Consequences of Perinatal Trauma - Genital Mutilation / Circumcision

advertisement
DRAFT RESEARCH PROPOSAL
CONSEQUENCES OF PERINATAL TRAUMA--GENITAL MUTILATION / CIRCUMCISION-AND SOMATOSENSORY AFFECTIONAL NURTURANCE
UPON THE ADULT BRAIN:
NUCLEAR MAGNETIC RESONANCE (NMR) AND POSITON EMISSION TOMOGRAPHY
(PET) SCAN EVALUATIONS OF BRAIN STRUCTURE AND FUNCTION
James W. Prescott, Ph.D.
Institute of Humanistic Science
5175 Luigi Terrace #35
San Diego, CA 92122
619: 455-0692
February 14, 1992
March 17, 1992 (REV)
CONSEQUENCES OF PERINATAL TRAUMA--GENITAL MUTILATION /
CIRCUMCISION--UPON THE ADULT BRAIN:
1
NUCLEAR MAGNETIC RESONANCE (NMR) AND POSITON EMISSION TOMOGRAPHY (PET)
SCAN EVALUATIONS OF BRAIN STRUCTURE AND FUNCTION
James W. Prescott, Ph.D.
Institute of Humanistic Science
A.
SPECIFIC AIMS
It is proposed that the extraordinary sensory bombardment of excruciating painful stimuli
associated with genital mutilation in infancy has deleterious effects upon the normal structural and
functional development of those brain structures associated with genital sensory
stimulation/function and the normative experiences of pain and pleasure. Such early trauma upon
the pleasure systems of the brain is proposed to have a deleterious impact upon the development
of affectional bonds that depends upon those intact brain structures that are essential for the
formation of affectional bonds in human relationships, particularly, the development of intimacy in
sexual affectional bonding.
It is further proposed that these early traumatic experiences of genital mutilation,
particularly, if they are uncorrected by subsequent positive pleasure experiences , are very likely
to be expressed later in life in personal and intrapersonal dysfunction involving depression,
alienation, anger, self-destructive and socially destructive behaviors. These hypotheses are based
upon the well established effects of early somatosensory stimulation and deprivation upon the
developing brain and behavior (Cannon, 1939; Cannon & Rosenbleuth, 1949; Berman, Berman
and Prescott, 1974; Coleman, 1971; Essman, 1971; Floeter & Greenough, 1979; Heath,
1968,1972ab, 1975, 1977; Higley, Suomi, Linnoila, 1991; Higley, Hasert, Suomi, Linnoila,1991;
Laudenslager, M.L., Reite, M., & Harbeck, 1982; Laudenslager and Reite, 1984; Laudenslager,
Capitanio and Reite, 1985; Melzack and Scott, 1957; Melzack and Burns, 1965; Prescott, 1967,
1968, 1971, 1975ab, 1976, 1977, 1979, 1980, 1990;
Reite, et. al., 1982; Riesen, Dickerson &
Struble, 1977; Rosenzweig, Krech, Bennett & Diamond 1968; Saltzberg, Lustick and Heath, 1971;
Struble and Riesen,1978; Sharpless, 1969, 1975; Snider & Maiti, 1976; and Tassinari, 1968; and
from the known effects of prenatal, perinatal and early infant trauma upon later adult functioning
(Anand & Hickey, 1987; Diamond, 1990; Faro & Windle, 1969; Jacobson, Eklund, Hamberger,
Linnarsson, Sedvall & Valverius, 1987; Jacobson, Nyberg, Eklund, Bygdeman & Rydberg, 1988;
Jacobson, Nyberg, Gronbladh, Edlund, Bygdeman & Rydberg;, 1990; Levy, 1945; Porter, Miller
& Marshall, 1986; and Salk, Lipsitt, Sturner, Reilly & Levat, 1985) and others.
1.
Consequences of Failure to Provide Effective Pain Relief During Surgical
Procedures Upon Physiology, Behavior and Health Status.
There is now incontrovertible evidence that the failure to provide effective pain relief during
surgical procedures in neonates results in such stress to the neonate that the psychophysiological
integrity of the neonate is so severely compromised that it results in enhanced morbidity and
mortality in such neonates. It is now informed medical opinion that "the response of newborns to
the stress of cardiac (Anand, Hansen and Hickey, 1990) and noncardiac (Anand, Sippell &
Aynsley-Green, 1987; Anand, Sippell, Schonfield & Aynsley-Green, 1988) operations is
substantially greater than that of adults " (Anand and Hickey, 1992). Specifically, Anand and
2
Hickey (1987) have previously proposed a physiologic basis "for the use of deep levels of
anesthesia and postoperative analgesia to attenuate the extreme responses of newborns to
perioperative pain and stress" (Anand & Hickey, 1992).
The findings of Anand & Hickey (1992) upon variations in the effectiveness of anesthesia
and postoperative analgesia in neonatal cardiac surgery are so dramatic and relevant to the study
proposed herein that direct quotations from their Results and Conclusions are particularly
warranted:
Results. The neonates who received deep anesthesia (with sufentanil) had significantly
reduced responses of beta-endorphin, norepinephrine, epinephrine, glucagon, aldosterone,
cortisol, and other steroid hormones; their insulin responses and rations of insulin to glucagon
were greater during the operation. The neonates who received lighter anesthesia (with halothane
plus morphine) had more severe hyperglycemia and lactic acidemia during more severe
hyperglycemia and lactic acidemia during surgery and higher lactate and acetoacetate
concentrations postoperatively (P<0.025). The group that received deep anesthesia had a
decreased incidence of sepsis (P=0.03), metabolic acidosis (P<0.01), and disseminated
intravascular coagulation (P=0 .03) and fewer postoperative deaths (none of 30 given sufentanil
vs. 4 of 15 given halothane plus morphine, P<0.01).
Conclusions. In neonates undergoing cardiac surgery, the physiologic responses to stress
are attenuated by deep anesthesia and postoperative analgesia with high doses of opioids. Deep
anesthesia continued postoperatively may reduce the vulnerability of these neonates to
;complications and may reduce mortality. (N Engl J Med 1992; 326: 1-9).
In the above study, the authors were particular to note and describe the mechanisms of
increased stress and its relationship to sepsis:
The increased incidence of sepsis in the halothane group may have been related to
postoperative changes in immune function; such changes have been correlated with hormonal
stress responses in adult patients. Beta-endorphins, glucocorticoids, catecholamines, and
prolactin are important regulators of immune responses; such interactions may be integrated by
the hypothalamus (p.7).
The consequences of significantly increased release of stress hormones upon the
developing brain and later behaviors have yet to be fully appreciated. The finding of extended
periods of non-rapid-eye-movement sleep in neonates undergoing circumcision without
anesthesia by Emde, et al (1971) is a case in point. The findings of Porter, Miller and Marshall
(1966) demonstrated that spectrographic analyses of neonatal pain cries could distinguish
between cries to circumcision and other stressors and that these neonatal cries could also be
distinguished by listening adults.
It takes no leap of faith or evidence to recognize that such pain states will interfere with the
maternal-infant bonding process which is known to have its own adverse consequences upon
brain structure, function and behavior (supra; and Marshal, et. al, 1982).
Although, it is beyond the scope of this introductory review to systematically cite numerous
animal studies on the effects of maternal stress upon fetal and neonatal brain development,
function and behavior, the study of Peters (1990) is illustrative of these kind of studies. Peters
(1990) reviewed earlier findings that maternal stress modifies 5-hydroxytryptamine (5-HT)
receptor binding in several brain regions of the adult offspring and alters the intensity of
behavioral responses to 5-HT receptor agonists. In a further study of the same maternal stressor
(crowding combined with daily saline injections during the final week of pregnancy), an elevated
maternal plasma free tryptophan level without significantly affecting total tryptophan was reported.
The increased maternal plasma tryptophan was associated with significantly increased fetal brain
levels of tryptophan, 5-HT and 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (5-HIAA, a metabolite of 5-HT). These
3
increases were found to be maintained at 10 days postnatal life.
Since 5-HT is recognized to have a role in the control of neuron development during the
perinatal period, it was suggested that the stress-induced increase in fetal brain 5-HT synthesis
may play a part in the mechanisms by which prenatal stress influences adult behavior.
Similar arguments can be made for the flooding of the neonatal brain with adrenal
corticosteroids and other stress hormones from the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal system
consequent to circumcision stress which have unknown long-term consequences for "encoding"
and influencing the developing brain with a possible permanent induction of a psychobiological
substrate or pattern of psychophysiological stress that interacts with, influences and confounds all
subsequent experiences of pain and pleasure.
This is to say that such profound neonatal pain involving the unique sensory-brain systems
that are designed to mediate pleasure may be contributory to the establishment of "sadomasochistic" processes at a most fundamental neurobiolgical level that is beyond the normative
range of conscious awareness. Additionally, the possible enhancement of "androgenization" of
the developing brain due to excessive output of adrenal androgens consequent to perinatal and
postnatal stress, e.g. circumcision, is a possible contributory mechanism to such later behaviors.
Another consideration. What extent, if any, are the mechanisms that convert androgens to
estrogens within the brain affected or impaired by perinatal and postnatal trauma? And what
are the long term behavioral implications of such a possibility? (Baum, 1979; Phoenix, Goy and
Resko, 1968; Diamond, Liacuna & Wong, 1973). What are the long-term consequences of a
"hyperandrogenization" of the developing brain for enhanced aggression; quality of male-female
relationships; and the capacity for intimacy in sexual relationships? Although, these questions are
not directly related to the immediate objectives of this proposal they do reflect issues that are
related to the overall consequences of perinatal and neonatal trauma, as it is reflected in acts of
circumcision, particularly, as these procedures have been conducted without analgesics or
anesthetics.
