LevinthalFranconeriGrouping

advertisement
In Press, Psychological Science
Common fate grouping as feature selection
Brian R. Levinthal and Steven L. Franconeri
Department of Psychology, Northwestern University
Address correspondence to either author:
b-levinthal@northwestern.edu, franconeri@northwestern.edu
Northwestern University
Department of Psychology
2029 Sheridan Rd
Evanston, IL 60208
Phone: 847-497-1259
Fax: 847-491-7859
The visual system groups elements within the visual field that are physically separated, yet similar to each other.
Despite intense study for a century, mechanisms for many of these grouping processes remain elusive. Here, we
propose that a primary mechanism for grouping by common fate is attentional selection of a direction of motion.
This account makes a unique prediction – that we must be limited to forming only a single group at a time. If so,
then finding a particular group among other groups, or among non-groups, should be highly inefficient. We show
that this is true in searches for moving dots for a vertical among horizontal groups (Experiment 1) and motion-linked
groups among non-linked objects (Experiment 2). Feature selection may limit the visual system to one common fate
group at a time, exposing the experience of simultaneous grouping as an illusion.
Key Words: Visual Perception, Perceptual Organization, Attention, Selection
The human visual system breaks the incoming image
into separate elements, but also reassembles these
elements in the form of groups. Grouping processes have
been studied for a century, and much of this work has
focused on classifying different types of groups
(Wertheimer, 1923), determining when grouping occurs
(Palmer, 2002), and measuring the relative strength of
one form of grouping compared to another (Kubovy &
van den Berg, 2008). Although neurally plausible
mechanisms have been proposed for some types of
grouping (e.g., contour grouping; Roelfsema, 2006), for
many other types of grouping such explanations remain
elusive. Here we propose a mechanism for the Gestalt
principle of common fate, where objects appear grouped
when they display the same pattern of motion. We argue
that a primary mechanism underlying this powerful form
of grouping is the selection of a direction of motion.
The parsimony of this explanation rests on previous
demonstrations that the selection of a direction of motion
(as well as features such as color and orientation) can
occur in parallel across the visual field (Andersen,
Müller, & Hillyard, 2009; Maunsell & Treue, 2006;
Saenz, Buracas, & Boynton, 2002). Selecting a direction
of motion could produce a spatially organized map of
visual field locations corresponding to the selected
direction (see Figure 1), with a ‘bump’ on the map for
each element that also moved in that direction (see
Huang & Pashler, 2007, for a similar account). The
initial motion direction could be extracted from a
statistical summary of motion patterns within a display
(Williams & Sekuler, 1984), or by selecting a single
object and then encoding its motion direction. As the
group's motion direction changes, a simple feedback loop
could update the selected direction to match that of the
group (see Martinez-Trujillo, Cheyne, Gaetz, Simine, &
Tsotsos, 2007, for evidence of motion direction updating
while tracking translating objects).
This map would provide two critical components of
the grouping process. First, it would help isolate
processing of features and identities to the objects at
those locations (Treisman & Gelade, 1980).
Simultaneously selecting these locations may lead them
to appear to ‘belong’ together, even though they are
spatially separated (Xu, 2008). This simultaneous
selection may also provide a summary representation
(e.g., Alvarez & Oliva, 2009) of other features available
at those locations, leading to a holistic representation of
these otherwise separate objects. For example, selecting
1
In Press, Psychological Science
a set of rightward moving objects could also facilitate
judgments about the number of objects (Halberda, Sires,
& Feigenson, 2006) or the distribution of sizes of those
objects (Chong & Treisman, 2005).
Second, because any area of the visual field
containing that feature would result in a ‘bump’ in the
spatially organized map, the complex distribution of the
selected locations would convey the shape of the group
(Huang & Pashler, 2007). By enhancing target activity
and attenuating distractors in early visual areas (e.g., area
V1; Saenz, Buracas, & Boynton, 2002), shape processing
could operate as if only those target locations were
present in the display. Alternatively, higher-level areas
such as the inferior intraparietal sulcus (Xu, 2008) may
represent these spatially organized maps. The IPS is
associated with tracking of selected locations in the
visual field Culham, et al., 1998), and is sensitive to
grouping among those locations (Xu, 2008).
Figure 1. Without selecting a direction of motion, all
moving elements in a display would be processed
together, and there would be no common fate grouping.
