given social identity in a particular circumstance, and group norms regarding performance may all impact the particular behaviors resulting from a strong sense of social identity (Haslam & Ellemers, 2005; van Knippenberg & Ellemers, 2003). Indeed, it is likely that there may even be distinctions in the types of performance-related behaviors that result from social identity linked to work groups vs. organizations. Having achieved the goal of the current studies of mapping social identity onto performance in general, future work should focus on how specific instantiations of social identity-based performance may vary depending on the focal social identity group. Third, a deeper understanding of the psychological mechanism by which social identity impacts performance is also needed. Does social identity shape performance because it a) prompts an intrinsic concern with the group’s welfare, b) leads to conformity to group norms, c) facilitates internalization of group goals, or d) leads people to seek acceptance via high performance that benefits the group? Uncovering the mechanisms involved is not only theoretically important, but also has practical significance because it facilitates an understanding of the circumstances under which social identity will matter most, the people for whom social Social Identity & Performance, page 34 identity will matter most, and the behaviors that social identity is most likely to provoke. Finally, research is needed to understand the linkages between social identity and other variables that assess attachment to organizations, such as affective commitment (Meyer & Allen, 1997). Like many other identity researchers (Ashforth & Mael, 1989; Ashmore, Deaux & McLaughlinVolpe, 2004; Cameron, 2004; Ellemers, Kortekaas, & Ouwerkerk, 1999; Haslam, 2004; Ouwerkerk et al., 1999; Pratt, 2000), we feel that social identity is fundamentally different from affective commitment because it relies on the self-concept to understand people’s attachment to organizations. Unlike affective commitment, social identity focuses on the basis of attachment to the group and is much more than a simple barometer of attachment. Thus, we view social identity as operating on a more fundamental level than affective commitment (Haslam, 2004) and we regard affective commitment as a potential outcome of social identity (Tyler & Blader, 2000). Yet clearly additional work is needed to understand the dynamics between social identity and affective commitment. Conclusion This need for future work notwithstanding, we hope that the current studies aid in highlighting the value of the group engagement model for organizational research. Like the group engagement model itself, these studies make strong theoretical contributions to research on procedural justice, on why money and related economic issues matter, and on how to foster higher levels of employee performance. The current research impacts these diverse research areas by highlighting one key integrating concept: social identity. As such, perhaps the single most important contribution of these studies is their argument that researchers and practitioners alike have much to gain by adopting a focus on social identity in efforts to understand people in group and organizational contexts. By understanding the fundamental concerns that drive Social Identity & Performance, page 35 people’s behavior in groups, it is more likely that groups will be designed and will operate in ways that both motivate and satisfy their members. Identity matters, page 36 FIGURE 1 Conceptual Model Procedural Justice Identification Pride Respect Economic Outcomes Evaluation of Pay Future Economic Outcomes Outcome Fairness Incentives Inrole Social Identity Employee Performance Extrarole Overall Identity matters, page 37 TABLE 1 Study 1: Means, Standard Deviations, and Correlations Among Scales Scale 1 0.96 2 1. Procedural justice M (SD) 3.34 (1.10) 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 2. Identification 4.45 (0.85) 0.43 0.85 3. Pride 4.05 (1.09) 0.36 