2.
Relationship of Perinatal Trauma To Later Violent Behaviors.
It is perhaps helpful and informative to describe in more detail some of the recent
experimental findings that link early perinatal trauma to adult violent behaviors. One of these most
dramatic studies was reported by Jacobson, et. al (1987). The summary of this study is provided
by the abstract which reads as follows:
The study was undertaken to test whether obstetric procedures are of importance for eventual
adult behavior of the newborn, as ecological data from the United States seem to indicate. Birth
record data were gathered for 412 forensic victims comprising suicides, alcoholics and drug
addicts born in Stockholm after l940, and who died there in 1978-1984. The births of the victims
were unevenly distributed among six hospitals. Comparison with 2,901 controls, and mutual
comparison of categories, showed that suicides involving asphyxiation were closely associated
with asphyxia at birth, suicides by violent mechanical means were associated with mechanical
birth trauma and drug addiction was associated with opiate and/or barbiturate administration to
mothers during labor. Irrespective of the mechanism transferring the birth trauma to adulthood-which might be analogous to imprinting--the results show that obstetric procedures should be
carefully evaluated and possibly modified to prevent eventual self-destructive behavior.
Specifically, the authors reported that perinatal Asphyxia ( a lack of oxygen) carried a risk
factor for suicides from hanging, strangulation, drowning and gas poisoning that was five
times greater than for controls; for perinatal mechanical trauma, e.g. breech presentations,
forceps delivery and multiple nuchal loops, the risk factor for suicides from hanging and other
mechanical injuries was twice as great as controls; for
the risk
4
factor for
was approximately three times greater than the controls.
In a subsequent study, Jacobson, et al (1988) evaluated the role of obstetric pain
medication in adult amphetamine addiction in the offspring. In this study of 200 amphetamine
addicts and 195 non-addicted sibling controls he reported that there was a 5.6 times greater
incidence of amphetamine addiction when nitrous oxide was given for 4.5 hours or longer vs. 0.25
hours or less. Based upon their statistical data, the authors also concluded:
To the extent that the cases in this study are representative for the addict population in
Stockholm, the number of amphetamine addicts in Stockholm can be estimated to have been less
than 60% of the present level if nitrous oxide had not been administered in the past (apparent
from data for various exposure levels, Fig. 3.).
In this writer's analyses of the data in Fig. 3, according to the percentage of amphetamine
addicts and non-addicted siblings for the five conditions of duration of nitrous oxide analgesia, the
risk factor for amphetamine addiction was 7.2 times greater when nitrous oxide was given for 4.5
hours or longer vs. 0.25 hours or less.
In this analyses, it should be noted that of the 141 amphetamine addicts there was a lower
percentage (17.0%) in the category of nitrous oxide duration of less than 0.25 hours than of the
154 non-addicted siblings where 27.9% fell into this category. In short, for this short duration of
nitrous oxide inhalation there were 64% fewer amphetamine addicts when compared to the
controls. Thus, this short term duration of nitrous oxide inhalation appears to confer some
prophylactic benefit for the prevention of amphetamine addiction when compared to the nonaddicted siblings.
For the most extreme group of nitrous oxide inhalation which was 4.5 hours or greater,
there was an 18.4 percent of amphetamine addicts in this group compared to 7.1 percent for the
non-addicted siblings. In short, there was a 159% greater incidence of amphetamine addicts in
this group compared to the incidence of non-addicted siblings.
When these two groups are compared, viz the 64% reduction of amphetamine addicts in
the short duration of nitrous oxide inhalation of less than 0.25 hours to the 159% increase of
amphetamine addicts in the longest duration of nitrous oxide inhalation of 4.5 hours or greater
(when compared to the controls), the increased risk for amphetamine addiction is 7.2 times
greater for the longest nitrous oxide inhalation group when compared to the shortest duration of
nitrous oxide inhalation of 0.25 hours or less.
The interesting unanswered question is why a mild exposure to nitrous oxide inhalation
would confer a "protective" function of reduced risk (64%) to amphetamine addiction when
compared to controls?.
In a third study by Jacobson, et al (1990) the risk for opiate addiction in adult offspring was
evaluated as a consequence of the administration of opiates, barbiturates and nitrous oxide for
greater than one hour in all subjects during labor within 10 hours before birth. There were 139
opiate addicts compared to 230 non-addicted siblings in this study.
It was found that in subjects who had subsequently become addicts there was a
significantly increased percentage of mothers (25%) who had received opiates or barbiturates, or
both, when compared to unmatched non-addicted siblings (16%); and these mothers also
received nitrous oxide for longer periods and more frequently. In short, there was a 56% increase
in opiate addiction, as a consequence of opiate, barbituate and nitrous oxide administration during
labor. After controlling for a number of other variables it was found that the risk for adult opiate
addiction increased to 4.7 times for the three drug administrations when compared to matched
sibling controls.
5
In an earlier study of 52 adolescent suicides, Salk, et. al., (1985) reported three
prenatal/perinatal risk factors that significantly discriminated the suicide group from the two
matched non-suicide control groups. These were: l) respiratory distress for more than one hour
at birth; 2) no antenatal care before 20 weeks of pregnancy; and 3) chronic disease of the mother
during pregnancy which were found in 81% of the suicide cases. These authors could not
speculate as to the mechanisms by which prenatal/perinatal birth trauma were linked to suicidal
behaviors.
David Levy in a 1945 study on the "Psychic Trauma of Operations In Children" reported on
three cases of male circumcision at ages 12 months (2) and 6-1/2 years. Psychological trauma
included the development of night terrors, temper tantrums and rage. In the 6-1/2 year old suicidal
impulses developed. Levy reports:
"...a circumcision at the age of 6 years 7 months, was preceded by a struggle of the patient
with his father and the anesthetist before they overpowered him. Immediately after the anesthesia
wore off, he said over and over, "They cut my penis. I wish I were dead." The rest of the day the
patient never left his mother's side. Thereafter his previous temper tantrums developed into
destructive rages. During the treatment he played numerous killing games, in which his father was
the principal victim. The operation represented a castration by his father."(p.10).
Questions must be raised as to the extent to which rage and suicidal behaviors are
engendered by the assaults of circumcision whether conducted during the newborn or child/prepubertal periods. (There are approximately a third more suicides than homicides in the U.S.). Is
there a link between circumcision and the willingness to kill oneself or others for one's religious or
national beliefs? The willingness of many fundamentalist monotheistic men to die or kill for their
religious beliefs is well known and the possible link of genital mutilation to these homicidal/suicidal
behaviors is more than worthy of study.
Jacobson, et al (l987) noted in their study that hypoxia during birth might cause minimal
brain damage that could result in destructive behaviors. However, these authors questioned
whether neurological injuries due to hypoxia could be the main factor, "since altogether rather few
victims suffered from asphyxia during birth. None of the 86 suicides from poisoning by solid or
liquid substance and none of the 53 alcoholics was reported to have been asphyxial."
There are several points to be made concerning these two studies. First, Faro and Windle
(1969) in their experimental asphyxial studies in monkeys documented both immediate and
delayed brain damage by as much as 10 years in these monkeys. In short, there is a delayed
"time bomb" of brain damage that occurs long after the initial injury or insult. It would appear that
later stages of brain development are dependent upon earlier stages of brain development which
must be normal if later stages of development are to be normal. Secondly, there are a variety of
traumas/injuries that can similate or mimic asphyxial effects. Thirdly, the criteria customarily
employed for the identification of "asphyxia" may be too insensitive to reflect the actual damage
sustained to the brain. Fourthly, there are interaction effects, e.g. maternal-infant separation or
isolation of prematures in incubators confers a degree of somatosensory deprivation that can
interact with mild degrees of hypoxia that can result in greater brain impairments than either injury
alone.
For example, sensory deprivation (like asphyxia) can damage the sensory receptors and
sensory pathways of the brain. This neuronal damage from sensory deprivation prevents normal
sensory stimulation of the brain, like asphyxia, which is essential for normal brain development
and function. The combination of these two events can and most likely are of greater damage
than either event taken separately (Prescott, 1975b).
The behavioral data from cross-cultural studies are summarized below that provides further
6
support for the role of early sensory experiences upon later behavior and thus, necessarily, the
brain processes that mediate such behaviors.
3.
CROSS-CULTURAL PERSPECTIVES: EARLY SENSORY INFLUENCES UPON BRAIN
DEVELOPMENT AND BEHAVIOR:
There is a well established body of scientific data that documents the role of sensory
stimulation and deprivation upon brain development and emotional-social, psychological and
mental development (supra, p.l). From the perspective of the developmental neuropsychological
sciences there can be little question that the extraordinary pain experienced by newborns,
children and adolescents who are subjected to ritual genital mutilations has a profound influence
upon the brain and later behaviors. It is this writers conviction that the extraordinary pain and
trauma experienced through genital mutilations--an organ and brain system that is designed for
the experience of sexual pleasure and the expression of sexual love--has permanently altered
normative brain development for the normal expression of sexual pleasure and love. It is
proposed that this genital pain, particularly in combination with the failure of affectional bonding in
human relationships has long-term developmental consequences for the ability of such individuals
to differentiate pain from pleasure in love relationships; to develop intimate sexual relationships;
and to be characterized as peaceful, egalitarian and compassionate individuals.