As a consequence of selecting a direction of motion,
target elements are enhanced, forming a common fate
group. Changes in the direction of selected motion could
be updated via feedback connections within motionsensitive areas of the visual system (e.g., MartinezTrujillo et al., 2007).
The proposed mechanism is consistent with a
powerful property of common fate grouping: a group can
be constructed in a massively parallel operation where all
elements across the visual field sharing a feature are
selected. However, it also makes a unique prediction
about a salient weakness – ‘bumps’ on a spatially
organized map would be distinguishable only by their
location, so we should only be able to construct one
group at a time. This contrasts with our phenomenology
of seeing multiple common fate groups simultaneously.
Here, we report the results of two visual search
experiments that demonstrate that this intuition is an
illusion. There are severe limitations on the number of
common fate groups that can be constructed at once.
Experiment 1
In Experiment 1 we asked participants to perform a
visual search for a vertical line among horizontal lines,
with search objects defined by pairs of moving dots.
Because searches for odd-oriented lines typically occur
efficiently (e.g., Treisman & Gelade, 1980), this search
task is well suited for determining whether common fate
groups can be formed in parallel. A control condition
verified this efficiency in our displays, by demonstrating
parallel search after each common fate group was
connected by a 1-pixel-thin line.
Methods
Participants. Ten participants, either volunteers
from the Northwestern University student population
(ages 18-21), or paid participants from the Evanston, IL
community (ages 18 – 21), participated in this
experiment.
Apparatus. Stimuli were presented on a 17-inch
CRT monitor with 1024x768 resolution and 85HZ
refresh rate, and were generated using MATLAB and the
Psych Toolbox (Brainard, 1997; Pelli, 1997).
Stimuli and Procedure. Displays consisted of
pairs of moving dots against a static background with 250
randomly-positioned dots (approximately .3 dots per
square degree). Dot pairs moved at a constant velocity
(2.65°/sec), and each pair was assigned an initial random
angle of motion. Dot pairs moved in a straight-line path
until reaching the boundary of an invisible 5.2° square
region, where the angle of motion was reflected. This
motion continued indefinitely until participants made a
present/absent response via key press.
Each dot maintained a constant distance (2°) from its
pair throughout each trial. In target-absent trials (50%),
all dot-pairs were arranged horizontally. In targetpresent trials (50%), a single pair of dots was arranged
vertically. In a connected control condition, a single
pixel line connected each dot in a pair. Either 2, 4 or 8
dot-pairs were present in each trial, and were placed in
adjacent positions along an imaginary 8° radius circle.
Movies of samples trials are available at
http://viscog.psych.northwestern.edu/demos/
Results
Figure 2 depicts the task and results. If only one
common fate group is created at a time, adding more
groups should substantially increase response times.
Response times revealed a significant increase in search
slope (55ms/item; t(9) = 17.3, p < .001) as more
2
In Press, Psychological Science
distractor objects were present in the display. When
common fate groups were replaced by connectivity
groups, which can be processed in parallel (Franconeri,
Bemis, & Alvarez, 2009; Rensink & Enns, 1995), adding
more groups to the display did not substantially affect
response times (6ms/item; t(9) = 2.7, p = .023).
Furthermore, search through common fate groups was
significantly less efficient than search through
connectivity groups (t(9) = 12.3, p < .001). Thus, the
costs of searching through common fate groups were
specific to the construction of those groups, and not to
increases in display complexity or shape identification
requirements of the search task.
Figure 2. In each experiment, participants indicated the
presence or absence of a target via keypress. Experiment
1 – Vertical targets and horizontal distractors moved in
straight-line paths and bounced off of invisible walled
regions around fixation.
Dot pairs either formed
common-fate groups (shown) or were connected by onepixel thin lines. When common fate grouping was
required to perform the task searches were inefficient
(55ms/group) compared to when the elements were
connected by lines (6ms/group). Experiment 2 – dot pairs
moved in orbiting paths; the target was a dot pair that
moved in-phase among dot pairs that moved out of phase.
In a control condition, target dots were indicated by a
valid color cue. When participants searched for a
common fate group among non-groups, their searches
were inefficient (82ms/group) compared to when the
target group was cued (7ms/group).
Experiment 2
Experiment 1 showed a severe capacity limitation on
extracting the shape of a common fate group. However,
even if shape information is not available, the visual
system may be able to detect coarse details of groups in
the display (e.g., the number of ‘clusters’ of grouped
elements; see Trick & Enns, 1997).
