Staub, E. (2006). Reconciliation after genocide, mass killing or intractable conflict: understanding the roots of violence, psychological recovery and steps toward a general theory. Political Psychology, 27,(6), 867-895. Reconciliation after genocide, mass killing or intractable conflict: understanding the roots of violence, psychological recovery and steps toward a general theory. Ervin Staub University of Massachusetts at Amherst Running Head: Reconciliation after genocide Ervin Staub’s address: Department of Psychology, Tobin Hall, University of Massachusetts, Amherst MA 01003 e-mail: estaub@psych.umass.edu 1 Abstract This article explores psychological avenues to reconciliation between groups. It describes the psychological changes in survivors, perpetrators and passive bystanders in the course of the evolution of increasing violence and points to healing from the psychological wounds created as an essential component of reconciliation. It also explores the role of understanding the roots of genocide, and of violence between groups in general, in contributing to healing, to the creation of a shared history in place of the usually contradictory histories held by groups that have been in violent conflict, and to reconciliation in general. The role of processes that have been emphasized in the literature on reconciliation, such as truth, justice, and contact between groups are discussed. Bottom up approaches focusing on the population and top down approaches involving leaders and the media, and the importance of changes in institutions and structures are discussed. The article exemplifies many of the issues and processes by a discussion of the genocide in Rwanda, and by the description of interventions, ranging from work with small groups, including leaders and the media, to radio programs that aimed to further reconciliation, as well as research evaluating an intervention. For indexing purposes: genocide healing/psychological recovery Reconciliation Understanding the roots of genocide Prevention Mass violence 2 Reconciliation after genocide, mass killing or intractable conflict: understanding the roots of violence, psychological recovery and steps toward a general theory. Ervin Staub University of Massachusetts at Amherst When violence between groups comes to a halt the building of constructive, nonviolent relations between groups becomes possible. However, violence often resumes (Long & Brecke, 2003). The psychological wounds that have resulted from the violence and fear, mistrust or hate may interfere with building better relations and lead to renewed violence (de la Rey, 2001). Violence may resume even if it was halted by negotiations and agreements between parties (de Silva & Samarasinghe, 1993). This is the case because agreements are often not satisfying to all segments of the hostile groups, because they may not be appropriately carried out, and by themselves they do not sufficiently alter the psychological realities of group members created by past history. This applies, for example, to the Oslo agreements between Israelis and Palestinians, which for a while diminished violence (McCauley, 2002, a, b). “The peace accord is at best a rough blueprint… Whether lasting peace will be constructed depends on what happens next” (Boyce, 2002, p.8). Renewed violence is probably an even greater danger when extreme violence is stopped by one party defeating the other, as in the case of Rwanda. One aim of this article is to note some of the psychological effects of group violence on the parties to the violence and thereby identify their psychological situation when reconciliation might start. Another is to describe an approach that my associates and I have developed and used in Rwanda to promote healing--or psychological recovery--and reconciliation, research we have conducted to evaluate its effectiveness (Staub, Pearlman, Gubin and Hagengimana, 2005), and varied uses of the approach in Rwanda (Staub and Pearlman, 2001; 2006; Staub, Pearlman, and 3 Miller, 2003). Reconciliation has special urgency for preventing new violence in situations, like Rwanda, where groups remain intermixed. We have used both bottom up approaches to reconciliation, which attempt to promote changes in the population, and top down approaches working with the media and leaders who can shape the attitudes of the community as well as the nature of institutions that may further reconciliation. I will also discuss the political and social context of these interventions. Another aim is to emphasize the value and potential uses of one of the elements of our approach, promoting understanding of the roots of violence. A final purpose is to briefly describe other processes that the literature indicates are important for reconciliation, to note relationships with elements of our approach, when they exist, and to initiate steps toward a general conception of reconciliation. What is reconciliation. Reconciliation may be defined as mutual acceptance by groups of each other (Staub and Pearlman, 2001; Staub and Bar-Tal, 2003). The essence of reconciliation is a changed psychological orientation toward the other. Reconciliation means that victims and perpetrators, or members of hostile groups, do not see the past as defining the future, as simply a continuation of the past. It means that they come to see the humanity of one another, accept each other, and see the possibility of a constructive relationship. This definition is consistent with other definitions, which focus on restoring a damaged relationship, and on both the processes involved and the outcome (Broneus, 2003; de la Rey, 2001; Kriesberg, 1998b; Lederach, 1997). While the focus of this definition (and of the interventions described in the article) is psychological change, institutions and how they operate are important, both in promoting reconciliation, and in solidifying or maintaining psychological changes that can be subverted by political and social processes. Whether the media devalues or humanizes groups, how the justice 4 system or schools operate, the nature of leadership, structural justice or the situation of groups in society, are crucial in promoting or hindering reconciliation. However, when psychological interventions affect leaders or the media, they can bring about institutional change. For example, if journalists come to present issues between groups in a manner that promotes reconciliation rather than incite hostility, this can be regarded as change in the media as an institution. There is extensive interest in reconciliation, but as yet limited research (e.g. Gibson, 2002, 2004; Byrne, 2003, 2004; Staub et al, 2005). Much of the already substantial literature describes practical efforts, like truth and reconciliation commissions, or interventions by practitioners, or focuses on theory (e.g. Abu-Nimer, 2001; Lederach, 1997; Proceedings, 2002; Volkan, 1997). However, reconciliation is also an aspect of the prevention of violence and peacemaking and can be facilitated by processes such as contact (Allport, 1954; Pettigrew and Tropp, in press), peace education (Salomon and Nevo, 2002), dialogue and interactive problem solving (Kelman, 1990; Kelman and Fisher, 2003; Rouhana and Kelman 1994; Ross and Rothman, 1999). A brief history of the genocide and its aftermath in Rwanda. Starting on April 7, 1994, the day after snipers shot down the plane carrying the President of Rwanda, over a 100 day period Hutus killed about 700,000 Tutsis, as well as about 50,000 politically moderate Hutus. Estimates of the number of people killed vary, and of how many people were killed in each group, but this is a relatively common estimate (des Forges, 1999; Mamdani, 2002; Prunier, 1995). The killings were accompanied by rape, the mutilation of women, and other forms of physical and psychological violence and torture. The perpetrators in this government-organized violence included parts of the military, young men in paramilitary groups, and ordinary people. This was an “intimate genocide” (Staub and Pearlman, 2001), 5 neighbors killing neighbors, and people in mixed families killing family members and occasionally even their own children (des Forges, 1999; Mamdani, 2002). The genocide evolved out of a past history of conflict and violence. A long history of dominance by the minority Tutsis (about 14% of the population) over the majority Hutus (about 85%) greatly intensified under the colonial rule of the Belgians. The Hutus and Tutsis in Rwanda live next to each other and share the same language and religion, primarily Catholic. They may have been originally different ethnic groups, but the primary differences over time became those of occupation, class, power and social identities. The Tutsis tended cattle, were wealthier and had power, the Hutus were agriculturalists (des Forges, 1999; Mamdani, 2002). Starting with a so-called white paper in 1916, the Belgians developed a racist ideology of Tutsi superiority (Mamdani, 2002) and used the Tutsis to govern the country. The condition of the Hutus progressively deteriorated. In 1959 a Hutu rebellion, in which about 50,000 Tutsis were killed, brought Hutus to power. After independence from Belgian rule in 1962, there was Hutu violence and discrimination against Tutsis, including mass killings in the early 1960s and 1970s. The government did not allow refugees from this violence to return. In 1990, the Rwandan Patriotic Army (RPA) entered the country from Uganda, composed mainly of children of Tutsi refugees. A civil war began, with a cease fire in 1993, followed by the Arusha Accords that delineated plans for a government shared by Tutsis and Hutus. While there was greater political openness and new political parties were created, among the Hutus an ideology of "Hutu power" developed and was propagated by elements of the government and media, intensifying fear and devaluation of Tutsis. The Tutsis were going to kill everyone, they will take away people’s property, they will rule over Hutus as before. A "Hutu Ten Commandments" propagated violent action against all Tutsis. The genocide began, and with 6 the world watching but remaining passive it continued until the RPA defeated the government army (des Forges, 1999; Prunier, 1995; Staub, 1999a). Since then, the power has been in the hands of the minority Tutsis. In the years immediately following the genocide, there were incursions by perpetrators, now refugees in the Congo, killing more Tutsis in the Northwest. With many Tutsis who were refugees in neighboring countries returning, the distribution of the population is about the same as before the genocide. Members of the groups live next to each other as neighbors, as before. The government initiated a policy of unity and reconciliation, a