The above described relationships seem more plausible when it is considered that the brain
system which is designed for the experience of pleasure and the expression of sexual love is first
encoded with extraordinary and excruciating pain through genital mutilations. In such individuals,
all subsequent acts or experiences of genital pleasure are experienced upon a neuronal
background of genital pain that is now deeply buried in the subconscious/unconscious brain.
As previously stated, it is this developmental neuropsychologist's conviction that these early
experiences of genital pain contributes to the encoding of the brain that begins the neurobiolgical
foundation for sado-masochistic behaviors. The brain system that has been designed for pleasure
has first and foremost become saturated or encoded with pain that now limits and qualifies all
subsequent experiences of pleasure. When these early experiences of genital pain are followed
by a developmental deprivation of physical affectional pleasure in the maternal-infant relationship
and in the adolescent sexual relationship then violent destructive behaviors are an extremely likely
outcome (Prescott, 1975, 1979ab, 1989,1990).
A summary of the cross-cultural studies conducted by this writer that have related
measures of infant/child/adolescent pain (genital mutilation, harsh child rearing practices and
painful initiation rites); pleasure; and pleasure deprivation (failure of affectional binding in the
maternal-infant relationship and adolescent sexual repression) to later adult anti-social and violent
behaviors are provided in Tables 1-VII to provide further behavioral support for the basic
hypothesis of this study.
It should be noted that in primitive cultures, genital mutilations during infancy are rare and
commonly occurs at later childhood or at adolescence as initiation rites or "rites of passage".
Thus, genital mutilation rituals in these cultures are not expected to have the same consequences
upon the developing brain and behavior, as expected in those cultures that perform genital
mutilations during the neonatal period or early years of childhood.
Tables 1 and II summarize the social-behavioral characteristics of primitive cultures that
engage in male and female genital mutilations. Tables III & IV provides a similar summary for
those cultures that punish pre-marital and extramarital sex. Table VI provides a similar summary
for those cultures that inflict pain on the infant by the nurturant agent. In general these cultures
are patrilineal, subordinate women to men, have low nurturance of children, are sexually
7
repressive, violent and have a high god that is perceived as violent and involved in human
morality (Tables 1-VI).
Table VII summarizes the 49 primitive culture study which was able to predict with 100%
accuracy the torture, mutilation and killing of enemy captured in warfare in 49 primitive cultures
distributed throughout the world from two measures of deprivation of physical affection, viz. in the
maternal-infant relationship and in the adolescent sexual relationship (Prescott, l975;1979;1990).
The deprivation of infant physical affection alone predicted the adult violence and non-violence in
80% (39/49) of the cultures, thus, attesting to the powerful role of early sensory experiences upon
later adult behaviors. It is emphasized that there is other theory or data base that has yielded such
high predictive validity for violent and non-violent behaviors and which is based upon the known
sensory systems and brain processes that mediate these behaviors.
Further, there is persuasive scientific evidence that brain dysfunction/damage very likely
underlies those violent behaviors induced by Somatosensory Affectional Deprivation--SAD-(Prescott,1968,1975,1976; Saltzberg, Lustick and Heath,1971; Heath, 1972ab, l975; Berman,
Berman and Prescott,1974; Struble and Riesen,1978; Floeter and Greenough,1979).
Equally significant is the prediction that the cumulative consequences of these
developmental experiences of genital pain and affectional deprivation precludes the possibility of
realizing the spiritual dimensions of human sexuality. These relationships have been discussed
elsewhere and are mentioned herein to indicate some of the far-reaching implications of this SAD
theory and the proposed study (Prescott, 1990).
In summary, the above cross-cultural data provide support for the thesis that the
confounding of pain and pleasure during the formative periods of brain development has far
reaching implications for the development of patterns of adult behaviors and value systems that
involve peaceful and violent behaviors. It is again emphasized that this theory and data support
the hypothesis that the early encoding of those developing brain structures that are designed to
experience pleasure with excruciating pain provides the neuropsychological foundation for
individuals who are predisposed to experience pain to experience pleasure; or who derive
pleasure from the experience of pain, i.e. sado-masochistic behaviors which are reflected in the
measure of "torture, mutilation and killing of enemy captured in warfare", as reported in the crosscultural studies cited above.
The above cited studies and many others provide support for the unequivocal conclusion
that variations in the sensory/physical environment during the formative periods of brain
development influence the structural and functional development of the brain and behavior,
particularly, the effects of maternal stress upon fetal and neonatal brain development and
behavior,as well as the effects of post-natal stress of various kinds upon brain development and
behavior.
It has yet to be established, however, whether a specific form of extreme sensory trauma
to the developing brain, viz, genital mutilation, has demonstrable effects upon the structural and
functional development of the adult human brain. It is the objective of this research proposal to
attempt to document such effects and that this search is more than warranted by the available
scientific findings on the role of early sensory experiences and trauma upon brain development
and behavior.
4.
Specific Hypothesis To Be Tested
Specifically, it is proposed to evaluate the genital sensory projection fields in the cerebral
neocortex and cerebellar cortex; the frontal cortical fields defined by projections from medialis
dorsalis; somatosensory projection fields in cerebral neocortex and cerebellar cortex; and
selected limbic system structures in genital mutilated and intact adults utilizing NMR imaging of
8
the above described brain structures. Surface coil technology will be utilized to obtain optimal
imaging enhancement of specially selected brain structures. Visual and auditory sensory
projection fields will also be examined, as part of the "controls" in this study. Abnormalities are
not expected to be found in the visual and auditory sensory projection fields of the cerebral
neocortex and cerebellar cortex.
It is expected that MR Imaging will detect structural differences in the genital sensory
projection fields in the cerebral neocortex and cerebellar cortex and in those brain sites involved
in mediating pain and pleasure between genital mutilated, intact and super-nurtured subjects.
Additionally PET Scans will be conducted to evaluate how the brain processes positive
(non-exploitive, non-violent) images of a sensual-sexual-erotic nature and negative (exploitive,
violent) images of a sexual nature. It is anticipated that there will be less activation of neocortical
and cerebellar cortical structures associated with the above imagery in genital mutilated subjects
than in intact or super-nurtured subjects. This PET scan test will also be able to document the
variability of higher brain integrations of sexually pleasant and unpleasant imagery in the subjects
to be evaluated.
Although, the challenges are great to demonstrate the injuries of a single massive sensory
assault against the developing brain, it is anticipated that for the reasons cited above and with
appropriate subject selection and controls that the hypotheses will be confirmed. It is
emphasized that the continuing sequalae of pain and discomfort over many months, if not years,
during "recovery" has a continuing influence upon the brain and its development which enhances
the possibility of finding brain effects consequent to genital mutilation. As previously stated, it is
well know that not all consequences of early "insults" or "injuries" to the developing brain are
immediately manifested but are often delayed in their manifestation many years later (Levy,
1945; Faro and Windle, 1969; Jacobson, et al,1987, 1989, 1990 and Salk, et al., 1985).
Further, it is emphasized that it is not only the effects of acute sensory trauma upon brain
development and behavior that will be evaluated but also the necessary co-factors of diminished
or failed "affectional bonding" due to the impairments inflicted upon the pleasure sensing systems
of the brain (by this neonatal pain) that now becomes cumulative throughout development; and
which are also manifested in major differences of "affectional bonding" due to significant
differences in child rearing practices in the subjects tested. The expected impaired ability to
experience and integrate sexual pleasure with the higher neocortical systems of the brain, as a
consequence of limited early affectional bonding, will also be contributory to finding differences
between the experimental and control groups.
5.
Specific Brain Structures For Enhanced MR Imaging Evaluation
a) Cerebrum and Cerebral Neocortex
The rationale for selecting specific sites of the cerebral neocortex for enhanced
imaging rests upon the thalamocortical radiations to the neocortex (all information from the
sensory systems are routed to the cerebral neocortex through the dorsal thalamus); and from
specialized studies that document specific neocortical foci associated with stimulation of the
genital system. The sensory system of primary importance for this study is the somatosensory
system with its projections to somatosensory cortex (Sm l and Sm 11). The prefrontal neocortex
which has strong connections with the limbic system receives and integrates information about
sexuality and emotionality and is also a cortical area for intensive evaluation.
The specific genital sensory projection field within Somatosensory Cortex can be seen from
Figure l(Geschwind, 1980) which portrays the "homonculus"--a body image projection upon the
cerebral neocortex. Guerit and Opsomer (1991), not surprisingly, demonstrated that the largest
somatosensory cortical evoked potential (SCEP) from stimulation of the dorsal nerve of the
9
penis/clitoris is recorded over EEG position Cz (See Figure 2). The smaller SCEP from genital
stimulation compared to posterior tibial nerve stimulation was attributed, in part, to the anatomical
deeper location of the genital sensory projection field within the interhemispheric fissure (Penfield
and Rasmussen, 1950) and poses special challenges for MR Imaging Analysis.
Specific areas within the frontal lobe is another primary area for enhanced MR Imaging
Analyses. In numerous studies involving methods of stimulation, lesions and ablations, MacLean
and his associates have provided extensive documentation of the cerebral representation of
penile erection in squirrel monkeys (MacLean and Ploog, 1962; MacLean, l962; MacLean,
Denniston & Dua, 1963; MacLean, Dua & Denniston, 1963; Dua & MacLean, 1964; MacLean,
1978; MacLean, 1990).