Whereas
discrimination of the shape of common fate groups may
be inefficient, observers may nonetheless be able to
efficiently detect the existence of such groups. To test
this possibility, we altered the visual search task so that
participants judged whether a single common fate group
was present in a display filled with non-grouped objects
(see Figure 2).
Methods
Participants. Twelve participants (ages 18 – 21)
took part in this experiment, either volunteers from the
Northwestern University student population or paid
participants from the Evanston, IL community.
Stimuli and Procedure. Dot-pairs were initially
arranged similarly to Experiment 1, except that 5 or 9
pairs were used in this experiment. All dots followed an
orbiting motion at a rate of 6.28°/sec around an
imaginary circle with a 1° radius. In distractor dot pairs,
each dot revolved around a separate locus, and 180° out
of phase. Distractor dots never approached closer than
0.5°, and were never separated by more than 3.5°. Target
dots moved in an identical fashion, but revolved around
separate loci in phase, and therefore always maintained a
constant 2° distance. Target dots were assigned a unique
phase and distractor dots were always at least 75° out of
phase from the target group. The motion persisted until
participants indicated a target present/absent response via
key press. Participants were instructed to determine
whether a target was present (50%) or absent (50%).
Movies of samples trials are available at
http://viscog.psych.northwestern.edu/demos/
Results
Response times revealed significant costs
(82ms/item; t(11) = 12.3, p<.001) as more distractor pairs
were present, suggesting that even detecting the presence
of common fate groups shows severe capacity
restrictions. To ensure that the costs of additional
distractors were not simply due to increased display
complexity, a control condition confirmed that when the
target group was cued with a unique color, adding more
distractors to the display did not affect response times
(7ms/item; t(11) = 1.49, p=.16). These two slopes also
differed significantly from each other, t(11) = 7.45, p <
.001.
3
In Press, Psychological Science
While the present experiment shows that detecting
groups is severely capacity-limited, there may be ‘tricks’
that would allow an observer to detect them efficiently in
other displays. For example, consider a common fate
group consisting of coherently moving elements placed
among randomly moving elements. As the number of
objects within the group increases (relative to the total
number on the screen), that group’s motion direction
would begin to dominate the ‘global’ distribution of
motion directions available from the entire display. In
this case, competition among multiple motion directions
may be resolved through simple feedback mechanisms in
the visual system (Chey, Grossberg, & Mingolla, 1997;
Tsotsos, et al., 1995), such that the dominant motion
direction would tend to be selected first. This would
result in what appears to be an efficient and seemingly
automatic representation of a common fate group. In
contrast, Experiment 2 shows that when a common fate
group’s motion direction does not dominate others by a
significant ratio, that group cannot be efficiently
detected.
Discussion
We have argued that a core mechanism for common
fate grouping is the selection of a direction of motion.
The selection of a motion direction would enhance the
neural activity associated with similarly-moving elements
across the visual field, providing the visual system with a
map of locations for further processing. This account
makes the surprising prediction that only one common
fate group can be formed at a time. We found evidence
consistent with this prediction across two experiments.
In Experiment 1, participants searched among groups for
a vertical line among horizontal lines. When common
fate grouping was required to perform the task, searches
were highly inefficient. But when elements were
connected by lines, the vertical group could be instantly
identified. Similarly, in Experiment 2, when participants
searched for a single common fate group among nongrouped objects, searches were highly inefficient.
We argue that these inefficient searches were due to
the need to sequentially select the current motion
direction of each group. We note that two alternatives
are theoretically possible. First, common fate groups
might be formed in parallel, but the search process
cannot efficiently access that representation. This
alternative is less likely, because previous studies show
visual search tasks to be a sensitive measure of visual
grouping processes (e.g., Trick & Enns, 1997; Rensink &
Enns, 1995; Wolfe & Bennett, 1997). Second, the
inefficient search for common fate groups might stem
from a limited-capacity parallel grouping process (see
Palmer, 1995, for discussion). This alternative is also
less likely, because it is unclear why such a resource
limitation might arise, and this type of solution would
require additional mechanisms that maintain the
representations of a potentially unrestricted number of
common fate groups. Compared to each of these
alternatives, the sequential selection account has the
powerful advantage of parsimony. Because previous
work shows that motion directions can be selected in a
global fashion (Saenz, Buracas, & Boynton, 2002), we
can concretely specify how this known mechanism could
support common fate grouping. The burden of proof
therefore falls on alternative accounts to demonstrate
experimental effects that cannot be explained by the
serial feature selection account.