0.57 0.88 4. Respect 4.48 (1.19) 0.58 0.50 0.48 0.97 5. Evaluations of pay 2.87 (1.13) 0.50 0.13# 0.26 0.43 0.95 6. Future economic outcomes 7. Outcome fairness 3.16 (1.01) 0.52 0.33 0.41 0.49 0.48 0.70 2.84 (1.19) 0.69 0.23 0.34 0.43 0.68 0.57 0.85 8. Incentives 3.15 (1.15) 0.68 0.26 0.39 0.57 0.61 0.57 0.74 0.88 9. Inrole performance 5.40 (0.63) 0.22 0.17# 0.12# 0.45 0.27 0.22 0.25 0.26 0.93 10. Extrarole perf. 4.72 (0.98) 0.26 0.30 0.24 0.39 0.21 0.19 0.21 0.27 0.54 0.94 11. Overall perf. 5.06 (0.78) 0.17# 0.18# 0.15# 0.31 0.20 0.19 0.20 0.24 0.72 0.61 0.94 12. Gender -- -0.03# 0.08# -0.01# 0.07# -0.15# 0.00 -0.05# -0.06# 0.03# 0.10# -0.07 -- 13. Tenure 13.70 (8.56) 0.00# -0.01# -0.09# -0.08# -0.05# -0.07# -0.19 -0.06# -0.16# -0.05# -0.07# -0.03# -- 14. Age 44.47 (8.75) 0.20 0.13# 0.09# 0.10# 0.14# 0.07# 0.17# 0.23 0.00# 0.01# 0.01# -0.05# 0.44 Note: n = 112. Higher numbers indicate greater endorsement of the construct (e.g., more positive fairness, stronger identification, etc.). Numbers on the diagonal represent coefficient alphas for the scale. All correlations are significant at p < .05 unless marked #. 13 Identity matters, page 38 TABLE 2 Study 1: Results of Structural Equation Models Model 1 b (SE) Model 2 b (SE) Model 3 b (SE) Model 4 b (SE) Model 5 b (SE) Model 6 b (SE) DV: Supervisor ratings of performance Social identity 0.32 (.10) 0.40*** ------- ------- ------- 0.50 (.17) 0.55** ------- ------- ------- 0.44 (.18) 0.45* 0.49 (.18) 0.52*** Procedural justice ------- ------- ------- 0.11 (.05) 0.25** -0.05 (.07) -0.11ns ------- ------- ------- ------- ------- ------- -0.11 (.08) -0.23ns Economic outcome evaluations ------- ------- ------- ------- ------- ------- ------- ------- ------- 0.21 (.07) 0.34*** .03 (.09) 0.05ns 0.12 (.12) 0.19ns R2 17% 7% 24% 12% 24% 26% 2 df Fit indices: CFI IFI 61.5 38.3 70.25 57.8 112.1 126.9 26 14 33 34 62 72 0.98 0.98 0.99 0.99 0.99 0.98 0.99 0.99 0.98 0.99 0.99 0.97 Economic outcome evaluations ------- ------- ------- ------- ------- ------- ------- ------- ------- ------- ------- ------- 0.43 (.09) 0.67*** 0.24 (.11) 0.36* Procedural justice ------- ------- ------- ------- ------- ------- 0.35 (.06) 0.67*** ------- ------- ------- ------- ------- ------- 0.19 (.07) 0.39** ------- ------- 45% ------- 45% DV: Social identity R2 50% Note: n = 112. All analyses include controls for the effects of age, gender, and tenure on performance (though in no case did any of these controls have significant effects). Identity matters, page 39 TABLE 3 Study 2: Means, Standard Deviations, and Correlations Among Scales Scale 1 0.95 2 1. Procedural justice M (SD) 3.24 (0.99) 2. Identification 3.10 (0.91) 0.48 0.84 3. Pride 3.57 (0.95) 0.56 0.58 0.90 4. Respect 5.22 (1.46) 0.51 0.46 0.44 0.94 5. Evaluations of pay 3.17 (0.95) 0.66 0.38 0.53 0.39 0.89 6. Future economic outcomes 7. Outcome fairness 3.07 (0.98) 0.58 0.43 0.51 0.44 0.66 0.72 2.89 (0.95) 0.83 0.44 0.50 0.43 0.71 0.60 0.90 8. Inrole performance 6.11 (0.80) 0.13 0.07# 0.03# 0.19 0.08 0.04# 0.08 0.94 9. Extra role perf. 5.49 (1.16) 0.16 0.15 0.07 0.23 0.07 0.06# 0.12 0.57 0.91 10. Overall perf. 4.26 (0.67) 0.15 0.13 0.07 0.21 0.08 0.11 0.12 0.58 0.60 0.91 11. Gender -- -0.02# 0.11 0.06# 0.05 -0.07 -0.06# -0.09 0.11 0.09 0.07 -- 12. Tenure 4.49 (0.70) -0.03# 0.08 0.08 0.01# 0.06# 0.09 -0.03# 0.08 0.05# 0.03# -0.03# -- 43.87 (10.36) 0.11 0.13 0.12 0.10 0.09 -0.01# 0.05# 0.03# 0.00# -0.05# 0.09 0.19 13. Age 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 Note: n = 831. Higher numbers indicate greater endorsement of the construct (e.g., more positive fairness, stronger identification, etc.). Numbers on the diagonal represent coefficient alphas for the scale. All correlations are significant at p < .05 unless marked #. 