These investigators found positive loci for erection from electrical stimulation in three
corticosubcortical subdivisions of the limbic system. The first subdivision corresponds to the
known anatomical distribution of hippocampal projections to parts of the septum, anterior and
midline thalamic nuclei, and hypothalamus. The second subdivision involves parts of the
anatomical system comprising the mammillary bodies, mammillothalamic tract, anterior thalamus,
and cingulate gyrus. The third subdivision involves loci found in the gyrus rectus, the medial part
of the medial dorsal nucleus of the thalamus, and regions of their known connections. Other
anatomical systems were found to be associated with penile tumescence, e.g. rhythmic
hippocampal after-discharges where the animal assumed a quiescent state upon termination of
the hippocampal after-discharges; and the ponto-cerebellar system.
The finding most relevant to this study is the central role that the medial dorsal nucleus of
the thalamus has in penile tumescence because of the projections of medialis dorsalis to
prefrontal cortex, particularly, orbitofrontal cortex. As MacLean (1962) noted: "Indeed, it (medialis
dorsalis) is such a potent point that stimulation here may elicit close to full erection even under
deep anesthesia" (p.299). MacLean (1962) continues:
The prefrontal cortex, which is connected to the medial dorsal nucleus, is a relatively recent
addition to the new mammalian brain. From our limited knowledge of its functions, it might be
inferred that it is largely concerned with anticipation and planning as it pertains to both selfpreservation and preservation of the species (p.299); and
In regard to this last finding it is pertinent to recall that frontal lobotomy, which severs the
connections between the medial dorsal nucleus and the orbitofrontal and prefrontal cortex,
sometimes results in bizarre, uninhibited sexual behavior (p.294).
It does not take a great deal of interpretive skills to suggest that genital trauma may induce
damage/dysfunction in medialis dorsalis and its resulting connectivity with orbitofrontal and
prefrontal cortex. Such damage/dysfunction could well provide the beginning neural substrate for
the latter manifestation of sexual-social dysfunctions of various kinds dependent upon
subsequent developmental experiences involving the affectional systems of the brain.
More specifically, Prescott (1971) outlined the special importance of medialis dorsalis and
its projections to orbitofrontal and prefrontal cortex with respect to his SAD(Somatosensory
Affectional Deprivation) theory of depression, sociopathy and violent behaviors:
The inhibitory forebrain areas, particularly the frontal orbital cortex, are implicated in this
theoretical neuronal model not only because of the study of Anand, et al. (1959) but also because
of those studies by Kennard and Ectors (1938); Kennard, Spencer and Fountain (1941); and
Ruch and Shenkin (1943) in which lesions in frontal orbital cortex (Area 13 of Walker) produced
hyperactivity in the form of 'pacing' or 'forced circling' movement stereotypes which would
perseverate for as long as the test period of three hours. Equally important was the observation of
a 'definite reduction in emotional expression' with reduction or elimination of 'fear' and 'aggressive'
10
behaviors which were apparently replaced by 'gazing into the distance with a blank expression'
when the animal was not engaged in stereotypical pacing or running. This was interpreted as
an'over-reactive' response to the presence of an observer. The dramatic similarities of these
behaviors in animals with frontal orbital cortex lesions with the behaviors of isolation reared
animals hardly need further emphasis.
Additional evidence implicating the frontal orbital gyri (Areas 13 & 14) in the behavioral
disorders of the somatosensory deprived animal are the marked autonomic changes produced by
stimulation of these areas, particularly, the development of sham rage by stimulation of Area 14
(Livingstone and Davis, 1947). More recent evidence has been obtained in implicating the orbital
gyri in emotional behaviors in the study of Butter,et al. (1970), in which orbital frontal lesions in
rhesus produced hyper-reactivity, heightened oral tendencies, and a reduction in aggressive
behaviors. As noted above, it is of more than passing interest that sham rage was also produced
by cerebellar stimulation (Dow and Moruzzi, 1958), which may be suggestive of further support for
a possible functional link between the cerebellum and frontal orbital gyri.
It should be noted that any cerebellar-orbital gyri linkage that would be mediated by
thalamic relay nuclei would necessarily involve the dorsomedial nucleus since Rose and Woolsey
(1948) have proposed that the frontal granular cortex be defined as that part of the frontal lobe
which serves as a projection areas for the dorsomedial nucleus. More specifically, it is known that
medialis dorsalis (MD) consists of three distinct substructures: Pars paralamellaris, Pars
parvocellularis, and Pars magnocellularis which projects to Areas 8, 9, and orbitalis (Areas 1, &
14), respectively, within frontal granular cortex (Akert, 1964).
Thus, Pars magnocellularis of medialis dorsalis (MD) assumes a critical role in any
thalamic mediated cerebellar-orbital gyri linkage. Evidence to support this actual anatomical
linkage is sparse; however, the study of Nimi, et al. (1962) demonstrated terminal degeneration in
medialis dorsalis subsequent to unilateral lesions of the cerebellar nuclei which is supportive of
these speculations. The finding of Fox (1949) that pars magnocellularis of DM receives pathways
from the amygdala and of Guillery (1959), who traced fibers to it from the midbrain tegmentum
and septal region, give greater interpretative significance to Heath's (1969/1975) findings of septal
and cerebellar spiking activity in the somatosensory deprived rhesus.
It is appropriate to mention the observations of Dow (1970) that the extraordinary
development of the neocerebellum in the higher primate corresponds specifically to the
development of the frontal and temporal association areas and not to the cerebral cortex as a
whole, and that projections from association areas of the frontal and temporal lobes to the pons in
man constitute 1/5 of the whole of the internal capsule (as the fronto and tempero-pontine fibers).
Thus, sufficient neuroanatomical, neurophysiological and behavioral evidence appears to exist to
encourage speculation of a cerebellar--orbital frontal--limbic--reticular circuit in the mediation of
abnormal emotional behaviors (pp10-11).
It is for the above reasons that special attention must be given to Areas 8,9,13 and 14
within the frontal granular cortex in the enhanced MR Imaging analyses of these brain structures.
b) Cerebellum
There is substantial evidence that the cerebellum plays a major role in the regulation
of autonomic nervous system activity and emotional-social behaviors (Prescott, 1976). It is
beyond the scope of this proposal to review this evidence as background for the study of
cerebellar structures in this proposed research. A few studies should suffice.
Anand, et. al (1959), utilizing evoked potential techniques, demonstrated strong functional
11
relationships between the paleocerebellum and limbic system structures. They report:
(i) Projections from posterior paleocerebellum. Stimulation of the posterior paleocerebellar
structures, including the flocculonodular lobe, evoked potentials in all the limbic regions from
which recordings were obtained. The results indicated quite heavy projections from the
flocculonodular and adjoining posterior regions.
(ii) Projections from anterior paleocerebellum. Stimulation of anterior paleocerebellum
evoked potentials mainly to orbital cortex and hippocampus. There was evidence also of
connections with the hypothalamus (p.452).
Specifically, stimulation of the lingula, centralis, and culmen of cerebellar cortex resulted in
evoked potentials in the limbic region; and stimulation of the pyramis, uvula, nodulus,
paraflocculus and flocculus resulted in evoked potentials in the orbital surface of the cerebral
cortex. These authors conclude:
The present observations point to the existence of extensive projections from the posterior
part of the paleocerebellum (including the flocculonodular lobe) to most of the limbic structures of
frontal and temporal lobes as well as to the hypothalamic regions. These projections appear to be
especially strong to orbitomesial cortex, and amygdaloid-hippocampal complex"(p.456).
Additionally, MacLean, Denniston and Dua (1963) reported positive loci for penile erection
in the substantia nigra, pons and the brachium pontis, thus, suggesting the involvement of a
"nigro-ponto-cerebellar" pathway in penile erection.
Heath (1972ab, 1975) has provided substantial electrophysiological evidence for extensive
connections between the cerebellum and limbic system structures through stimulating and
recording from the deep cerebellar nuclei.
In addition to the above specific areas of the cerebellum that have been identified for
enhanced MR Imaging there is merit to also examining the corticopontine projection areas on the
cerebellar cortex which involves primarily Lobules H1-H IX of the intermediate-lateral cerebellar
hemispheres.
B
SIGNIFICANCE
The effects of sensory deprivation or altered and abnormal sensory environments upon
brain development and behavior are well documented. It is this mechanism of somatosensory
trauma and deprivation which is proposed to be the mediating process of a variety of early
traumatic events of obstetric drugs, genital pain, sensory deprivation and other stressors,
including asphyxia that damages the affectional neuronal systems of the brain. It is this damage
that compromises or prevents the development of affectional bonds of pleasure since the brain
systems that mediate pleasure have been damaged by these early insults and injuries that began
in utero. Thus, the presence of postnatal TLC or somatosensory affectional stimulation may not
be assimilated by the infant because of prenatal/perinatal neuronal damage to the pleasure
systems of the brain. This thesis constitutes the neuropsychological foundation of my SAD
(Somatosensory Affectional Deprivation) theory of alienation, depression, addiction and violent
behaviors.
It is for these reasons that SAD is proposed to be the process or mechanism which can
account for the linkage between prenatal/perinatal trauma, obstetric drugs and suicides that have
12
been reported by Salk, et al.(1985), and by Jacobson, et, al. (1987, 1988, 1990). It is emphasized
that the first consequences of SAD are hyper-reactivity and depression which are the precursors
to other forms of violence, particularly that of suicide. Unfortunately, neither the Salk, et al. nor
Jacobson, et al studies have reported on the degree of infant isolation in incubators or the degree
of post-natal somatosensory affectional deprivation of the infant/child who later became addicted
or committed suicide. It is emphasized that these phenomena must be quantified in any future
developmental studies of this kind, if the most valid and reliable assessment of early
insults/injuries are to be assessed.