rare, intentional societal process to help people heal and reconcile. However, as part of unity, people are expected to consider themselves Rwandans, and not use the terms Hutu and Tutsi. Discussing differences between the groups has been called “divisionism.” This policy does now allow Hutus to express their concerns and identity. In the former Yugoslavia, where the government, led by Tito, did not allow an exploration of the mass killing of Serbs by Croats that took place during the second World War, the inattention to deep psychological wounds and past group relations is likely to have contributed to the violence during the 1990s (Staub, 1996). While in Rwanda there is a justice process (see below), this policy is a source of potential conflict. Over the years the government in Rwanda has become increasingly intolerant of “divisionism,” at times applying the term, and also the more extreme accusation of propagating genocidal ideology, to individuals and groups that it sees as potential opposition. Some of these individuals have been prosecuted and some groups have been outlawed, including the primary opposition party at the time of the 2003 presidential elections. Such societal processes affect reconciliation (Uvin, 2003). However, in Rwanda there are also continuing positive processes, such as equality in admission to education, and activities that promote reconciliation. In some of 7 our work we have collaborated with the Unity and Reconciliation Commission, a government organization. And it was at the invitation of high level government officials to expand the scope of our reconciliation efforts that we began to develop the radio programs described below, which among other things advocate opposition to leaders who promote policies of devaluation and hostility, active bystandership and pluralism. Inconsistent and at times contradictory policies and practices are likely to be present in many post-conflict situations, due to the influence of complex circumstances and psychological forces. The impact of mass violence. The evolution of violence. Understanding the psychological situation of survivors, perpetrators and bystanders—witnesses who are part of the perpetrator group but have not been directly involved in violence—is important for reconciliation. This psychological situation is not only the result of the violence, but also of what happens in the course of its evolution. The effects on survivors. Whether it is the outcome of a conflict that has become intractable, or the result of difficult life conditions and psychological and social processes that follow from them, mass killing and genocide are the end points of an evolution of harmdoing (see Staub, 1989b, 2003). In the course of this evolution the victims are increasingly devalued, are identified as enemies of an ideology that perpetrators create, and are harmed in varied ways. They tend to feel abandoned, and betrayed by former neighbors, friends, lovers and in mixed families even by family members who break off relations with them, as in Germany during the Nazi era (Staub, 1989b), or engage in violence against them, as in Rwanda (des Forges, 1999; Mamdani, 2002). The survivors carry the impact of both the increasing persecution and violence and of the mass killing or genocide. Their basic needs for security, for feelings of effectiveness and control over important events in one’s life, for positive identity, for positive connections to 8 other people and communities, and for a comprehension of reality and of one’s own place in the world, have all been deeply frustrated (Staub, 1989b, 2003; Staub and Pearlman, 2001; 2005). Victimized people tend to suffer from both PTSD and complex trauma (Herman, 1992; McCann and Pearlman, 1990). They feel diminished and vulnerable, seeing the world, other people, and especially members of groups other than their own as dangerous. Without healing, when there is new group conflict, it will be difficult for them to consider the needs of the other and to trust the other, and thereby to resolve conflict peacefully. In response to threat they may strike out, believing that they need to defend themselves, but actually becoming perpetrators. Healing, or psychological recovery, can improve the quality of their lives, make unnecessary “defensive” violence by them less likely, and help them become open enough to the perpetrator group to engage with processes of reconciliation (Staub, 1998; Staub and Pearlman, 2005). The effects on perpetrators and passive bystanders. Usually a group turns against an already devalued group, or is unable to resolve conflict because of its negative view and mistrust of the other group. In the course of harmful actions, individual actors and the group as a whole change. They learn by doing. They justify their actions by increasingly devaluing the victims, and excluding them from the moral realm (Fein, 1979, 2003; Opotaw, 1990; Staub, 1989b,1990). In the end there may be a reversal of morality; killing the victims becomes the right thing to do (Staub, 1989b). New institutions are created that serve discrimination and violence. In intractable conflict group beliefs evolve about the group’s just cause and the enemy’s responsibility for the conflict and violence (Bar-Tal, 2000; Staub and Bar-Tal, 2003). Participation in mass violence also creates trauma. People who have killed, including soldiers, are traumatized by their actions (Laufer, Brett, & Gallops, 1985; McNair, 2002; Parson, 1984; Rhodes, Allen, Nowicki, & Cillesen, 2002). Vietnam veterans who engaged in atrocities, 9 in violence that is extreme, unnecessary and/or directed at civilians, were especially affected (McNair, 2002). Those who execute genocide routinely engage in atrocities. Group support, ideology and changes due to learning by doing serve to some extent as protective mechanisms. But as they protect from trauma, guilt and shame, they diminish empathy for victims (Browning, 1992). The restriction of empathy tends to generalize, and violence often expands beyond the original victim group (Staub, 1998b; des Forges, 1999). After the violence is stopped, perpetrators tend to maintain the rightness of their actions and continue to devalue and blame the victims. They surround themselves with a defensive shield (Staub and Pearlman, 2006). Although once a genocide starts some people endanger themselves to save lives (Africa Rights, 2002; Oliner and Oliner, 1988), passivity by witnesses/ bystanders in the course of the evolution of harmdoing is the norm (Staub, 1989b). The psychological effects of passivity are similar to but less intense than the effects of perpetration. To reduce their own distress, passive members of the perpetrator group tend to distance themselves from victims, in part by accepting justifications offered by perpetrators, and by blaming and devaluing victims. This reduces empathy and inhibits guilt, perhaps with the help of the commonly held belief in a just world, where innocent people do not suffer (Lerner, 1980). As bystanders change, at least some of them join the perpetrators (Lifton, 1986; Staub, 1989a,b). Perpetrators of even relatively minor harmdoing tend to minimize the harm they have caused (Baumeister, 1997). The profound changes in identity, values, views of themselves, views of the victims and probably of human beings in general makes it extremely difficult for perpetrators of and passive bystanders to mass violence to acknowledge, presumably to themselves as well as others, that their actions were wrong. Few perpetrators showed regret or expressed sympathy for their victims in testimonies before the Truth and Reconciliation 10 Commission in South Africa (Broneus, 2003; Byrne, 2003, 2004). Serbs, Croats and Muslims who participated by e-mail in the New York Times “conference” on the Internet on Bosnia: Uncertain Paths to Peace in 1995 did not acknowledge their own group’s contribution to the conflict and violence but always pointed at the other groups1. The inability to acknowledge harmdoing interferes not only with reconciliation but also with being reincluded in the moral community, which perpetrators presumably want (Nadler, 2003). Healing from the wounds created by one’s actions or the actions of one’s group may lighten their weight, and may enable people to begin to assume responsibility and undo the transformation--- in their orientation to the victims, to people in general, in their personality and values. The resulting greater openness to the other group may facilitate reconciliation. While the nature of psychological wounds of survivors, perpetrators and bystanders varies greatly, as does the moral meaning of their woundedness, healing by each group can facilitate reconciliation. Elements of an approach to reconciliation and its applications in Rwanda. Effective reconciliation requires engaging with and changes in a whole range of actors in a society, from members of the population whose psychological orientation is the core to reconciliation, to national leaders who can shape policies, practices and institutions. In this spirit, our “interventions” have come to include over time training of the stuff of NGOs that worked with groups in the community, seminars/workshops with community leaders, journalists, and national leaders (government ministers, the heads of commissions, members of the supreme court and advisors to the President), and the development of nationally broadcast radio programs that according to surveys conducted in the summer of 2005, 90 percent of the radio listening population regularly listens to. We also trained trainers in the approach and conducted research 1 The “conference” was organized by topics, each topic guided by a moderator, who introduced the topic, followed the 11 evaluating its effectiveness (Staub et al., 2005; Staub and Perlman, 2001, 2006). Both the vision and the possibility of such a wide-ranging engagement have evolved over time. Healing from past victimization. A number of scholars and practitioners have suggested that healing is important for reconciliation (Montville, 1993; Kriesberg, 1998b; Staub,1998; Staub and Pearlman, 2001, 2006). Galtung (2201) defines reconciliation as “the process of healing the traumas of both victims and perpetrators after violence, providing a closure of the bad relations” (p. 3). Healing strengthens the self, moderates the perception of the world as dangerous, and makes it more likely that positive changes in the other group are perceived. Trauma researchers and therapists, working with individuals, have suggested engagement with rather than avoidance of painful, traumatizing experiences as one process in healing (Herman, 1992; Pearlman and Saakvitne, 1995). When this happens under emotionally supportive conditions, so that people can learn that the danger present at the time of their traumatizing experiences is not present any more, it reduces the negative emotional force of painful memories. Another central process, since victimization creates mistrust and fear of other people, especially those outside the group, is reconnecting with and gaining new trust in people. Acknowledgment by others of the suffering of survivors also furthers healing (Gibson, 2004; Proceedings, 2002). In the course of healing people may come to experience empathy with themselves, which opens them to empathy with other people. They may also feel less vulnerable and more trusting. This makes the beginning of reconciliation possible. As reconciliation begins, creating further safety and trust, healing can progress. After group violence healing will ideally be a group process. First, a huge numbers of people have been affected—in Rwanda the whole population. Second, the violence was a group discussion and made occasional entries to guide it. I was the moderator of the discussion on “healing and reconciliation”. 