12 Identity matters, page 40 TABLE 4 Study 2: Results of Structural Equation Models Model 1 b (SE) Model 2 b (SE) Model 3 b (SE) Model 4 b (SE) Model 5 b (SE) Model 6 b (SE) DV: Supervisor ratings of performance Social identity 0.57 (.09) .26*** ------- ------- ------- 0.40 (.15) 0.26** ------- ------- ------- 0.51 (.19) .34** 0.49 (.19) 0.32** Procedural justice ------- ------- ------- 0.19 (.03) 0.21*** 0.01 (.08) 0.01ns ------- ------- ------- ------- ------- ------- 0.17 (.08) 0.19^ ns -0.32 (.15) -0.27* Economic outcome evaluations ------- ------- ------- *** ------- ------- ------- ------- ------- ------- 0.16 (.04) 0.15 9% 7% 9% 5% 8% 11% 126.1 78.4 173.7 118.2 274.1 403.7 34 20 42 34 62 71 0.99 0.99 0.99 0.99 0.99 0.99 0.99 0.99 0.99 0.99 0.99 0.99 Economic outcome evaluations ------- ------- ------- ------- ------- ------- ------- ------- ------- ------- ------- ------- 0.62 (.04) 0.83*** 0.43 (.07) 0.55*** Procedural justice ------- ------- ------- ------- ------- ------- 0.46 (.03) 0.79*** ------- ------- ------- ------- ------- ------- 0.18 (.05) 0.31*** ------- ------- ------- 69% R2 2 df Fit indices: CFI IFI -0.15 (.13) -.14 DV: Social identity R2 63% 71% Note: n = 831. All analyses include controls for the effects of age, gender, tenure, and company size on performance. In all models, there was a significant, positive effect for tenure. All models also demonstrated an effect for gender such that females were rated as having higher performance. There were no significant effects for age. Identity matters, page 41 References Abrams, D., Ando, K., & Hinkle, S. (1998). Psychological attachment to the group: Crosscultural differences in organizational identification and subjective norms as predictors of workers’ turnover intentions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 1027-1039. Ashforth, B. E., & Johnson, S. A. (2001). Which hat to wear? The relative salience of multiple identities in organizational contexts. In M. Hogg & D. Terry (Eds.), Social identity processes in organizational contexts (pp. 31-48). Philadelphia: Psychology Press. Ashforth, B. E., & Mael, F. (1989). Social identity theory and the organization. Academy of Management Review, 14, 20-39. Ashmore, R. D., Deaux, K., & McLaughlin-Volpe, T. (2004). An organizing framework for collective identity: Articulation and significance of multidimensionality. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 80-114. Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 51, 1173-1182. Bartel, C. A. (2001). Social comparisons in boundary-spanning work: Effects of community outreach on members’ organizational identity and identification. Administrative Science Quarterly, 46, 379-413. Baumeister, R. F. & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motive. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497-529. Becker, T. E., Billings, R. S., Eveleth, D. M., & Gilbert, N. L. (1996). Foci and bases of employee commitment: Implications for job performance. Academy of Management Journal, 39, 464-482. Identity matters, page 42 Bergami, M., & Bagozzi, R. P. (2000). Self-categorization, affective commitment and group selfesteem as aspects of social identity in the organization. British Journal of Social Psychology, 39, 555-577. Blader, S. L. & Tyler, T. R. (2003). A four component model of procedural justice: Defining the meaning of a "fair" process. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 747-758. Blader, S. L., & Tyler, T. R. (2005). How can theories of organizational justice explain the effects of fairness? In J. Greenberg & J. A. Colquitt (Eds.), Handbook of organizational justice (pp. 329-354). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Publishers. Boezeman, E.J. & Ellemers, N. (in press-a). Volunteering for charity: Pride, respect, and the commitment of volunteer workers. Journal of Applied Psychology. Boezeman, E.J. & Ellemers, N. (in press-b). Pride and respect in volunteers' organizational commitment. European Journal of Social Psychology. Brewer, M. B. (1979). In-group bias in the minimal intergroup situation: A cognitivemotivational analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 307-324. Brewer, M. B., & Gardner, W. (1996). Who is this “we”? Levels of collective identity and selfrepresentations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 83-93. Brewer, M. B., & Roccas, S. (2001). Individual values, social identity, and optimal distinctiveness. In C. Sedikides & M. Brewer (Eds.), Individual self, relational self, collective self (pp. 219-237). New York: Psychology Press. Brewer, M. B., & Weber, J. G. (1994). Self-evaluation effects of interpersonal versus intergroup social comparison. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 268-275. Identity matters, page 43 Brockner, J. & Wiesenfeld, B. M. (1996). An integrative framework for explaining reactions to decisions: Interactive effects of outcomes and procedures. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 189-208. Cameron, J. E. (2004). A three-factor model of social identity. Self & Identity, 3, 239-262. Cohen-Charash, Y., & Spector P. E. (2001). The role of justice in organizations: A metaanalysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 86, 278-321. Colquitt, J. A., Conlon, D. E., Wesson, M. J., Porter, C., & Ng, K. Y. (2001). Justice at the millennium: A meta-analytic review of 25 years of organizational justice research. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 425-445. Corley, K., Harquail, C. V., Pratt, M. G., Glynn, M. A., Fiol, C. M., Hatch. M. J. (2006). Guiding organizational identity through aged adolescence. Journal of Management Inquiry, 15, 8599. Deci, E., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. (1999). A meta-analytic review of experiments examining the effects of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 627-668. Dukerich, J. M., Golden, B. R., & Shortell, S. M. (2002). Beauty is in the eye of the beholder: The impact of organizational identification, identity, and image on the cooperative behaviors of physicians. Administrative Science Quarterly, 47, 507-533. Dutton, J. E., & Dukerich, J. M. (1991). Keeping an eye on the mirror: Image and identity in organizational adaptation. Academy of Management Journal, 34, 517-554. Dutton, J. E., Dukerich, J. M., & Harquail, C. V. (1994). Organizational images and member identification. Administrative Science Quarterly, 39, 239-263. Identity matters, page 44 Ellemers, N. (1993). The influence of socio-structural variables on identity management strategies. In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Eds.), European Review of Social Psychology (Vol. 4). London: Wiley. Ellemers, N., de Gilder, D., & Haslam, A. (2004). Motivating individuals and groups at work: A social identity perspective on leadership and group performance. Academy of Management Review, 29, 459-478. Ellemers, N., Kortekaas, P., & Ouwerkerk, J. W. (1999). Self-categorization, commitment to the group, and group self-esteem as related but distinct aspects of social identity. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 371-389. Ellemers, N., Spears, R., & Doosje, B. (1997). Sticking together of falling apart: In-group identification as a psychological determinant of group commitment versus individual mobility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 617-626. Ellemers, N., Spears, R., & Doosje, B. (2002). Self and social identity. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 161-186. Foreman, P., & Whetten, D. (2002). Members’ identification with multiple-identity organizations. Organization Science, 13, 618-635. Fuller, J. B., Hester, K., Barnett, T., Frey, L., Relyea, C., & Beu, D. (2006). Perceived external prestige and internal respect: New insights into the organizational identification process. Human Relations. 