The SAD (Somatosensory Affectional Deprivation) theory is offered as an alternate
explanatory mechanism to that of "imprinting" that has been offered by Jacobson, et al (1987,
1988, 1990) for the explanation of the consequences of adult drug addiction and suicide to
perinatal trauma.
In further elaboration, Somatosenory Affectional Deprivation (SAD) theory is based on the
nature of brain processes where it has been well established that activation of the pleasure
circuits of the brain inhibit the activity of the violence circuits of the brain. This normative
reciprocal relationship ceases to function when the neuronal circuits of the brain that mediate
pleasure are insufficiently developed--a form of brain dysfunction/damage--due to sensory
deprivation of physical affectional pleasure, specifically, somesthetic (touch) and vestibular
(movement) sensory stimulation. Gentle touch and rocking of the infant are powerful sensory
stimuli for pleasure, comfort and a sense of "basic trust" for the infant/child. When these sensory
stimuli are absent and, in addition, are replaced by painful sensory stimulation, then the
neurobiological foundation for depressive, addictive and violent behaviors is established.
In brief, the emotional pain of affectional deprivation--the failure of love--in the maternalinfant and parent-child relationships; and in our sexual relationships seeks relief through drugs,
alcohol and violence--in the rage of our physical assaults against others; beatings, rape, homicide
or assaults against our own bodies in self-mutilation and drug addiction where the final solution is
suicide--one of the leading causes of death in our children and youth. Why should our children
and youth be committing suicide in such great numbers--a 112% increase from l980 to l985 for
children aged 10 to 14 years? Does genital mutilation in combination with SAD facilitate suicidal
and assaultive behaviors?
A further correlate to the above relationships and why certain postnatal traumas are
tolerated (genital mutilations) is to be found in our cultural mores that places a high moral value
on pain, suffering, deprivation; and, conversely, immorality upon the physical pleasures of the
body, particularly the sexual pleasures of the body. It is for these reasons that the pain of genital
mutilations are tolerated and supported and why the pleasures of genital stimulation are
commonly forbidden, e.g. masturbation--with all the developmental neuropsychological and moral
consequences that such events have upon the individual and culture. These issues have been
reviewed elsewhere and are mentioned herein, to indicate the developmental and cultural
complexity of the phenomena to be studied, namely, the consequences of genital mutilation upon
brain development and behavior (Prescott, 1989)
For all of the above reasons it is reasonable to pursue an exploratory study of the possible
consequences to the developing brain from the sensory trauma inherent in genital mutilations.
Positive findings would, necessarily, have a dramatic effect upon raising the level of human
consciousness of the world that is permitting such damage through genital mutilations upon
helpless newborns and children throughout the world.
In addition to providing scientific information to help end the torture and mutilation of
children inherent in genital mutilations, these research findings would also provide a foundation
for later studies that would examine the possible linkages of these early traumas upon the
development of later violent and sado-masochistic behaviors, particularly its contribution to the
various forms of sexual violence that are all too prevalent in those cultures that inflict genital
13
mutilation upon infants and children. In this respect, the commentary of MacLean (1962) on the
relationship between sexuality, fear and aggression is relevant:
Sexuality, fear and aggression: The foregoing considerations are germane to the close
connection between sexuality and fear and aggression. As is well recognized, fighting is
frequently a preliminary to both feeding and mating. One sees combative behavior even in the
nursing babe which will angrily fight the breast if no milk if forthcoming, and at the same time
develop penile erection. Proceeding caudally from the level the level of the anterior commissure,
one can follow neural structures involved in fearful or angry, combative behavior lying proximal to
those concerned with feeding and sexual responses. Within the space of a millimeter, one may
pass from a point at which stimulation results in erection and an apparent state of placidity to one
at which the electrical current elicits erection in conjunction with an angry or fearful ;type of
vocalization and showing of fangs ((MacLean and Ploog, 1962). As one lowers the electrode a
little deeper, one may obtain only fearful or angry-appearing manifestations during stimulation, yet
see erection appear as a rebound phenomenon after stimulation is terminated.
One is reminded of a statement in Freud's "Three Contributions to the Theory of Sex": "The
sexually exciting influence of some painful affects such as fear, shuddering, and horror is felt by a
great many people throughout life and readily explains why so many seek opportunities to
experience such sensation..." (3, p.63). One thinks of pyromania, for example. (p.296).
I would add other examples besides pyromania that may be more relevant to our cultural
condition: the significant rise and predominance of horror films; "slasher" films, sexual violence in
films and in real life (rape). It is not an excess of speculation to suggest that dysfunction in the
basic neural systems of the brain that regulate sexuality, emotionality, social-sexual relationships
and "Consciousness" may well underlie the prevalence of sexual sociopathy and psychopathy in
our culture. And that genital mutilation and other forms of assault to the development of the
pleasure/ affectionale systems of the brain would constitute significant contributions to those brain
dysfunctions that, undoubtedly, underly these social-cultural disorders.
On the positive side and from an evolutionary perspective there are also significant
implications on the role of the above discussed brain structures for the development of social and
sexual altruism and the realization of the spiritual dimensions of human sexuality which must
include an integration of the sexual systems of the limbic brain with the frontal lobes and
cerebellum (Prescott, 1976ab,1990). It is here that the commentary of MacLean (1962) on
sexuality and altruism is also of particular relevance:
Sexuality and altruism: Thus far we have considered neural and behavioral findings that
would appear to shed some light on psychiatric observations that the acts of mastering, devouring
and procreating seem to be inextricably tied to one another.
In concluding, we have yet to consider a subject that goes to the roots of man's idealism
and altruism--namely, the close connection of feeding, sustaining and sexuality. A concern for the
welfare and preservation of the species is based on sexuality. The mother when feeding her baby
at breast experiences well-being and may have sensations associated with tumescence in the
genital region. Subjective analyses have shown that there may be a comparable reaction in the
feelings of those who find joy in helping and sustaining others.
In giving the results of the stimulation studies we pointed out that penile erection was
obtained by stimulating structures along the course of the mammillothalamic tract and in parts of
the anterior and medial thalamic nuclei (MacLean and Ploog, 1962). Heretofore these structures
have been looked upon as silent areas of the brain. The mammillothalamic pathway, the pathway
numbered three in Figure 2, is, as LeGros Clark and Meyer emphasized, not found in the reptilian
brain but appears for the first time in the mammal (Clark and Meyer, 1950). It is possibly pertinent
to the increasingly complicated sociosexual behavior that one finds in ascending the phylogenetic
14
scale of the mammal, that this pathway and its related nuclei attain their greatest size in man.
Thus there seems to be support for the assumption that Nature did not stop at the primitive level
of the hypothalamus, but endowed us with considerable brain stem and cortex with which to
promote the preservation of the species.
It may be particularly significant that the medial dorsal nucleus which projects to the orbital
and prefrontal cortex develops in association with the anterior group of nuclei and literally
embraces them. As pointed out, the ancient midline portion of the medial dorsal nucleus is
implicated in penile erection. Indeed, it is such a potent point that stimulation here may elicit close
to full erection even under deep anesthesia (MacLean and Ploog, 1962).
The prefrontal cortex, which is connected to the medial dorsal nucleus, is a relatively recent
addition to the new mammalian brain.
It is here that we must return to the cerebellum and the concept of the Quadrune Brain
(Prescott, 1983) and its role in the regulation of social-sexual and emotional behaviors and its
further role as an integrative system of higher brain systems --the frontal lobe--(the brain of
Consciousness) with that of lower brain systems--limbic brain--(the brain of the Sub-Conscious
and Unconscious). As Dow (1974) has noted there is an evolutionary significance of the
cerebellum that is poorly understood:
This extreme phylogenic development (of the cerebellum) is not related to the cerebral
cortex as a whole but specifically to the development of the frontal and temporal association
areas whose projection to the pons in man as the fronto-and temporo-pontine fibers makes up
one-fifth of the whole of the internal capsule (p.110).
....Something important seems to happen to convulsive phenomena, to a variety of
vegetative function, to emotional behavior and perhaps even to the highest level of intellectual
function when a cerebellar influence is introduced into the nervous system. Whatever it does to
modify the activity of other parts of the brain, it probably does the same thing in all its possible
varied roles. Its uniformity of structure throughout vertebrates and within its various division in
higher mammals makes this most likely. (p.115)
The evolutionary and developmental significance of the cerebellum in understanding the
higher functions of homo sapiens and the cultural evolution of homo sapiens has been uniquely
addressed by Torgersen (1954) which merits restatement here:
The data indicate that girls have a larger cerebellum in early childhood, whereas boys
probably have a slightly larger cerebral cortex. The rapid growth of the cerebellum in girls
parallels the precocious ossification and maturation in the female sex. It is probable that
endocrinology in later years has prevailed to such an extent that the role of the central nervous
system in maturation has been overlooked. The observations recorded indicate that the
cerebellum promotes maturation whereas the cerebral cortex has a retarding effect. This is also
what is to be expected considering the importance of a slow maturation rate to the processes of
learning involved in cerebral activity.