12 process. Third, Rwanda in particular is a collectivist, community oriented society. To promote healing, in small groups (and through the radio), we provided information about the impact of traumatizing events on people. The purpose of this was to help people understand changes in themselves and in others around them as a natural, normal consequence of extreme and in their case horrendous events. In small groups, people also engaged with their experiences during the genocide, with empathic support from others. The radio programs provided community education, through the example of people in radio dramas as well as in informational programs, including people supporting each other through empathic listening. Large group approaches to healing include testimonials and commemoration. They offer the opportunity both for engagement with experience and reconnection. It seems important for such commemorations to focus not only on the pain and suffering, which can make wounds persist and make the past into a “chosen trauma” (Volkan, 1997), but also on hope, the possibilities of a better future. For example, a historical/psychological focus on the military defeat of Serbia by the Turks at Kosovo in 1389 seemed to reaffirm Serb victimization and the sense of the world as dangerous. A televised re-enactment of the battle of Kosovo in 1988 (Leatherman, et al., 1999), arranged by Milosovic at a time of increasing societal problems (Staub, 1996), may have added to the nationalism that resulted in the wars and mass killings in the former Yugoslavia. Understanding the origins of violence. Understanding the origins of violence appears to be an important tool in promoting reconciliation and preventing new violence. Prevention requires, in part, the opposite of the influences that lead to violence, such as humanization of the other, in place of devaluation (see Table 1 for influences that contribute to mass killing or genocide, as well as some of the opposite influences important in prevention). Understanding 13 what leads to violence can shape the emotional orientation of and practices by members of the community, including leaders. It can also promote healing. Understanding how social conditions and culture have shaped perpetrators may ease the anger and hostility by survivors. It may also ease the defensiveness of perpetrators. Meaning making is central to being human and in the course of successful therapy usually a story develops that integrates and creates meaning out of experiences of suffering (Herman, 1992; Pennebacker, 2000). This meaning may consist of understanding why a perpetrator acted as he or she did (O’Connell Higgins, 1994). Another important meaning may be to decide to act either to help others who have suffered or to prevent such suffering (Kestenberg and Kestenberg, 1988; Staub, 2005b; Valent, 1998). Basic human needs. An element of our approach has been education about basic human needs and their role in violence, trauma and healing. Maslow’s (1968) theory of human needs has inspired other theories. The frustration of universal human needs has been viewed as a source of intractable conflict (Burton, 1990; Kelman, 1990). The frustration of basic needs such as security, a feeling of effectiveness and control, a positive identity, positive connection to other people, autonomy, and comprehension of reality has been seen as an important contributor to the psychological and social processes that, in certain cultural contexts, can initiate processes leading to mass killing or genocide (Staub, 1989b; 1996; 2003). These needs are profoundly frustrated by victimization and violence, and they require some degree of fulfillment for healing to occur (McCann and Pearlman, 1990; Pearlman and Saakvitne, 1995). Their fulfillment facilitates the resolution of conflict, and helps to develop caring about others’ welfare (Staub, 2005b). An intervention and its experimental evaluation. In an intervention to promote healing and reconciliation in Rwanda, 35 individuals were trained, both Tutsi (about two thirds of the group) and Hutu, who worked for local organizations 14 which worked with groups in the community (Staub et al., 2005). The participants then used this training, integrated within the seminar with their prior approach, with groups in the community. In the training, there were brief lectures and extensive discussion. One topic was the impact of traumatizing events, like genocide. Another was avenues to healing. A third topic was understanding the origins of genocide, with the group applying this understanding to Rwanda . A fourth topic was basic human needs. In another element of the intervention participants first individually thought about and then shared their experiences during the genocide in small groups. While writing about painful experiences facilitates healing (Pennebacker, 2000), about 50% of the population, and thus 50% of the people who the people we trained would later work with, could not write. There was demonstration, rehearsal and discussion of empathic responding to others’ distress. Then, to be consistent with the procedure they would later need to use with community groups, participants were asked to think about their experiences. After that, in small groups participants talked about their painful experiences. There was a great deal of open and highly emotional sharing of what happened to participants during the genocide (Staub and Pearlman, 2001, 2006; Staub, et al., 2003; Staub et al., 2005). Trauma creates insecurity, mistrust and disconnection from people. While it was Tutsis, the survivors of the genocide, who talked about their painful experiences during the genocide, they did this in a mixed group, with Hutus present as empathic witnesses. This was likely to contribute to another important element of healing, reconnecting with other people. Reconnecting with members of the other group may be expected to contribute to healing by members of both groups. The effects of the training were evaluated not on the participants in the training, but on people in community groups they subsequently worked with (Staub et al.,2005). New groups 15 were created, led by some of the people we trained using their new “integrated approach” (integrated groups). Other newly created groups were led by facilitators from the same organizations who did not receive the training (traditional groups). These groups met for four weeks, twice a week, for two hours. In control groups community members did not receive treatment but were evaluated, using questionnaires, about the same time as participants in the treatment groups--before the treatment, immediately afterward and two months later. The participants in the integrated group showed a significant reduction in trauma symptoms from before the treatment to two months afterwards, both over time and in relation to the two other groups, which showed slight deterioration. (In following up on the findings of a significant analysis of covariance, the significance level of the mean difference between integrated and traditional groups was p. 012, between integrated and control p. 000, while the other two groups did not differ). They also showed a more positive orientation toward members of the other ethnic group—or more readiness to reconcile, as we also called it-- both over time and in relation to the traditional and control groups, which did not change on this dimension. (The difference between integrated and traditional was p.008; integrated and control p. 006). This measure indicated greater awareness of the complexity of the roots of violence, greater willingness to work together for a better future, and “conditional forgiveness,” that is, greater inclination to forgive members of the other group if they acknowledge what they have done and apologize (the latter applying more to Tutsis then to Hutus). Healing wounds due to membership in a perpetrator group. Perpetrators, and the usually much larger number of people in the perpetrator group who have been passive bystanders, who are central to reconciliation, also need to heal. Their main avenues to healing are also those described above. Their defense against guilt and shame may make it difficult for them to engage 16 with the painful effects of their own or their group’s actions. However, external pressures and their desire to justify themselves and regain a moral standing (Nadler, 2003) can motivate them to engage in potentially healing activities. For example, they may expose themselves to, engage with, and hear the stories of survivors, as did Hutus in the study reported above (Staub et al., 2005). Exposing perpetrators to the stories of survivors unrelated to people they themselves have harmed is an approach that has had some success with violent perpetrators in the U.S. Such experiences can facilitate empathy with the victims. Empathy for the perpetrators or for passive bystanders by other people which manages to circumvent their defensiveness (GobodoMadikezela, 2003), while it can be difficult to offer, may also be important for healing by them. Healing by the perpetrator group can be furthered by certain societal processes. For example, in Rwanda, about 50,000 Hutus were killed by the Hutu perpetrators, many of them because they were seen as potentially or actually opposing the genocide. During the genocide, some Hutus tried to save Tutsis, sometimes successfully (Africa Rights, 2002; Staub and Pearlman, 2001), at other times losing their lives in the process (des Forges, 1999). Commemorating the Hutus who opposed the genocide and acknowledging and celebrating those who attempted or actually did save lives shows that the group whose members perpetrated the genocide is not rejected as a whole. It acknowledges that the perpetrator group was not monolithic, providing a more complex and differentiated view of the group, both in its own eyes and in the eyes of the survivors. Extensive discussion in our seminars with leaders-- with Tutsi leaders originally resisting the idea of commemorating rescuers at that time (Staub and Pearlman, 2002)-- may have contributed, together with attention created by the publication of a book on rescuers (Africa Rights, 2002), to including them since 2003 in commemoration of the genocide. Understanding, reconciliation and prevention. 