59, 815-846. Furnham, A., & Argyle, M. (1998). The psychology of money. Florence, KY: Taylor & Frances/Routledge. Gintis, H., Bowles, S., Boyd, R., & Fehr, E. (2005). Moral sentiments and material interests: The foundations of cooperation in economic life. Cambridge: MIT Press. Identity matters, page 45 Haslam, S. A. (2004). Psychology in organizations: The social identity approach. London: Sage. Haslam, S. A., & Ellemers, N. (2005). Social identity in industrial and organizational psychology: Concepts, controversies and contributions. In G. P. Hodgkinson (Ed.), International Review of Industrial and Organizational Psychology (vol. 20). Chichester: Wiley. Haslam, S. A., Postmes, T., & Ellemers, N. (2003). More than a Metaphor: Organizational Identity Makes Organizational Life Possible. British Journal of Management, 14, 357-369. Haslam, S. A., Powell, C., & Turner, J. C. (2000). Social identity, self-categorizations, and work motivation: Rethinking the contribution of the group to positive and sustainable organizational outcomes. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 49, 319-339. Haslam, S. A., & Reicher, S. (2006). Stressing the group: Social identity and the unfolding dynamics of responses to stress. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 1037-1052. Hinkle, S. W., & Brown, R. J. (1990). Intergroup comparisons and social identity: Some links and lacunae. In D. Abrams & M. A. Hogg (Eds.), Social identity theory. New York: Springer-Verlag. Hogg, M. A. (2001). Social identity and the sovereignty of the group: A psychology of belonging. In C. Sedikides & M. Brewer (Eds.), Individual self, relational self, collective self (pp.125-146). New York: Psychology Press. Hogg, M. A., & Abrams, D. (1990). Social motivation, self-esteem and social identity. In D. Abrams & M. A. Hogg (Eds.), Social identity theory: Constructive & critical advances. New York: Springer-Verlag. Jenkins, G. D., Mitra, A., Gupta, N., & Shaw, J. D. (1998). Are financial incentives related to performance? Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 777-787. Identity matters, page 46 Johnson, M., Morgeson, F., Ilgen, D., Meyer, C., Lloyd, J. (2006). Multiple professional identities: Examining differences in identification across work-related targets. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 498-506. Judge, T. A., Thoresen, C. J., Bono, J. E., & Patton, G. K. (2001). The job satisfaction-job performance relationship: A qualitative and quantitative review. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 376-407. Kanfer, R. (1990). Motivation Theory and Industrial and Organizational Psychology. In M. D. Dunnette & L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of Industrial and Organizational Psychology (pp. 75-170). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Karau, S. J., & Williams, K, D. (1993). Social loafing: a meta-analytic review and theoretical integration. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 681-706. Kelly, C., & Kelly, J. (1994). Who gets involved in collective action? Human Relations, 47, 6388. Klein, O., Spears, R., & Reicher, S. (2007). Social Identity Performance: Extending the Strategic Side of SIDE. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11, 1-18 Kohn, A. (1993). Why incentive plans cannot work. Harvard Business Review, 71, 54-63. Kohn, A. (1999). Punished by rewards. New York: Houghton Mifflin Company. Kramer, R. M., Hanna, B. A., Su, S., & Wei, J. (2001). Collective identity, collective trust and social capital: Linking group identification and group cooperation. In M. E. Turner (Ed.), Groups at work: Theory and research (pp.173-196). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. LaLonde, R. N., & Silverman, R. A. (1994). Behavioral preferences in response to social injustice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 78-85. Identity matters, page 47 Lawler III, E. E. (1990). Strategic pay. San Fransisco: Jossey-Bass. Lawler III, E. E. (2000). Rewarding excellence: Pay strategies for the new economy. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Lind, E. A. & Tyler, T. R. (1988). The social psychology of procedural justice. New York: Plenum Press. Mael, F., & Ashforth, B. E. (1992). Alumni and their alma mater: A partial test of the reformulated model of organizational identification. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 13, 103-123. March, J. G., & Simon, H. A. (1958). Organizations. New York: John Wiley & Sons. Meyer, J. P., & Allen, N. J. (1997). Commitment in the workplace. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Milkovich, G. T., & Newman, J. M. (1999). Compensation (6th ed.). Boston: Irwin/McGrawHill. Miller, D. T., & Ratner, R. K. (1998). The disparity between the actual and assumed power of self-interest. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 53-62. Mitchell, T. R., & Mickel, A. E. (1999). The meaning of money: An individual-difference perspective. Academy of Management Review, 24, 568-579. Naumann, S., & Bennett, N. (2000). A case for procedural justice climate: Development and test of a multilevel model. Academy of Management Journal, 43, 881-889. Olkkonen, M.E., & Lipponen, J. (2006). Relationships between organizational justice, identification with organization and work unit, and group-related outcomes. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. 100, 202-215. Opsahl, R. L., & Dunnette, M. D. (1966). Role of financial compensation in industrial motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 66, 94-118. Identity matters, page 48 Ouwerkerk, J. W., Ellemers, N., & de Gilder, D. (1999). Group commitment and individual effort in experimental and organizational contexts. In N. Ellemers & R. Spears (Eds.), Social identity: Context, commitment, content (pp. 184-204). Oxford, England: Blackwell Science Ltd. Pearce, J. L. (1987). Why merit pay doesn’t work: Implications from organizational theory. In D. B. Balkin & L. R. Gomez-Meija (Eds.), New perspectives on compensation. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Porter, L. W., Bigley, G., & Steers, R. M. (1996). Motivation and work behavior. New York: McGraw-Hill. Pratt, M. G. (2000). The good, the bad, and the ambivalent: Managing identification among Amway distributors. Administrative Science Quarterly, 45, 456-493. Pratt, M. G., & Foreman, P. (2000). Classifying managerial responses to multiple organizational identities. Academy of Management Review, 25, 18-42. Rhoades, L., & Eisenberger, R. (2002). Perceived organizational support: A review of the literature. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 698-714. Roe, R. A. (1999). Work performance: A multiple regulation perspective. In C. L. Cooper & I. T. Robertson (Eds.), International review of industrial and organizational psychology. New York: John Wiley & Sons Ltd. Rynes, S.L., Gerhart, B., & Parks, L. (2005). Personnel Psychology: Performance Evaluation and Pay for Performance. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 571-600. Skitka, L. (2003). Of different minds: An accessible identity model of justice reasoning. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 7, 286-297. Identity matters, page 49 Smith, C., Organ, D., & Near, J. (1983). Organizational citizenship behavior: Its nature and antecedents. Journal of Applied Psychology, 68, 653-663. Smith, H. J., & Tyler, T. R. (1997). Justice and power: When will justice concerns encourage the advantages to support policies which redistribute economic resources and the disadvantaged to willingly obey the law? European Journal of Social Psychology, 26, 171-200. Sparrowe, R.T., Soetjipto, B.W., & Kraimer, M.L. (2006). Do leaders' influence tactics relate to members' helping behavior? It depends on the quality of the relationship. Academy of Management Journal, 49, 1194-1208. Tajfel, H. (1978). Social categorization, social identity and social comparison. In H. Tajfel (Ed.), Differentiation between Social Groups: Studies in the Social Psychology of Intergroup Relations (pp. 77-98). London: Academic Press. Tajfel, H. (1982). Social psychology of intergroup relations. Annual Review of Psychology, 33, 1-39. Tajfel, H., Billig, M. G., Bundy, R. P., & Flament, C. (1971). Social categorization and intergroup behavior. European Journal of Social Psychology, 1, 149-178. Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In W. Austin & S. Worchel (Eds.), The social psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 33-47). Monterey, CA: Brooks. Turner, J. C. (1985). Social categorization and the self-concept: A social cognitive theory of group behavior. In E. J. Lawler (Ed.), Advances in group processes. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Turner, J. C., Oakes, P. J., Haslam, S. A., & McGarty, C. A. (1994). Self and collective: Cognition and social context. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 545-63. Identity matters, page 50 Turner, J. C., & Reynolds, K. J. (2001). The social identity perspective in intergroup relations: theories, themes and controversies. In R. J. Brown & S. Gaertner (Eds.), Blackwell Handbook of Social Psychology. Oxford: Blackwell. Tyler, T. R. (1999). Why people cooperate with organizations. Research in Organizational Behavior, 21, 201-246. Tyler, T.R. (2000). Social justice: Outcome and procedure. International Journal of Psychology, 35, 117-125. Tyler, T. R., & Blader, S. L. (2000). Cooperation in groups: Procedural justice, social identity, and behavioral engagement. Philadelphia: Psychology Press. Tyler, T. R., & Blader, S. L. (2001). Identity and cooperative behavior in groups. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 4, 207-226. Tyler, T. R., & Blader, S. L. (2002). Autonomous vs. comparative status: Must we be better than others to feel good about ourselves? Organizational Behavior & Human Decision Processes, 89, 813-838. Tyler, T. R., & Blader, S. L. (2003). The group engagement model: Procedural justice, social identity, and cooperative behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 7, 349-361. Tyler, T. R., Degoey, P., & Smith, H. J. (1996). Understanding why the justice of group procedures matters: A test of the psychological dynamics of the group-value model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 913-930. Tyler, T. R. & Lind, E. A. (1992). A relational model of authority in groups. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 25, 115-191. New York: Academic Press. Identity matters, page 51 Van den Bos, K. & Lind, E.A. (2002). Uncertainty management by means of fairness judgments. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (vol. 34, pp. 1-60). N.Y.: Academic. van Knippenberg, D., & Ellemers, N. (2003). Social identity and group performance: Identification as the key to group-oriented effort. In S. A. Haslam, D. van Knippenberg, M. J. Platow, & N. Ellemers (Eds.) Social identity at work: Developing theory for organizational practice (pp. 29-42). Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press. van Knippenberg, D. (2000). Work motivation and performance: A social identity perspective. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 49, 357-371. van Knippenberg, D., van Knippenberg, B., De Cremer, D., & Hogg, M. A. (2004). Leadership, self, and identity: A review and research agenda. Leadership Quarterly, 15, 825-856. van Knippenberg, D., & van Schie, E. C. M. (2000). Foci and correlates of organizational identification. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 73, 137-147. van Leeuwen, E., van Knippenberg, D., & Ellemers, N. (2003). Continuing and changing group identities: The effects of merging on social identification and ingroup bias. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 679-690. Williams, L., & Anderson, S. (1991). Job satisfaction and organizational commitment as predictors of organizational citizenship and in-role behaviors. Journal of Management, 17, 601-617. Williams, K., Karau, S. J., & Bourgeois, M. (1993). Working on collective tasks: social loafing and social compensation. In M. A. Hogg & D. Abrams (Eds.), Group motivation: Social psychological perspectives (pp. 130-148). London: Harvester Wheatsheaf. Identity matters, page 52 Worchel, S., Rothgerber, H., Day, E. A.,, Hart, D., & Butemeyer, J. (1998). Social identity and individual productivity within groups. British Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 389-413. Wright, T. A., & Cropanzano, R. (2000). Psychological well-being and job satisfaction as predictors of job performance. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 5, 84-94.