The sex differences in muscular performances have probably appeared too obvious to
appeal to the scientific mind. As known, girls generally prefer to put the thread in the needle, boys
vice versa. The cerebellar pattern of coordination in girls expresses itself in "grace" and in
particular performances like dancing, writing, sewing and not least talking. Stuttering is rare in
girls. May this be due to their relatively well developed cerebellum? (p.411).....and
Considering the small muscular mass compared with the apes, man is a cerebellar
species. The decrease in brain volume which has taken place since the middle pleistocene
probably mainly concerns the cerebral cortex. The remarkable variability of the brain weight with a
range of 1000-2000 grams is characteristic of a new specific trait. Man is by far the youngest
15
among the higher species and the human brain (is) in a stage of reorganization which tends to
eliminate the cortical representation of ancient structures which are about to lose their functional
significance. The net result will be an increase in relative cerebellar weight. Most probably the
cerebellum is a "coming" organ in human evolution.
The frequent disturbances of coordination in higher cortical functions such as speech,
indicate that man is in a stage of what may be called cerebro-cerebellar schizophrenia, the cortex
being precociously evolved compared with the cerebellum. As regarding specific traits in man
generally, the cerebro-cerebellar proportion evolves towards a feminization of the species and a
better coordination.
No wonder that men of ideas like Goethe and Bolk did not find it a waste of time to meditate
on the craniovertebral border and the folia of the cerebellum, nor that the following generations
were stimulated by their ideas. Few fields in morphology show how much modern experimental,
genetic and biometric research owes to the knowledge which has been accumulated by earlier
generations, and to the inspiration which emanates from morphological tradition (p.416).
Although, there may be disagreement with some of these perspectives by Torgersen, there
can be little doubt that the evolutionary and developmental significance of the cerebellum in the
higher functions of homo sapiens has been seriously underestimated and neglected. This is
particularly the case for the human functions of aggression/warfare;sexuality;
superstition/religiosity; parapsychism and spirituality.
These latter issues have been addressed by many traditional scientists, as well as, non-traditional
thinkers such as Gooch (1980) who has expressed an innovative and extraordinary vision of the
role of the cerebellum in human mental life.
The significance of this research proposal goes well beyond the immediate objectives of
documenting the effects of a particular onerous perinatal trauma upon the developing brain-genital mutilation. The ultimate significance of this research project addresses some of the most
challenging concepts of the brain and behavior in the evolution of the human species, as they
have been presented by MacLean (1962, 1990);
Heath (1964); Dow (1974, 1992); Torgersen (1954); and Prescott (1971, 1972, 1976ab,1990).
The issues of brain evolution and behavior addressed in this proposal must ultimately
involve the spiritual dimensions of being human where sexuality is at the core of that spirituality
and which has special implications for the role of the feminine in that sexuality and in the survival
of the human species itself.
In summary, if the brain systems associated with sexuality, in the broadest meaning of that
term, are damaged in the male through genital mutilation and/or other assaults and traumas, then
it may never be possible for such males to participate in nor fully understand the higher
psychological, (e.g. altruistic, nurturant and spiritual) processes associated with sexual
functioning--a functioning that is already burdened and crippled with a psychology of shame and a
religion of moral guilt. Positive findings from this research project would illuminate the deep
schism of alienation that exists between male and female at the most fundamental
neurobiological and neuropsychological levels which, in turn, can only further illuminate the
disastrous consequences of such events for the future of the human species.
And the findings from this research project can only contribute to that "morphological
tradition" that can yield "inspiration" for future generations.
C.
EXPERIMENTAL DESIGN
16
It is recognized that there is difficulty in demonstrating the effects of a single injurious
assault of limited duration upon the developing brain, although such insults to the developing
brain have been previously demonstrated in other scientific studies, e.g., Faro and Windle (1969).
An important part of this difficulty are the ameliorative influences of subsequent positive
developmental life experiences upon the developing brain and behavior. For these reasons, it is
imperative to control, as much as possible, for the presence or absence of such ameliorative
developmental life experiences in the selection of the subject sample for study. The crucial
variables of control involve the degree of somatosensory stimulation and deprivation (presence or
absence of pain or pleasure, specifically, Somatosensory Affectional Deprivation--SAD) and of
sexual activity/expression during the formative periods of development. It is these considerations
that have influenced the selection of the subject sample and experimental design that are
described below.
Subject Sample: The subject sample will consist of four groups of subjects consisting of
six subjects per group for a total of twenty -four male subjects for study. Subjects will be 21 years
of age or greater. The four groups are as follows:
A.
Group A subjects will have experienced extensive genital mutilation that has required
reconstructive surgery. Attempts will be made to obtain subjects with genital amputation and
where reconstructive surgery has been performed to change the sex gender from male to
female. This group represents the most extreme condition of genital mutilation.
B.
Group B subjects will consist of those males who have been strongly emotionally and
psychologically affected by circumcision to the extent that they have sought corrective procedures
to restore the penile foreskin.
C.
Group C subjects will consist of those males who have not been emotionally or
psychologically impacted by being circumcised. These males will be characterized as being
psychologically indifferent to the fact of their circumcised state.
D.
Group D subjects will be a non-circumcised control group that will be drawn from La Leche
League type families. This group will represent a "super-nurturant" control group that will reflect
the optimal developmental conditions for optimal brain development. The selection criteria for this
group are listed below.
DEVELOPMENTAL CRITERIA FOR OPTIMAL CONTROL GROUP
1.
No use of alcohol, drugs or tobacco during pregnancy.
2.
No complications of pregnancy, delivery and early post-natal life.
3.*
No circumcision.
4.
Apgar score eight or greater.
5.*
No separation of infant from mother at birth; nor during first two years of
postnatal life (not greater than 24 hours).
6.*
Breastfeeding for at least two years desirable--not less than one year.
7.*
Carrying of infant on body of mother is frequent.
8.*
No placement of infant/child in day care before two years of life.
9.*
Little or no use of physical punishment as a common practice of
discipline in child rearing.
10.
Low maternal stress: low spousal conflicts; dual parents and/or
significant extended family support, i.e. a happy family.
17
11.
Early onset of sexual activity. (Age of onset and degree of sexual
activity will be obtained. Subjects with early rather than later age of onset
sexual activity will be selected for this group).
12.
Positive sex values.,e,g, masturbation is a normal healthy sex activity.
of
The following information will be obtained, wherever possible, for the other three study
groups:
1.
Use of alcohol, drugs or tobacco during pregnancy.
2.
Complications of pregnancy, delivery and early post-natal life.
3.*
Complications of circumcision; difficult recovery from circumcision
4.
Apgar score .
5.*
Extent of separation of infant from mother at birth or
during first two years of postnatal life.
6.*
Extent of Breastfeeding .
7.*
Extent of carrying of infant on body of mother or caretaker.
8.*
Time of onset and extent of placement of infant/child in day care .
9.*
Time of onset and extent of physical punishment in the discipline of the
10.
Extent of maternal and family stress:,e.g., spousal conflicts;
11.
Whether and when being a single parent and nature of extended family
support.
12.
Time of onset and nature of sexual activity (Age of onset and nature/degree
of sexual activity will be elicited) , as well as a history of sexual values.
child.
The starred items are essential information variables. Given the design of this study where
subjects are selected according to a developmental history of enriched vs impoverished
Somatosensory Affectional Stimulation, it will be possible to address for the first time the
consequences of such early sensory stimulation/deprivation upon brain development whether or
not there are significant differences found on the circumcision variable. In short, the scientific
outcome of this study is not limited to only the circumcision variable. Positive findings on the
somatosensory affectional deprivation-stimulation continuum alone would fully justify the
conduction of this pilot study.
D.
SUBJECT IDENTIFICATION AND SELECTION PROCESS
To be developed in concert with:
NOCIRC
Paul M. Fleiss, M.D. M.P. H.: La Leche League control subjects
James L. Snyder, M.D.: Genital reconstructive surgery group
James Bigelow, Ph.D. Foreskin reconstruction group
Others
E.
SPECIFIC BRAIN STRUCTURES FOR ENHANCED MR IMAGING ANALYSES
Cerebral Neocortex
l. Somatosensory Cortex: Sm I & Sm II
2. Cz: deep interhemispheric fissure: genital sensory projection field
18
3. Frontal Granular Cortex:
a) Areas 8 and 9
b) Areas 13 and 14: Orbital
Cerebellum
1. Posterior Lobe: Paleocerebellum
a) Flocculus
b) Paraflocculus
c) Nodulus
d) Uvula
e) Pyramis
2. Anterior Lobe: Paleocerebellum
a) Lingula
b) Centralis
c) Culmen
3.
Neocerebellum: Intermediate-Lateral Cerebellar Hemispheres (Cortico-Pontine
Projection Areas)--See Diagram
a) H I-Intermediate
b) H II-Intermediate
c) H III-Intermediate
d) H IV-Lateral
e) H V-Lateral
f) H VI-Intermediate/Lateral
g) H VIIA- Intermediate/Lateral
h) H VIIB- Intermediate/Lateral
I) H VIIIA- Intermediate/Lateral
j) H VIIIB- Intermediate
k) H IX-- Intermediate
F.
BUDGET--To be developed.
G.
Co-Sponsor: NOCIRC.
H.