17 In the study described earlier, the various elements joined in contributing to healing (the reduction in trauma symptoms) and reconciliation (a more positive orientation toward the other group). The contribution of the different elements was not separable. However, there were informal indications that consideration of the psychological/cultural roots of genocide had significant psychological benefits. The frequently articulated view in Rwanda as the reason for the genocide has been bad leaders and ignorance by the people. In the project described above, participants were presented with a conception of the origins of genocide and mass killing (Staub, 1989b, 1996,1999a, 2003), with many examples of how this conception applies to instances around the world. They were told about instigating conditions such as difficult life conditions (economic problems, political turmoil, and so on), conflict between groups, war, and the frustration of basic needs these create. They were told that these frequently give rise to greater identification by people with a group, scapegoating, and positive visions of the future that become destructive ideologies by identifying enemies who supposedly stand in the way of creating that better future. These are psychological and social processes that groups of people turn to in their attempts to satisfy frustrated needs for security, effectiveness and control, positive identity, connection, and comprehension of a changing and difficult world. An evolution of harmdoing and violence may follow in the course of which individuals and the group change in ways that makes greater violence likely. Certain cultural characteristics make this process more likely, such as a history of devaluation of some group, overly strong respect for authority and the absence of pluralism, and a past history of victimization and woundedness. The passivity and often also complicity of internal and external bystanders allows the evolution of violence, the unfolding of “steps along a continuum of 18 destruction” (See Table 1). An analysis of the influences leading to the genocide in Rwanda indicates that the processes these concepts describe were present in Rwanda (Staub, 1999a). The participants in various seminars/workshops, after a presentation of a general conception of the origins of genocide, with examples from other places, were asked to apply the conception to Rwanda. With the exception of a group of high level government leaders in 2001, who were hampered by the ideology that they have created, that there are no Tutsis and Hutus only Rwandans, all groups readily did this and found all the elements present in Rwanda. The substantive but also emotional discussions, which combined intellectual understanding with the participants’ own experiences, seemed to lead to an experiential understanding of the roots of genocide (Staub, 2000). The leaders in the course of the discussion acknowledged the psychological and social differences between groups and agreed with our application of the conception to the Rwandan genocide (see Staub and Pearlman, 2002; 2006). As participants in the various seminars/workshops came to see the genocide they suffered as one of a number of genocides, the perpetrators’ actions as psychologically and culturally understandable, even though horrible, and genocide as not an incomprehensible evil but the outcome of understandable human processes even if extreme and terrible in their consequences, they seemed to feel humanized or ‘rehumanized.’ They expressed the newfound belief that they were not the victims of blind evil or God’s punishment and the hope that understanding the roots of violence, they can act to prevent it (Staub et al., 2005; Staub and Pearlman, 2005). The combination of research findings and observations in Rwanda suggest that understanding the roots of violence between groups can contribute to both healing and reconciliation. This seems true of survivors, as well as members of the perpetrator group whose 19 defensive processes might have eased as they came to understand the influences leading to their group’s actions and saw that others understood these influences as well. Insert Table 1 about here Understanding origins suggests practices for prevention. Devaluation suggests the importance of humanizing the other. The role of overly strong respect for authority suggests the importance of moderating authority, for example, by considering the merit of leaders’ actions, rather than their position. The powerful role of destructive ideologies suggests the importance of positive visions that are inclusive, that join all groups in building a better future. In working with national leaders in Rwanda, practices for prevention were identified (Table 1), and leaders examined policies they have recently created from the perspective of their potential to contribute to or prevent violence (Staub and Pearlman, 2002, 2005). Education through the media: radio programs The experiences and knowledge gained through engagement with participants in the seminars and through the research were used to develop, in collaboration with the Dutch NGO LaBenevolencija, two educational programs, using the radio, the primary media outlet in Rwanda. One of them, beginning to broadcast in May 2004, to continue through 2007, is a weekly radio drama, a story of two villages in conflict. Elements of the approach described above about the origins and prevention of violence, healing and reconciliation, are embedded in the story. A second, monthly program that began to broadcast in October 2004 is informational, the concepts of the approach communicated in a direct manner, with Rwandans elaborating on the concepts. To improve the quality of the programs there is continuous feedback from “listener” (focus) groups (40 groups of 10 people each in 10 settings around the country). Evaluation of the programs’ impact on the population indicates benefits in knowledge, attitude 20 and behavior in comparison to a treatment control group (Levy-Paluck, 2006). Following the same approach new programs will begin in Burundi and the Congo in 2006. Other elements required for reconciliation. Reconciliation must take places at various levels, the psychological, political, institutional/structural, and cultural. The contribution of each is crucial, but interconnected. Institutions of justice may not be created, and principles and rules of justice may not be evenly applied without psychological changes—in attitudes and feelings toward the “other.” Psychological changes will not occur or be maintained without justice and other institutions that serve reconciliation. These processes and levels are interlocking; progressive change in one requires changes in others. Their relative contribution is difficult to identify, but in future work the way they interrelate will be important to further specify (but see also conclusions). For a summary of important processes required for reconciliation see Table 2. Insert Table 2 about here. Truth and a shared history (collective memory). Establishing the truth seems an essential motive of victims/survivors. After the military dictatorships in South America, with their torture and killings, truth commissions emerged to investigate and describe what was done (e.g. Nunca Mas, 1986). South Africa created the Truth and Reconciliation commission on the assumption that truth is essential for reconciliation (Gibson, 2002, 2004; Tutu, 1999). The truthful description of harmful actions is an acknowledgment of the suffering of victims and is therefore healing to them. Truth that is empathic with the victims and indicates that the perpetrators’ actions are unacceptable also reaffirms the moral order (de la Rey, 2001). By expressing the community’s refusal to accept such actions, it can enhance feelings of safety. 21 The establishment of the truth makes it more difficult for the perpetrators to deny their actions or their responsibility for the harm they caused, and to claim that they themselves were the victims. After the Holocaust, the Nuremberg trials, using thousands of pages of documents and many films the Nazis created, showed the German people what was done in their name and with their participation. This was probably crucial in starting Germany on the road to democracy. The Holocaust was unusual in that violence was completely one sided. Even in a genocide, however, perpetrators and victims have different stories. In intractable conflict, usually each sides sees its cause and actions as just (Bar-Tal, 2000b, Staub and Bar-Tal, 2003). When Palestinian and Israeli high school students were asked to describe the conflict from the perspective of the other side, they were not able to do so (Salomon, 2003). In many instances of mass killing there is some harm done by both sides, if not at the time of the mass killing or genocide, then over a longer historical period. In Rwanda, the genocide was one-sided, and was preceded by repeated mass killings of Tutsis, by sporadic killings at other times and by discrimination and persecution (des Forges, 1999). However, Hutus refer to the period before 1959, when Tutsis were dominant in Rwanda, and especially the period during Belgian colonial (des Forges, 1999; Mamdani, 2001), which seems to have become a “chosen trauma” (Volkan, 1997, 1998) for them, as a period of servitude, even slavery. In addition, in the course of the civil war starting in 1990, Hutu civilians were killed. While after the genocide was stopped the government was successful, on the whole, in inhibiting revenge killings, in the course of the fighting in 1994 to end the genocide and defeat the government there were revenge killings (des Forges,1999). Afterwards, in the course of fighting infiltrators from the Congo who were killing Tutsis, Hutu civilians were killed. A large number of Hutu 22 refugees into Zaire were also killed in the course of the advance of Kabila’s army against Mobutu, helped by the Rwandan army (de Forges, 1999; Mamdani, 2002). Survivors of genocide understandably focus on the immediate truth, the perpetration of horrendous violence against them. Perpetrators and