The feature selection account, at first glance, appears
to contradict prior observations that feature-based
grouping can be accomplished ’outside the focus of
attention‘ (Kimchi & Razpurker-Apfeld, 2004; Moore &
Egeth, 1997; Russell & Driver, 2005). Those prior
studies showed that when participants perform a
demanding primary task at fixation, the perceptual
organization of an irrelevant background (groupings of
luminance, color, or orientation) influenced performance,
even when participants reported no awareness or memory
for the background's content. However, any ostensible
conflict with our results would be due to different
definitions of 'attention.' Prior studies show a processing
bottleneck where perceptual groups do not reach the
processing stages required for awareness or memory.
Grouping by feature selection would still be possible,
especially if that type of selection were not incompatible
with the primary task.
Although the present study has focused on common
fate grouping, feature selection may be a more
generalized mechanism for any type of similarity
grouping, including not just similarity of motion, but also
similarity of color, shape, or orientation (Huang &
Pashler, 2007). If so, similarity grouping may be subject
to the same one-group limits as common fate grouping:
only one group can be created at a time. This prediction
finds support in recent studies –when observers are asked
to determine whether multicolored patterns are
symmetrical, judgments are fast when the displays
consist of few colors, and slower when displays consist
of several colors, suggesting that symmetry judgments
can be made for only a single color subpattern at a time
(Morales & Pashler, 1999; Huang & Pashler 2002). The
proposal that similarity grouping stems from feature
selection implies a substantial difference between
similarity grouping and other types of grouping.
Specifically, grouping by proximity, connectivity, or
4
In Press, Psychological Science
common region may rely on a different mechanism that
can produce discrete units in parallel (Franconeri Bemis,
& Alvarez, 2009; Palmer & Rock, 1994; Rensink &
Enns, 1995). The term ‘grouping’ is likely too broad to
capture the diversity of mechanisms that cause some
elements of the visual field to associate with others.
Selection of a motion direction may not be the only
mechanism available for grouping moving objects. For
some types of frequently encountered moving patterns,
such as bodies walking, wings flapping, or mouths
moving during speech, there are almost certainly longterm representations for more specific patterns of motion
(Cavanagh, Labianca, & Thornton, 2001) that could
facilitate perception. A challenge for our proposed
mechanism, which predicts the availability of only a
single surface at a time, is to explain more complex
grouping abilities related to common fate such as the
appearance of rigid three-dimensional objects from
numerous and dense patterns of moving dots (structurefrom-motion, Wallach & O’Connell, 1953). While such
percepts might require complex local processing of the
motion pattern (Ullman, 1979) we argue that the simpler
mechanism described here may produce surprisingly rich
percepts, especially when combined with other cues such
as statistical information (Alvarez & Oliva, 2009; Balas,
Nakano, & Rosenholtz, 2009) about the distribution of
motion directions, and edges created by motion contrast
(Regan & Beverley, 1984).
For both simple common fate groups, and possibly
more complex motion patterns, feature selection presents
a parsimonious account of grouping by motion. We may
construct a single such group at a time, and anything
more may be an illusion of perceptual detail.
Acknowledgements.
We thank Hyunyoung Park for assistance in data
collection. This work was partially supported by NSF
CAREER grant BCS-1056730 (S.F), and NIH grant T32NS047987(B.L.).
References
Alvarez, G.A., & Oliva, A. (2009). Spatial ensemble
statistics are efficient codes that can be represented
with reduced attention. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences, 106, 7345-7350.
Andersen, S.K., Müller, M.M., & Hillyard, S.A. (2009).
Color-selective attention need not be mediated by
spatial attention. Journal of Vision, 9(6):2, 1-7.
Balas, B., Nakano, L., & Rosenholtz, R. (2009). A
summary-statistic representation in peripheral vision
explains visual crowding.
Journal of Vision,
9(12):13, 1-18.
Brainard, D.H. (1997). The Psychophysics Toolbox.
Spatial Vision, 10, 433-436.
Cavanagh, P., Labianca, A.T., Thornton, I.M. (2001).
Attention-based visual routines: sprites. Cognition,
80 (1-2), 47-60.
Chey, J., Grossberg, S., & Mingolla, M. (1997). Neural
dynamics of motion grouping: from aperture
ambiguity to object speed and direction. Journal of
the Optical Society of America, 14, 2570-2594.
Chong, S.C., & Treisman, A. (2005).
Statistical
processing: computing the average size in perceptual
groups. Vision Research, 45, 891-900.