MEDICAL FACILITY: Department of Pediatrics, University of California, San
Diego and/or other
SELECTED REFERENCES
19
Anand, B.K., Malhotra, C.L., Singh, B. & Dua, S. (1959). Cerebellar projections to
system. J. Neurophysiology, 22 (4), 451-457.
limbic
Alexander, R.C., Schor, D.P. & Smith, W.L. (1986). Superiority of magnetic resonance
imaging for documenting intercranial injuries resulting from shaking.Abstract. The
University of Iowa Hospitals and Clinics. Iowa
Anand, K.J.S. and Hickey, P.R. (1987). Pain and its effects in the human neonate and
The New England J. of Med., (Nov 19). V 317 No. 21.
fetus.
Anand, K.J.S., Sippell , W.G., & Aynsley-Green, A. (1987). A randomized trial of
fentanyl
anaesthesia in preterm neonates undergoing surgery: effects on the
stress response. Lancet
1: 62-6. (Erratum, Lancet 1987; 1: 243-8).
Anand, K.J.S., Sippell , W.G., Schonfield, N.M., & Aynsley-Green, A. (1988). Does
anaesthesia decrease the metabolic and endocrine stress responses of newborn
undergoing operation? BMJ 296: 668-72.
halothane
infants
Anand, K.J.S. , Hansen, D.D,. & Hickey, P.R. (1990) . Hormonal-metabolic stress
in neonates undergoing cardiac surgery. Anesthesiology. 73:661-70.
responses
Anand, K.J.S., & Hickey, P.R.
(1992).
Halothane-Morphine compared with high-dose
sufentanil for anesthesia and postoperative analgesia in neonatal cardiac surgery.
N
Eng J Med. 326 (1): 1-9.
Baum, M.J. (1979). Differentiation of coital behavior in mammals--A comparative
Neurosci. Biobeh. Rev. 3: 265-284.
analysis.
Berman, A.J., Berman, D. & Prescott, J.W. (1974). The effect of cerebellar lesions on
emotional behavior in the rhesus monkey. In: The Cerebellum, Epilepsy and
Behavior.
(Cooper, I.S., Riklon, M.V. & Snider, R.S. (Eds) Plenum, NY
Butter, C.M, Snyder, D.R., and McDonald, J.A. (1970). Effects of orbital frontal lesions
on
aversive and aggressive behaviors in rhesus monkeys. J. comp. physiol.
Psycholo. 72: 132144.
Cannon, W.B. A Law of Denervation (1939). Amer. J. Med. Sci. 193, 737-749
Cannon, W.B. and Rosenbleuth, A. (1949). The Supersensitivity of Denervated
MacMillan, NY
Structures.
Clark, W.E. LeGros and Meyer, M. (1950). Anatomical relationships between the
cortex and the hypothalamus. Brit. Med. Bull., 6:341-344.
cerebral
Coleman, M. (1971). Platelet serotonin in disturbed monkeys and children. Clinical
the Children's Hospital. 27(7), 187-194.
Proceed. of
Diamond, M., Liacuna, A. & Wong, C. (1973). Sex Behavior after neonatal progesterone,
testosterone, estrogen or antiandrogens. Horm. Behav. 4 (102): 73-88.
Diamond, M. (1992). Bisexualities: A Biological Perspective. In: Bisexualities, Vol.4
(E. Haeberle, Ed). Walter de Gruyter, Berlin. In Press.
Dow, R.S. (1970). Personal communication.
20
Dow, R.S. (1974). Some Novel Concepts of Cerebellar Physiology. The Mount Sinai Journal
Medicine. Vol XLI, No. 1, Jan-Feb, pp. 103-119.
of
Dow, R.S., Kramer, R., & Robertson, L. (1991). Diseases of The Cerebellum (Chap.37,
Vol.3). In: Clinical Neurology, Robert J. Joynet, Ed., Lippencott, New York.
Dua, S. and MacLean, P.D. (1964). Localization for penile erection in medial frontal
American J. Physiology. V. 207, No. 6. Dec., pp. 1425-1434.
lobe.
The
Emde, R.N., Harmon, R.J., Metcalf, D. Koenig, K.L., & Wagonfeld, S. (1971). Stress and
neonatal sleep. Psychosom Med 33:491-7.
Essman, W.B. (1971). Neurochemical changes associated with isolation and
stimulation. Biological Psychiatry, 3, 141.
environmental
Faro, M.D. and Windle, W.V. (l969): Transneuronal degeneration in brains of
asphyxiated at birth. Experimental Neurology 24, 38-53.
monkeys
Floeter, M.K. and Greenough, W.T. (1979). Cerebellar plasticity: Modification of
structure by differential rearing in monkeys. Science, 206, 227- 229.
purkinje cell
Fox, C.A. (1949). Amygdalo-thalamic connections in Maccaca mulatta. Anat. Rec. 103:
538.
Freud, S. (1948). Three contributions to the theory of sex. (Brill, A.A., Transl.). Nervous
Mental Disease Monogr. New York.
537and
Geschwind, N. Specializations of the Human Brain (1980). In: Mind and Behavior:
Readings
from Scientific American (R.L. Atkinson and R. C. Atkinson, Ed). W.H. Freeman and Co. San
Francisco. pp. 206-215.
Gooch, Stan (1980). The Double Helix of The Mind. Wildwood House Ltd. London.
Green, J.D. and Shimamoto, T. (1953) Hippocampal seizures and their propagation. Arch.
Neurol. Psychiat. 70: 687-702.
Guerit, J.M. and Opsomer, R.J. (1991). Bit-mapped imaging of somatosensory evoked
potentials after stimulation of the posterior tibial nerves and dorsal nerve of the
penis/clitoris. Electroenceph. & clin. Neurophysiol., 80: 228-237.
Guillery, R.W. (1959). Afferent fibers to the dorsomedial thalamic nucleus in the cat. J.
Lond. 93: 403-419.
Anat.
Heath, R.G. (1964). Pleasure response of human subjects to direct stimulation of the brain:
Physiological and psychodynamic considerations. In: The Role of Pleasure In Behavior (Heath,
R.G., Ed). Harper and Row, New York.
Heath, R.G. (1972a). Electroencephalographic studies in isolation raised monkeys with
behavioral impairment. Diseases of the Nervous Systems, 33, 157-163
Heath. R.G. (1972b). Physiologic basis of emotional expression: Evoked potential and
mirror focus studies in rhesus monkeys. Biological Psychiatry, 5: 15-31.
Heath, R. G. (l975): Maternal-social deprivation and abnormal brain development:
21
Disorders
of emotional and social behavior. In Brain Function and
Malnutrition:
:Neuropsychological
Methods of Assessment (Prescott, J.W.,
Read, M.S., &Coursin, D.B., Eds). John Wiley New
York.
Higley, J.D., Suomi, S.J., Linnoila, M. (1990). Parallels in Aggression and Serotonin:
Consideration of Development, Rearing History, and Sex Differences. In:
Violence
and Suicidality: Perspectives In Clinical and Psychobiological
Research (Herman van Praag,
Robert Plutchik and Alan Apter, Eds) NY:
Brummer/Mazel.
Higley, J.D., Hasert, M.F., Suomi, S.J. and Linnoila, M. (1991). Nonhuman primate
alcohol abuse: Effects of early experience, personality, and stress on alcohol
consumption.Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA V. 88, 7261-7265.
Iwata,
model
K. and Snider, R.S. (1959). Cerebello-hippocampal influences
electroencephalogram. Electroenceph. clin. Neurophysiol., 11: 439-446.
on
of
the
Jacobson, B.., Eklund, G., Hamberger, L., Linnarsson, D., Sedvall, G., and Valverius, M.
(l987): Perinatal origin of adult self-destructive behavior. Acta Psychiatrica
Scandinavia, 76, 364-371.
Jacobson, B., Nyberg, K., Eklund, G. Bygdeman, M., and Rydberg, U. (1988). Obstetric
Pain
Medication and Eventual Adult Amphetamine Addiction in Offspring. Acta
Obstet
Gynecol
Scand 67: 677-682.
Jacobson, B., Nyberg, K., Gronbladh, L., Edlund, G., Bygdeman, Rydberg, U. (1990). Opiate
addiction in adult offspring through possible imprinting after obstetric
treatment.
British
Medical Journal, Vol 301. pp. 1067-1070.
Kennard, M.A. (1945). Focal autonomic representation in the cortex and its relation to
sham rage. J. Neuropath. Exp. Neurol. 4: 295-304.
Kennard, M.A. and Ectors, L. (1938). Forced circling in monkeys following lesions of the
lobes. J. Neurophysiol. I: 45-54.
frontal
Kennard, M.A., Spencer, S. and Fountain, G. (1941). Hyperactivity in monkeys following
of the frontal lobes. J. Neurophysiol. 4:512-524.
lesions
Kluver, H. and Bucy, P.C. (1939). Preliminary analysis of functions of the temporal
monkeys. Arch. Neurol. Psychiat., 42: 979-1000.
lobe
in
Laudenslager, M.L., Reite, M., & Harbeck, R.J. (1982). Suppressed Immune Response
in
Infant Monkeys Associated with Maternal Separation. Behavioral and Neural Biology, 36, 4048.
Laudenslager, M.L. & Reite, J.L.,
(1984). Losses and Separations: Immunological
Consequences and Health Implications. In:
Review of Personality and Social
Psychology: 5. Emotions, Relationships and Health. Sage Publications. Beverly
Hills.
Laudenslager, M., Capitanio, J.P., & Reite, M. (1985). Possible Effects of Early
Separation
Experiences on Subsequent Immune Function in Adult Macaque Monkeys. Am. J. Psychiatry
142:7 July.
Levy, D. M. (l945). Psychic Trauma Of Operations In Children. American Journal of
Of Children (January) 69, (1), 7-25.