members of the perpetrator group tend to focus on an account of events that includes, and usually emphasizes, either the harm they had suffered or the threat to them by the other party. Consideration of injuries to both sides, even if substantially unequal, makes the development of a shared history, of shared collective memories possible (Cairns and Roe, 2002; Bar-Tal, 2002; Kriesberg, 1998b; Staub and Bar-Tal, 2003). When antagonistic groups have conflicting accounts of the roots of violence, their antagonism is likely to continue. But resolving differing views of a violent history is hard. Deeply wounded survivors may find it impossible to consider the injury their group has done to the other, which in their consciousness pales in significance. And members of the perpetrator group (Baumeister, 1997) may overemphasize their group’s suffering at the hands of the other and use it as justification for their actions. Establishing a shared collective memory can be the result of reexamination and reinterpretation of history, involving a process of negotiation. One example of such negotiation was the Franco-German commission of historians, which in the 1950s had critically scrutinized the myths of hereditary enmity between the French and German peoples and revised the existing history textbooks. As a final product the commission provided new accounts of the history of the two nations (Willis, 1965). However, such negotiations seem very difficult when wounds are fresh. Other processes, such as healing, may need to precede them. In the seminar in Rwanda with high level leaders participants strongly affirmed the importance of a shared collective memory, but regarded it as an ideal that may not be possible to 23 achieve in the short run (Staub and Pearlman, 2002, 2006). However, as already noted, Rwanda is engaged in societal processes that aim to promote reconciliation, with seminars, national conferences that are televised and on the radio, the gacaca proceedings (see below), the release of large numbers of prisoners in 2003/2004 and the promotion of reconciliation by national leaders. The ministry of education in Rwanda has made the introduction of peace education and reconciliation a top priority for schools, with ongoing projects on the teaching of history (Murenzi, 2004). While all this may make the creation of a shared history possible, openness to a shared history also requires societal processes that address the past in just manner, and political developments that create just relations between groups. To rework the past, people must have confidence in a shared future. (See Table 2 for processes limiting or promoting reconciliation). The role of understanding the origins of violence in creating a shared history in Rwanda. Establishing a shared collected memory may be greatly facilitated by understanding how both one’s own group’s and the other group’s harmful actions have come about. Such understanding can facilitate acceptance. In seminars in Rwanda in 2000 to 2003, with trauma workers, journalists, commissioners of the Unity and Reconciliation Commission, government leaders and others, we have experimented with creating a shared history by offering explanations of historical events rooted in our conception of the origins of genocide. These explanations were welcomed by and seemed useful for participants. As an example, we applied the concepts used in understanding the genocide at a societal level to individuals, in an attempt to provide understanding of young killers, part of the paramilitary groups, the Interahamwe, made up of young men. This took the following form: Starting in the 1980s, Rwanda was in a very bad economic situation, with many young men without jobs, without prospects. With the invasion of the RPF in 1990 a civil war has begun Relative suddenly there was substantial political activity, with new 24 political parties, an unusual situation for Rwanda that required adjustment to unfamiliar and for a very hierarchical society seemingly chaotic circumstances. The political parties started to create youth groups, apparently to gain young followers. Out of these youth groups grew the Interahamwe. At some point the parties began to give them military training, making them into militias. These young men had acquired some material security, a community, a sense of importance. Their needs for security, identity, connection, and comprehension of reality, previously presumably frustrated, were at least somewhat satisfied. In the U.S. young men in similar situations join gangs. These young men did not join to be killers. That evolved later. However, they have learned for many years to devalue Tutsis. Added to this were recent events that had to strongly affect them: the victories of the Tutsi led invaders; the intense, vicious propaganda against Tutsis on the radio and in newspapers, with constant reminders of Hutu “slavery” and with constant exposure to the ideology of Hutu power and to the Hutu “ten commandments” which demanded violent action against Tutsis; the killing of the democratically elected Hutu President in Burundi by segments of the Tutsi army which fed Hutu fears that the Tutsis would not share power with Hutus; all the authorities over them-from the leaders of the Interahamwe to higher authorities--telling them that violence against Tutsis was right; witnessing violence against Tutsis before the genocide that was in no way discouraged by authorities; the evolution and change in them as violence began; the development of a group value and spirit in killing; and the development of commitment to the group of young men they were part of. As it has been described in relation to terrorists, commitment to cause and comrades join each other. All of this makes it psychologically understandable what they did, horrible as it was, and that being part of the Interahamwe made it difficult not to do what they did. It is important not to take this understanding as an excuse for what they were doing, since people can make choices. But to prevent violence, to practically enable people to make different choices, it is essential to inhibit the development of the kind of societal system and process that came into being in Rwanda. The process of creating a shared history may need to combine a focus on the roots of events, with negotiation about both what has happened and their meaning--in seminars, dialogue 25 groups and other formats used in conflict resolution. When views about what has happened and why cannot be reconciled, a joint history ought to include a plurality of perspectives, varied statements about events and about their interpretation. This can provide an example of and foster pluralism, which is an important aspect of the creation of a non-violent society. When the history taught in schools and described in history books includes such varied elements, the groups that constitute that society are likely to be on their way to healing and reconciliation. Justice and reconciliation. Justice is another important need of survivors of genocide and mass killing. Justice is served, in part, by punishing perpetrators. Justice requires truth. It acknowledges the harm done to victims. It helps them heal, makes a moral statement about the perpetrators’ actions, and to some extent balances the harm, suffering and loss of status by victims (Berscheid, Boye & Walster, 1968; Staub, 2005a). It is sometimes assumed that punishment will discourage future perpetration. However, punishment usually focuses on a limited number of people. Moreover, genocide and mass killing are societal processes that have deep societal, cultural and psychological roots. Punishment alone, without addressing those roots, is unlikely to inhibit group violence. For the sake of building peace, in part to avoid creating new hostility in members of the perpetrator group, most post conflict societies punish only some (at times very few) of the perpetrators. Often this is less than what is needed to satisfy and help heal the survivors—or to change society in a constructive way. Punishment of at least important perpetrators, public discussion of the reasons to limit punishment, and the use of other forms of justice may together satisfy psychological needs and provide the psychological benefits required by the different parties to advance reconciliation. 26 Restorative justice refers to contribution by perpetrators to the lives of survivors and to rebuilding society. It may include monetary compensation, as well as work. Compensation for victims/survivors, if necessary by the state, however that is now constituted, helps them improve their economic situation devastated by violence. This has been viewed as important by theorists (Broneus, 2003) and been found important for the population in South Africa (Gibson, 2002). In Rwanda, at hearings held by the Unity and Reconciliation Commission in 1999 about what people require for reconciliation, survivors often mentioned improvement in their material conditions, devastated by the genocide. In the radio listening (focus) groups across Rwanda, in January 2004, many people said that their poverty reminds survivors of what they have lost in the genocide and intensifies trauma. Justice is a matter of both actions and the perceptions they create. Just societal procedures have been found especially important (Tyler and Smith, 1998). In an analysis of interviews of women who were raped during the genocide in Rwanda (Lillie, 2005), lower trauma symptoms were associated more with the perception of an effective justice system than with the personal experience of justice. For lasting reconciliation, just relations between groups must be created (Kelman, 1990). Past harm done and ways to balance it in the present need to be considered. However, like truth, this is a complex matter that goes beyond exact equality or equity. Community courts in Rwanda and the requirements of justice, healing and reconciliation. After intense violence between subgroups in a society, how can justice be promoted in ways that build peace? South Africa created the Truth and Reconciliation process, which in addition to establishing the truth also offered a balance of amnesty and punishment, and compensation for victims (Byrne, 2003, 2004). 27 In Rwanda, the justice system was destroyed in the genocide. To deal with the approximately 125 thousand prisoners accused of having perpetrated the genocide, people’s courts, the gacaca, have been created, based on an ancient community practice. 