Culham, J.C., Brandt, S.A., Cavanagh, P., Kanwisher,
N.G., Dale, A.M., & Tootell, R.B.H. (1998).
Cortical fMRI activation produced by attentive
tracking of moving targets.
Journal of
Neurophysiology, 80(5), 2657-2670.
Franconeri, S.L, Bemis, D.K., & Alvarez, G.A. (2009).
Number estimation relies on a set of segmented
objects. Cognition, 113, 1-13.
Halberda, J., Sires, S.F., & Feigenson, L. (2006).
Multiple spatially overlapping sets can be
enumerated in parallel.
Psychological Science,
17(7), 572 – 576.
Huang, L., & Pashler, H. (2002). Symmetry detection
and visual attention: a “binary map” hypothesis.
Vision Research, 42, 1421-1430.
Huang, L., & Pashler, H. (2007). A Boolean map theory
of visual attention. Psychological Review, 114, 599631.
Kimchi, R., & Razpurker-Apfeld, I. (2004). Perceptual
grouping and attention: Not all groupings are equal.
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11(4), 687-696.
Kubovy, M., & van den Berg, M. (2008). The whole is
equal to the sum of its parts: a probabilistic model of
grouping by proximity and similarity in regular
patterns. Psychological Review, 115(1), 131 – 154.
Martinez-Trujillo, J.C., Cheyne, D., Gaetz, W., Simine,
E., & Tsotsos, J.K. (2007). Activation of area
MT/V5+ and the right inferior parietal cortex during
the discrimination of transient direction changes in
translational motion. Cerebral Cortex, 17(7), 17339.
Maunsell, J.H.R., & Treue, S. (2006). Feature-based
attention in visual cortex. Trends in Neuroscience,
29, 317-322.
Moore, C.M., & Egeth, H.E. (1997). Perception without
attention: Evidence of grouping under conditions of
inattention. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Human Perception and Performance, 23, 339-352.
Morales, D., & Pashler, H. (1999). No role for colour in
symmetry perception. Nature, 399, 115-116.
5
In Press, Psychological Science
Palmer, S., & Rock, I. (1994). Rethinking perceptual
organization: The role of uniform connectedness.
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1(1), 29-55.
Palmer, J.
(1995).
Attention in visual search:
Distinguishing four causes of set-size effects.
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 4, 118123.
Palmer, S.E. (2002). Perceptual grouping: It’s later than
you think. Current Directions in Psychological
Science, 11(3), 101-106.
Pelli, D.G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for
visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into
movies. Spatial Vision, 10, 437-442.
Regan, D., & Beverley, K. I. (1984). Figure-ground
segregation by motion contrast and by luminance
contrast. Journal of the Optical Society of America,
1, 433 – 442.
Rensink, R.A., & Enns, J.T. (1995). Preemption effects
in visual search: Evidence for low-level grouping.
Psychological Review, 102, 101-130.
Roelfsema, P.R. (2006).
Cortical algorithms for
perceptual grouping.
Annual Review of
Neuroscience, 29, 203-227.
Russell, C., & Driver, J. (2005). New indirect measures
of “inattentive” visual grouping in a change-detection
task. Perception & Psychophysics. 64(4), 606-623.
Saenz, M., Buracas, G. T., & Boynton, G. M. (2002).
Global effects of feature-based attention in human
visual cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 5, 631-632.
Trick, L. M., & Enns, J. T. (1997). Clusters precede
shapes in perceptual organization. Psychological
Science, 8 (2), 124-129.
Treisman A.M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A featureintegration theory of attention.
Cognitive
Psychology, 12(1), 97-136.
Ullman, S. (1979). The interpretation of structure from
motion. Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 203,
405-426.
Wallach, H., & O’Connell, D. N. (1953). The kinetic
depth effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology,
45, 205-217.
Wertheimer, M. (1923). Untersuchungen zur Lehre von
der Gestalt. Psychologishe Forschung, 4, 301-350.
Williams, D.W., & Sekuler, R. (1984). Coherent global
motion percepts from stochastic local motions.
Vision Research, 24(1), 55-62.
Wolfe, J.M. & Bennett, S.C. (1997). Preattentive Object
Files: Shapeless bundles of basic features. Vision
Research, 37(1), 25-44.
Xu, Y. (2008). Representing connected and disconnected
shapes in intraparietal sulcus. Neuroimage, 40, 1849
– 1856.
6
Download