22
Diseases
MacLean, P.D. (1962). New findings relevant to the evolution of psychosexual functions
the brain. The J. Nervous and Mental Disease. V. 135, No.4 pp. 289-30.
of
MacLean, P. D. and Ploog, D. (1962). Cerebral representation of penile erection. J.
Neurophyssiol., 25. pp. 29-55.
MacLean, P.D., Denniston, R.H. and Dua, S. (1963). Further studies on cerebral
representation of penile erection: caudal thalamus, midbrain, and pons. J.
Neurophyysiol., 26, pp. 273-293.
MacLean, P.D., Dua, S. and Denniston, R.H. (1963). Cerebral localization for scratching
seminal discharge. Archives of Neurology, Vol. 9, pp. 485-497.
and
MacLean, P.D. (1978). Effects of lesions of globus pallidus on species-typical display behavior of
squirrel monkeys. Brain Research, 149, pp. 175-196.
MacLean, P.D.(1990). The Triune Brain in Evolution: Role in Paleocerebral Functions.
Plenum Press, New York.
Marshall, R.E., Stratton, W.C. ,Moore, J.A., , & Boxeman, S.B.
I.Effects upon newborn behavior. Infant Behav Dev. 3: 1-14.
(1980). Circumcision.
Marshall, R.E., Porter, F.L., Rogers, A.G., Moore, J.A., Anderson, B., & Boxeman, S.B.
(1982). Circumcision. II. Effects upon mother-infant interaction. Early Human Dev 7:36774.
Mason, W.A. and Berkson, G. (1975). Effects of Maternal Mobility on the Development
Rocking and Other Behaviors in Rhesus Monkeys: A Study with Artificial
Mothers.
Developmental Psychobiology, , 8, 197-221.
of
Nauta, W.J.H. (1960). Anatomical relationships between the amygdaloid complex, the
dorso-medial thalamic nucleus and the orbitro-frontal cortex in monkey (abstract). Anat.
Rec., 136: 251.
Melzack, R. and Burns, S.K.(1965). Neurophysiological effects of early sensory
Exp. Neurol., 13: 163-175.
restriction.
Mitchell, G.D. (1968). Persistent behavior pathology in rhesus monkeys following
isolation. Folia Primat., 8, 132-147.
early social
Nimi, K., Fujiwara, N., Takimoto, T. and Matsugi, S. (1962).The course and termination
the ascending fibers of the brachium conjunctivum in the cat as studied by the Nauta
Tokushima J. Exper. Med. 8: 269-284.
Moh., P.C., Adams, R., Greer, D.M., & Sheley, K.A. (1981). Prepuce Restoration
Psychiatric Aspects. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 10(4): 383-393.
Osofsky, J.D. (1976). Neonatal characteristics and
observational situations. Child Dev. 47: 1138-47.
mother-infant
Seekers:
interaction
Peters, D.A. (1990). Maternal stress increases fetal brain and neonatal cerebral cortex
hydroxytryptamine synthesis in rats: a possible mechanism by which stress
influences
development. Pharmacology, Biochemistry & Behavior. April
35(4): 943-7.
23
of
method.
in
two
5
brain
Phoenix, C.H., Goy, R.W. & Resko, J.A. (1968). Psychosexual differentiation as a
function of
androgenic stimulation. In: Perspectives in Reproduction and Sexual
Behavior. (Diamond, M.,
Ed). Indiana University Press. Bloomington.
Porter, F.L., Miller, R.H. and Marshall, R.E. (1986) Neonatal Pain Cries: Effect of
Circumcision on Acoustic Features and Perceived Urgency. Child Development
57,
790-802.
Prescott, J.W. (1971a). Early Somatosensory Deprivation As An Ontogenetic Process
In The Abnormal Development of The Brain and Behavior. In I.E. Goldsmith and
Moor-Jankowski (Eds): Medical Primatology 1970. Basel: Karger
Prescott, J.W. (1971b). Sensory deprivation vs. sensory stimulation during
development: A comment on Berkowitz's study. The Journal of Psychology
77: 189-191.
early
Prescott, J.W. (1972a). Cannon's law of denervation supersensitivity: Implications for
psychophysiological assessment. Psychophysiology. 9: 279.
Prescott, J.W. (1972b). Before ethics and morality. The Humanist Nov/Dec.
Prescott, J.W. (l975a): Body Pleasure and The Origins of Violence. The Futurist.
The Bulletin of The Atomic Scientists November 1975
April l975; &
Prescott, J.W. (1975b). Developmental Neuropsychophysics. In: Malnutrition and
Brain
Function: Neuropsychological Methods of Assessment. pp. 325-358.
(Prescott, J.W., Read,
M.S. & Coursin, D.B., Eds). New York John Wiley.
Prescott, J.W. (1976). Somatosensory deprivation and its relationship to the blind. In: The Effects
of Blindness and Other Impairments on Early Development. (pp. 65121)
(Jastrzembska,
Z.S., ed). New York: American Foundation For The Blind
Prescott, J.W.
(1977). Phylogenetic and ontogenetic aspects of human affectional
development. In: Selected Proceedings of the 1976 International Congress of Sexology.
(pp. 521-534). ( Gemme. R. & Wheeler, C.C. , eds). New York: Plenum.
Prescott, J.W. (1979). Alienation of Affection. Psychology Today. (Dec).
Prescott, J.W. (l979): Deprivation of physical affection as a primary process
in the development of physical violence. In. Child Abuse and Violence
(Gil, D. G., Ed). AMS Press New York pp 66-137.
Prescott, J.W. (1980) Somatosensory Affectional Deprivation (SAD) Theory of Drug and Alcohol
Use. In: Theories On Drug Abuse (Lettieri, D.J., Sayers, M. and Pearson, H.W., Eds) NIDA
Research Monograph 30. National Institute on Drug Abuse, Rockville, MD, pp.286-296.
Prescott, J.W. (1983). Invited Address: The Quadrune Brain: Cerebellar Regulation of
Emotional Behaviors. European Seminar on Developmental Neurology. Institute fur
Kindesentwicklung, Gmbh. Hamburg, Germany February 14-17, 1983.
Prescott, J.W. (1989). Genital Pain vs. Genital Pleasure: Why The One And Not The Other. The
Truth Seeker July/August 1989
24
Prescott, J.W. (1989) Failure of Pleasure As A Cause of Drug Abuse.
The Truth Seeker (Nov/Dec)
Prescott, J.W. (l990): Affectional bonding for the prevention of violent behaviors:
Neurobiological, Psychological and Religious/Spiritual Determinants. In. Violent Behavior
Vol. I: Assessment and Intervention. (L.J. Hertzberg, et. al., Eds). PMA Publishing NY pp. 110142.
Reite,
M., Seiler, C., Crowley, T.J., Hydinger-MacDonald,
Chronobiologia Vol IX No.1. January-March
M.
&
Short,
R.
(1982).
Reite, M. & Capitanio, J.P. (1985). On the nature of social separation and social
attachment.
In: The Psychobiology of Attachment ( Reite, M. & Field, T., Eds) Academic Press, NY.
Riesen, A.H., Dickerson, G.P. and Struble, R.G. (1977). Somatosensory Restriction and
Behavioral Development in Stumptail Monkeys. Annals New York Academy of Science, ,
285-294.
290,
Rose, J.E. and Woolsey, C.N. (1948). The orbitofrontal cortex and its connections with
the
mediodorsal nucleus in rabbit, sheep and cat. Res. Publ. Ass. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 27: 210-232.
Ruch, T.C. and Shenkin, H.A. (1943). The relation of area 13 on orbital surface of
lobes to hypractivity and hyperphagia in monkeys. J. Neurophysiol. 6:
349-360.
frontal
Saltzberg, B. Lustick, L.S. and Heath, R.G. (1971). Detection of Focal Depth Spiking in the Scalp
EEG of Monkeys. Electroencephalography and Clinical
Neurophysiology. , 31, 327-333
Struble, R.G. and Riesen, A.H. (1978) Changes in Cortical Dendritic Branching
Subsequent
to Partial Social Isolation in Stumptail Monkeys. Developmental Psychobiology, 11(5): 479-486
Salk, L., Lipsitt, L.P., Sturner, W.Q., Reilly, B.M., & Levat, R.H. (l985): Relationship of maternal
and perinatal conditions to eventual adolescent
suicide. The Lancet. March 16, l985.
Sharpless, S.K. Isolated and deafferented neurons: Disuse supersensitivity (1969). In:
Basic
Mechanisms of the Epilepsies (Jasper, Ward & Pope (Eds). Little
Brown & Co., NY 329355.
Sharpless, S.K. Disuse supersensitivity (1975). In: The Developmental
Sensory Deprivation (Riesen, A.H., Ed). Academic Press, NY
Neuropsychology
of
Snider, R.S. and Maiti, A. (1976). Cerebellar contributions to the Papez circuit.
J. of Neuroscience Research. 2, 133-146
Tassinari, C.A. (1968). Suppression of focal spikes by somatosensory stimuli. Electroenceph.
and Clin. Neurophysiol. 25, 574-578.
Textor, R.B. (l967): A Cross-Cultural Summary. Human Relations Area Files
(HRAF) Press. New Haven.
Torgersen, J. (1954). The occiput, posterior cranial fossa and the cerebellum. In:
Aspects of
Cerebellar Anatomy (Jansen, J. & Brodal, A., Ed). Johan Grundt Tanum Forlag. Oslo, Norway.
25
26
Download