250 thousand people have been elected to serve as judges, in groups of 19, in 9-10 thousand locations around the country. It was hoped that these courts will further establish the truth, create justice, and that participation by everyone, which is required by the “gacaca law,” will bring people together as they work for the shared goal of a just and peaceful society (Honeyman et al., 2004). However, after the horrors of genocide or mass killing, probably all justice processes are unavoidably imperfect. In South Africa, these imperfections included perpetrators describing their acts without apparent regret or empathy for victims, testimony by survivors that retraumatized them, and the state delaying compensation for victims and planning to reduce the level of compensation from what was originally promised (Byrne, 2003, 2004). In Rwanda the people’s courts are creating further upheaval, as over a period of years people describe or are exposed to the descriptions of horrible acts. Both national leaders and community leaders have been concerned that these experiences will retraumatize survivors, and create new trauma, for example, in young children, or in relatives of women who describe rapes that other people did not know about. An additional concern is renewed anger among survivors, as well as new hostility among some members of the perpetrator group who may have been passive bystanders, but not perpetrators, and constantly hear about the terrible violence by their group, this implicating them as members of their group (Staub and Pearlman, 2002, 2006). To limit the harmful potential of the gacaca and promote its potential to advance reconciliation, in working with the media and community and national leaders we discussed how they can help members of the community protect each other from retraumatization, by providing 28 information about how people can support each other, for example, through empathic listening. We considered how understanding the roots of violence, for example, by describing how a young man would have become an Interahamwe killer, can help people cognitively process the origins of the violent actions they hear about in the gacaca. This can, in turn, limit survivors’ emotional reactions to what they hear. Such cognitive processing and the understanding it entails may also lessen the feeling by passive bystanders that they are being blamed and shamed. As a result, instead of feeling hostility, they may be able feel empathy for the victims. Forgiveness and reconciliation. There has been controversy about the importance of forgiveness in reconciliation (Hayes, 1998; Shriver, 1995). Forgiveness has been defined as a change from negative emotions and thoughts about the offender such as anger, resentment and the desire for revenge to more positive, benevolent ones (McCullough, Fincham and Tsang, 2003; Worthington, 1998). Research with individuals, such as rape victims or people harmed by relatives, friends or associates, suggests that forgiving can relieve psychological distress (Worthington, 1998). With regard to genocide, an important question is what is of primary benefit for healing and reconciliation, forgiving the perpetrators, or the bystanders—passive members of the perpetrator group—who are usually the substantial majority of that group. Forgiving and reconciliation are related, overlapping concepts. However, forgiveness is usually described as one sided, in contrast to reconciliation, which is mutual. In the case of intense, one-sided violence, forgiveness without appropriate actions by perpetrators or members of the perpetrator group can have harmful effects (Perlman, 2002; Staub, 2005a). Victimization creates wounds, as well as an imbalance in the relationship between victims and perpetrators. It diminishes the status of the former in relation to the latter, and also in relation to other, nonvictimized people. 29 Being forgiven may bring about positive change in some perpetrators. However, it does not require anything of them. As a result, it may limit acknowledgement and empathy by perpetrators, and maintain or enhance the imbalance between the groups (Staub, 2005a). In South Africa, as part of the TRC process (Byrne, 2003), survivors appeared to have been further injured by confessions by perpetrators without indications of regret. In Rwanda as well, some perpetrators confessed their actions in front of the community but did so without any expression of feelings, creating distress among community members. The need for reciprocity in the forgiveness process was shown in the study described above (Staub, et al. 2005), where participants expressed “conditional forgiveness,” contingent on the acknowledgment of harmdoing and apology (see also Staub, 2005a). This is consistent with past research which has shown that apology is important in promoting forgiveness(see Worthington, 1998). The more perpetrators acknowledge their actions and the harm they have created, assume responsibility, express regret and apologize, show empathy and concern for the pain and suffering of the victims, and offer reparation, whether it is monetary or compensatory actions, the more they help survivors feel safe, affirm their worth and balance the relationship. These processes all promote forgiveness. In turn, forgiveness can give rise to such processes and actions in members of a perpetrator group. Forgiving the other group seems more important for advancing reconciliation than forgiving the actual perpetrators. Members of the perpetrator group who have not participated in perpetration may do a great deal to facilitate reconciliation and forgiveness by acknowledging the harm done by their group and apologizing for its actions. Contact between groups and community building as avenues to reconciliation. The contact hypothesis (Allport, 1954; Sherif et al.,1961; Pettigrew and Tropp, 2006), stresses the 30 importance of contact between people belonging to different groups as a way to overcome prejudice and hostility. Thus, contact is an avenue to reconciliation. Contact and its potential benefits are a crucial element in the success of dialogue groups, problem solving workshops and other forms of conflict resolution (Kelman and Fisher, 2003; Rouhana and Kelman, 1994). Deep contact, genuine engagement, what Deutsch (1973) has called “cross-cutting relations,” must exist for contact to work. Joint activities, with shared, “superordinate” goals, facilitate such contact (Sherif et al., 1961). A number of other conditions, such as equality between the parties and support by authorities have been identified as important for effective contact (Allport, 1954; Pettigrew, 1997; 1998). In dialogue groups, in the course of contact, as progressively their pain and suffering is heard and acknowledged, participants can acknowledge more their own group’s harmful actions. The “authorities” present, usually neutral third parties who facilitate the engagement, support these processes. There is also self-selection: people with intense negative views and closed minds about the other party are unlikely to participate. Equality between the parties can be strived for, even if in the outside world there is difference in power. However, such differences in the larger society, especially if they are unacknowledged, make the process more difficult. For example, with the minority Tutsis holding power in Rwanda and with the ideology of “unity,” Hutus may feel that they not only lack power but are also deprived of an identity and a voice. Structural arrangements in a society that create both opportunities for, and the type of contacts that promote positive relations are helpful. Schools can promote reconciliation by having children from different groups in the same classrooms, with cooperative learning procedures (Aronson et al., 1978) and other practices (Staub, 2003) that create positive contact. Individuals from different groups (as well as nations) can be part of institutions that create 31 contact and serve shared interests. In India, in three cities where significant Hindu-Muslims violence did not occur in response to instigating conditions, there were institutions that included members of both groups, who organized themselves to combat rumors as they arose and to engage with politicians and inhibit them from making speeches that would increase tensions. In three cities where instigating conditions led to violence no such institutions existed (Varshney, 2002). Reconciliation between the French and Germans was facilitated by contact between cities, and the creation of institutions after World War II the countries were part of, for example, the European Coal and Steel Community (ECSC) (Kriesberg, 1998). People joining together to rebuild communities after great violence is another avenue to reconciliation. Such efforts can enhance security, feelings of effectiveness, develop positive identity and connection, and provide a new and more positive comprehension of reality. They can also fulfill the need for transcendence (Staub, 2003), as individuals focus on and work for something beyond and greater than themselves. In Angola and other places, members of communities have developed ceremonies, combining tradition with psycho-social principles, to reintegrate child soldiers into the community (Wessells and Monteiro, 2001). The children were enticed to be soldiers with food or other incentives, or were abducted. As soldiers, they sometimes killed people in their own communities. As communities engaged in reintegration, they were also rebuilding the community and fostering healing, reconciliation and forgiveness. Raising inclusively caring children. Finally, raising children in ways that makes it likely that as adults they will not devalue others, and will be active bystanders in the face of potentially destructive social processes, is important for the prevention of violence and reconciliation (Table 2; Staub, 2003, 2005b). Creating the societal and cultural conditions and processes important for the prevention of violence between groups and for reconciliation, as discussed in this article and 32 presented in Tables 1 and 2, makes it more likely that socializers will raise children in ways that promotes these characteristics. Raising children in such ways will in turn help further develop these societal characteristics and processes. Cultural specificity and sensitivity A number of authors point to the importance of sensitivity to culture (Broneus, 2003; Lederach, 1998) as well as to the specific characteristics of the situation (Leatherman, DeMars, Gaffney, & Vayrynen, 1999) in attempting to promote reconciliation. In this article, the importance of establishing general principles has been implicit, if not explicit all along. However, general principles need to be applied and modified as appropriate depending on the history of group relations, culture, the role of bystanders. The concept of reconciliation, and especially of forgiveness, which have religious sources, may find more resonance in a religious, predominantly Catholic society like Rwanda, than in less religious societies (see Staub, 2005a). While justice seems universally important, cultural characteristics and practices shape what will satisfy people’s need for justice. A justice process like the gacaca may be more difficult to introduce in a society without the cultural tradition in which the practice is rooted. Theorists, researchers and practitioners must consider both universal principles and the specific, and at times unique elements of each situation. Conclusions:Psychology, social structure, politics and institutions. Efforts to promote reconciliation need to focus on at least three domains. First, on changing attitudes by the people, as we have attempted to do by training facilitators who work with groups in the community and through radio programs. Second, on changing the words and actions of those who can both influence people, and affect societal processes and the nature of institutions, which was an aim of our work with leaders and journalists. Third, on creating and 33 changing societal institutions, such as schools, the justice system, the political system, and NGOs, in part by working with leaders as we did, but also by directly working with those institutions. Psychology has an important role in all these. While there are often realistic constraints on creating positive structural conditions, such as established institutions and lack of societal resources, frequently psychological elements like fear and hostility play a crucial role. A societal process of reconciliation requires changes in structures, in societal institutions, policies and practices that can promote change in the attitudes of the population, as well as help maintain change. For example, reducing inequalities, establishing more equal opportunity in schools and employment for Catholics in Northern Ireland has helped prepare the ground for the peace process there (Cairns, E. & Darby, J. (1998). Our “interventions” aimed to promote psychological change, through healing, understanding, and the emotional changes and attitudes associated with these. However, psychological change and changes in policies and institutions are interconnected. To establish a fair system of justice requires a reasonably positive attitude toward a group that has been an enemy. The operation of such a system, in turn, can promote healing (Lillie, 2005) as well as positive attitudes (Gibson, 2004). Creating schools that promote reconciliation also requires already existing positive attitudes. Early in this article I noted the current policies of the Rwandan government with regard to “divisionism,” which deprives opposition in general and Hutus in particular of a voice, making reconciliation more difficult. The reason for such policies can be psychological woundedness and the resulting vulnerability, view of the world as dangerous and negative attitudes to the other group. It can also be realistic concern about the still hostile intentions of the other group, as well as the desire of those in power to retain their power. Working with leaders may help them see the 34 dangers in such policy, and as long as the realistic danger is not too great enable them to take the risks involved in allowing and promoting pluralism, guided, in part, by enlightened self-interest. Reconciliation is an extended process requiring a long term vision of desired changes. (The right side of Tables 1 and 2 incorporate such a vision). It needs to cut across levels, involving both leaders and the population, and create a “sense of participation, responsibility and ownership in the process across a broad spectrum of the population” (Lederach, 1998, p. 242). The example of South Africa, and of Europe, with a long history of violence and seemingly deep-seated antagonisms between countries, show that reconciliation is possible. Hopefully, reconciliation can be brought about even in such extremely demanding conditions as Rwanda, with its intimate genocide, with perpetrators, bystanders and survivors continuing to live together and with the country ruled by a small minority of the population that was the victim of the genocide. 35 Table 1: The origins and prevention of violence between groups A: Starting points: Difficult life conditions (economic, political, social change, war) Conflict between groups Consequences: The resulting frustration of basic psychological needs; Turning to a group for identity and support; Scapegoating; (Destructive) Ideologies B: History, culture, and current practices Devaluation of the Other Humanizing the Other Destructive, Exclusive Ideology Constructive, Inclusive Ideology Unhealed Wounds Healing of Past Wounds Uncritical Respect for Authority Moderate Respect for Authority Monolithic Society Pluralism (structures; processes) Unjust societal arrangements Just social arrangements Passive bystanders Active bystanders C. Continuous Processes: The evolution of harmdoing (changes in perpetrators, bystanders, institutions, social norms and culture). The role of leaders (The role of followers) 36 Table 2: Reconciliation and the prevention of new violence Lack of Truth Truth (complex: shared) Lack of Justice Justice Unhealed wounds Healing of past wounds Conflicting views of history Shared views of history/ Shared collective memories Lack of Contact, Superficial Contact Deep Contact, Shared Goals Raising children as obedient followers Raising inclusively caring children with moral courage Lack of understanding the roots of violence 37 Understanding and actions guided it References Abu-Nimer, M. (2001).(ed.). Reconciliation, justice and coexistence. New York: Lexington Books. Africa Rights (2002). Tribute to courage. Kigali, Rwanda: African Rights. Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Bar-Tal, D. (2000). Shared beliefs in a society: Social psychological analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Bar-Tal, D. (2002). Collective memory of physical violence: Its contribution to the culture of violence. In E. Cairns & M. D. Roe (Eds.) Memories in conflict. London: Macmillian. Baumeister, R.F.(1997). Evil: Inside human violence and cruelty. New York: Freeman and Co. Berscheid, E., Boye, D., & Walster, E. (1968). Retaliation as a means of restoring equity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 10, 370-376. Boyce, J. K. (2002) Investing in peace: Aid and conditionality after civil wars. Oxford: Oxford University Press, Adelphi Paper No. 351. Broneus, K. (2003). Reconciliation – Theory and Practice for Development Cooperation. Stockholm: Swedish International Development Cooperation Agency. Browning, C.R. (1992). Ordinary Men: Reserve Batallion 101 and the Final Solution in Poland. New York: Harper Collins. Burton, J. W. (1990). Conflict: Human needs theory. New York: St. Martin's Press. Byrne, C. (2003). Beyond accounts: Accounts in a human rights violation context. Unpublished manuscript. Solomon Asch Center for Study of Ethnopolitical Conflict. 38 Byrne, C. (2004). Benefit of Burden: Victims’ Reflections on TRC Participation. Peace and Conflict: Journal of Peace Psychology. 10(3), 237-256. Cairns, E. & Darby, J. (1998). Conflict in Northern Ireland. American Psychologist, 53, 754-76. Cairns, E. & Roe, M.D. (2002). Memories in conflict. London: Macmillan. De la Rey, C. (2001). Reconciliation in divided societies. In D.J. Christie, R.V, Wagner, & D.D. Winter (Eds.). Peace, conflict and violence. Upper Saddler River, New Jersey: Prentice Hall. Des Forges, A. (1999). Leave none to tell the story: Genocide in Rwanda. New York: Human Rights Watch. de Silva, K. M. & Samarasinghe, S. W. R. de A., (Eds). (1993). Peace accords and ethnic conflicts. London: Pointer Publishers. Deutsch, M. (1973). The resolution of conflict: Constructive and destructive processes. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Fein, H. (1979). Accounting for Genocide: Victims and survivors of the Holocaust. New York: Free Press. Fein, H. (1993). Accounting for Genocide after 1945: Theories and some findings. International Journal of Group Rights, 1, p. 79-106. Galtung, J. (2001). After violence, reconstruction, reconciliation and resolution: coping with visible and invisible effects of war and violence. In Abu-Nimer, M. (ed.). Reconciliation, justice and coexistence. New York: Lexington Books. Gibson, J. L. (2002). Truth, Justice, and Reconciliation: Judging the Fairness of Amnesty in South Africa. American Journal of Political Science, 46: (3): p. 540-556. 39 Gibson, J. L. (2004). Does Truth Lead to Reconciliation? Testing the Causal Assumptions of the South African Truth and Reconciliation Process. American Journal of Political Science. 48 (2l) Girard, P. (1980). Historical foundations of anti-semitism. In Dimsdale, J. (Ed.), Survivors, victims and perpetrators: Essays on the Nazi Holocaust. New York: Hemisphere Publishing Company. Gobodo-Madikezela, P. (2003). A Human Being Dies that Night: A South African Story of Forgiveness, Boston: Houghton-Mifflin. Hayes, G. (1998). We suffer our memories: Thinking about the past, healing, and reconciliation. American Imago, 55, 29-50. Herman, J. (1992). Trauma and recovery. New York: Basic Books. Honeyman, C., Hudami, S., Tiruneh, A., Hierta, J., Chirayath., L., Iliff, A. & Meierhenrich, J. (2004). Establishing collective norms: Potentials for participatory justice in Rwanda. Peace and Conflict: Journal of Peace Psychology, 10(1), 1-24. Kelman, H. C. (1990). Applying a human needs perspective to the practice of conflict resolution: The Israeli-Palestinian Case. From J. Burton (Ed.), Conflict: Human Needs Theory. New York: St. Martin's Press. Kelman, H. C. & Fisher, R. J., (2003). Conflict analysis and resolution. In D. Sears, L. Huddy, & R. Jervis, (Eds). Political Psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Kestenberg, M. & Kestenberg, J. S., (1988). The Sense of belonging and altruism in children who survived the holocaust. Psychoanalytic Review, 75 (4), p 532-560. Kriesberg, L. (1998a). Intractable conflicts. In E. Weiner (Ed.), The Handbook of interethnic coexistence. (p. 332-342). New York: Continuum. 40 Kriesberg, L. (1998b). Coexistence and the reconciliation of communal conflicts. In E. Weiner (Ed.), The Handbook of interethnic coexistence. (p. 182-198). New York: Continuum. Laufer, R.S., Brett, E., & Gallops, M.S. (1985). Symptom patterns associated with posttraumatic stress disorder among Vietnam veterans exposed to war trauma. American Journal of Psychiatry, 142, 1304-1311. Leatherman, J., DeMars, W., Gaffney, P.D., & Vayrynen, R. (1999). Breaking cycles of violence: Conflict prevention in intrastate cries. CT: Kumarian Press, Inc. Lederach, J.P. (1997). Building peace: Sustainable reconciliation in divided societies. Washington, D.C.: United States Institute of Peace Press. Lederach, J. P. (1998). Beyond violence: Building sustainable peace. In E. Weiner (Ed.). The handbook of interethnic coexistence (pp. 236-245). New York: Continuum. Lerner, M. (1980). The belief in a just world: A fundamental delusion. New York: Plenum Press. Levy-Paluck, E. (2006). Unpublished data. Ph.D. Dissertation in Progress, Yale University. Lifton, R.J. (1986). The Nazi Doctors. New York: Basic Books Lillie, C. (2005). Women survivors’ experience of justice in Rwanda. Poster Presentation at the Meetings of the International Society for Political Psychology, Toronto, July. Long, W. J. & Brecke, P. (2003). War and Reconciliation: Reason and Emotion in Conflict Resolution. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Mamdani, M. (2002). When Victims Become Killers. Princeton: Princeton University Press. Maslow, A. H. (1968). Toward a psychology of being (2nd ed.) New York: Van Nostrand. McCann, I. L., & Pearlman, L. A. (1990). Psychological trauma and the adult survivor: Theory, therapy, and transformation. New York: Brunner/Mazel. 41 McCauley, C. (2002a). Psychological issues in understanding terrorism and the response to terrorism. In C. Stout (Ed.), The Psychology of Terrorism, Volume III Theoretical Understandings and Perspectives, pp. 3-30. Westport, CN: Praeger. McCauley, C. (2002b). Understanding the 9/11 perpetrators: crazy, lost in hate, or martyred? In N. Matuszak (Ed.), History Behind the Headlines: The Origins of Ethnic Conflicts Worldwide, Volume 5, pp. 274-286. New York: Gale Publishing Group. McCullough, M., Finchman, F. D., and Tsang, J., (2003). Forgiveness, forbearance, and time: the temporal unfolding of transgression-related interpersonal motivations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. Vol. 84, (3), p 540-557. McNair, R. M., (2002). Perpetration-Induced Traumatic Stress. London: Praeger. Montville, J. V. (1993). The healing function in political conflict resolution. In D.J. D. Sandole & H. Van der Merve (Eds.), Conflict resolution theory and practice: Integration and application. Manchester, England: Manchester University Press. Murenzi, R. (2004). Personal communication from the Minister of Education in Rwanda, as well as other officials, accompanied by documents of the ministry laying out these plans. Nadler, A. (2003). Opening comments on the Social Psychology of Reconciliation. Presentation at the conference on Social Psychology of Reconciliation: Moving from Violent Confrontation to Peaceful Coexistence. University of Connecticut, Storrs. Nunca Mas. (1986). The report of the Argentine National Commission on the disappeared. New York: Farrar, Straus, Giroux. O'Connell Higgins, G. (1994). Resilient adults overcoming a cruel past. San Francisco: JosseyBass. 42 Oliner, S.B., & Oliner, P. (1988). The altruistic personality: Rescuers of Jews in Nazi Europe. New York: Free Press. Opotaw, S. (Ed.) (1990). Moral exclusion and injustice. Journal of Social Issues, 46, (1). Parson, E.O. (1984). The reparation of the self: Clinical and theoretical dimensions in the treatment of Vietnam combat veterans. Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy, 4, 4-56. Pearlman, L. A. & Saakvitne, K. W. (1995). Trauma and the therapist: Countertransference and vicarious traumatization in psychotherapy with incest survivors. New York: W. W. Norton. Pennebaker, J. W. (2000). The effects of traumatic disclosure on physical and mental health: The values of writing and talking about upsetting events. In Violanti, J. M., Paton, D., & Dunning, C. (Eds.) Posttraumatic Stress Intervention. Springfield, IL: Charles Thomas Publisher. Perlman, D. (2002). Reparalogy: Toward a scientific evolutionary model of healing after protracted collective trauma. Unpublished Manuscript. Pettigrew, T.F. (1997). Generalized intergroup contact effects on prejudice. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, (2), 173-185. Pettigrew, T.F. (1998). Intergroup contact theory. Annual Review of Psychology. 49, 65-85. Pettigrew, T. F. & Tropp, L. R. (in press). Does intergroup contact reduce prejudice? Recent meta-analytic findings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Proceedings of Stockholm International Forum: A conference on Truth, Justice and Reconciliation, Stockholm (2002, April 23-24), Stockholm: Regeringskanliet Prunier, G. (1995). The Rwanda crisis: History of a genocide. New York: Columbia University Press. 43 Rhodes, G., Allen, G. J., Nowicki, J., & Cillesen, A. (2002). The violent socialization scale: Development and initial validation. In J. Ulmer & L. Athens (Eds.), Violent acts and violentization: Assessing, applying, and developing Lonnie Athens' theories, vol. 4 (pp. 125-144). Elsevier Science Ltd. Ross, M. H., & Rothman, J. (1999). Theory and practice in ethnic conflict management: Theorizing success and failure. New York: Macmillian. Rouhana, N.N. & Kelman, H.C. (1994). Promoting joint thinking in international conflicts: An Israeli-Palestinian Continuing Workshop. Journal of Social Issues, 50, 157-178. Salomon G. (2003). Effects of Peace education in an intractable conflict. Unpublished manuscript. University of Haifa, Center for Research on Peace Education. Salomon, G. & Nevo, B. (2002) Peace education: The concept, principles and practice around the world. (pp 27-36). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Sherif, M., Harvey, D. J., White, B. J., Hood, W. K., & Sherif, C. W. (1961). Intergroup conflict and cooperation: The Robber's cave experiment. Norman, OK: University of Oklahoma Book Exchange. Shriver, D.W., Jr., (1995). An ethic for enemies: Forgiveness in politics. New York: Oxford University Press. Staub, E. (1989a). Steps along the continuum of destruction: The evolution of bystanders, German psychoanalysts, and lessons for today. Political Psychology, 10, 39-53. Staub, E. (1989b). The roots of evil: The origins of genocide and other group violence. New York: Cambridge University Press. Staub, E. (1990). Moral exclusion, personal goal theory and extreme destructiveness. In S. Opotaw (Ed.), Moral exclusion and injustice. Journal of Social Issues, 46, 47-65. 44 Staub, E. (1996). Cultural-societal roots of violence: The examples of genocidal violence and of contemporary youth violence in the United States. American Psychologist, 51, 17-132. Staub, E. (1998). Breaking the cycle of genocidal violence: Healing and reconciliation. In Harvey, J. (Ed.). Perspectives on Loss. Washington, DC: Taylor and Francis. Staub, E. (1999a). The origins and prevention of genocide, mass killing and other collective violence. Peace and Conflict: Journal of Peace Psychology, 5, 303-337. Staub, E. (1999b). The roots of evil: Personality, social conditions, culture and basic human needs. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 3, 179-192. Staub, E. (2000). Genocide and mass killing: Origins, prevention, healing, and reconciliation. Political Psychology, 21(2), 367-382. Staub, E. (2003). The psychology of good and evil: Why children, adults and groups help and harm others. New York: Cambridge University Press. Staub, E. (2005a). Constructive Rather than Harmful Forgiveness, Reconciliation, and Ways Promote Them after Genocide and Mass Killing. In Worthington, E. (ed.). Handbook of Forgiveness. Brunner-Routledge . Staub, E. (2005b). The roots of goodness: The fulfillment of basic human needs and the development of caring, helping and non-aggression, inclusive caring, moral courage, active bystandership, and altruism born of suffering. In Carlo, G. & Edwards, C. (Eds.) Moral development across the life-span. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. Lincoln: Nebraska University Press. Staub, E., (in preparation). Another form of heroism: survivors saving themselves and its impact on their lives. In Feldman, O. & Tetlock, P., Personality and Politics: Essays in Honor of Peter Suedfeld. 45 Staub, E. & Bar-Tal, D. (2003). Genocide, mass killing and intractable conflict: roots, evolution, prevention and reconciliation. In Sears, D., Huddy, L. and Jarvis, R. (Eds.). Handbook of Political Psychology. New York: Oxford University Press. Staub, E. and Pearlman, L. A. (2001). Healing, reconciliation and forgiving after genocide and other collective violence. In S. J. Helmick & R. L. Petersen (Eds.) Forgiveness and Reconciliation: Religion, Public policy and Conflict Transformation. Randor, PA: Templeton Foundation Press. Staub, E. and Pearlman, L. A. (2002). Facilitators' Summary of Observations and Recommendations from Leaders Seminar. See website at www.heal-reconcilerwanda.org. Staub, E. and Pearlman, L.A. (2006). Advancing healing and reconciliation. Barbanel, L. & Sternberg, R. (eds). Psychological interventions in times of crisis. New York: Springer-Verlag. Staub, E., Pearlman, L.A., Gubin, A., and Hagengimana, A. (2005). Healing, forgiveness and reconciliation in Rwanda: Intervention and experimental evaluation. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 24 (3). 297-334. Staub, E., Pearlman, L. A., and Miller, V. (2003). Healing the roots of genocide in Rwanda. Peace Review : 15:3 287-294. Tajfel, H. (1982). Social psychology of intergroup relations. Annual Review of Psychology, 33, 1-39. Tutu, D. (1999). There is no future without forgiveness. NY: Doubleday. 46 Tyler, T. R. & Smith, H. J. (1998). Social justice and social movements. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.) Handbook of social psychology (4th ed. Vol. 2, pp 595-629). New York: McGraw-Hill. Uvin, P. (2003). Wake up. Some policy proposals for the international community in Rwanda. Unpublished manuscript. Tufts University. Valent, P. (1998). Child survivors: A review. In J. Kestenberg and C. Kahn (eds.). Children surviving persecution: An international study of trauma and healing. New York: Praeger. Varshney, A. (2002). Ethnic conflict and civic life: Hindus and Muslims in India. New Haven, CN: Yale University Press. Volkan, V. (1997). Blood lines: From ethnic pride to ethnic terrorism. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux. Volkan, V. D. (1998). Tree model: Psychopolitical dialogues and the promotion of coexistence. In E. Weiner (Ed.), The handbook of interethnic coexistence. New York: Continuum. Wessells, M., & Monteiro, C. (2001). Psychosocial Interventions and post-war reconstruction in Angola: Interweaving western and traditional approaches. In D.J. Christie, R.V. Wagner, & D.D. Winter (Eds.), Peace, conflict and violence. Upper Saddler River, New Jersey: Prentice. Willis, F. R. (1965). France, Germany, and the New Europe, 1945-1963. Palo Alto: Stanford University Press. Worthington, E. L. (1998). (Ed). Dimensions of forgiveness, Philadelphia: Templeton Foundation Press. 47 48