Psychomotor Functioning of Children with Psychiatric Disorders Claudia Emck1, Ruud Bosscher2 & Theo Doreleijers3 Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam, The Netherlands 1,2 Faculty of Human Movement Sciences 3 Medical Faculty Abstract: Children with psychiatric disorders often show disturbances and peculiarities in psychomotor functioning. Although studies about specific groups on specific areas of motor functioning exist, a broader investigation of psychomotor functioning on relevant aspects for diverse psychiatric disorders is lacking. This presentation aims to give an overview of knowledge derived from scientific studies on psychomotor functioning of three subgroups, children with emotional (anxiety, depression), developmental (autism spectrum) and behavioural (hyperactivity and conduct) disorders. Psychomotor functioning in children is defined as a compound developmental area, which contains two main ingredients: movement behaviour and body image. Concretely, delays in motor development, fine and gross motor functioning are important domains, as are physical fitness, perceived motor competence, play behaviour, bodily experience and body image. Subsequently, preliminary results of our empirical study will be presented. We are testing a group of psychiatric children with the following battery: the test of gross motor development (TGMD-II - Ulrich, 2000), a Dutch fitness test (MOPER) (Leyten et al 1982), questionnaires on perceived motor competence and physical appearance (CBSK M and Ph - Veerman et al 1997; Van Rossum & Vermeer 2000), a Dutch standardised clinical psychomotor observation (PMDC) (Hammink 2003), a behavioural inventory (CBCL: parent version and TRF: teachers version) (Achenbach & Rescorla 2001), questionnaires on social behaviour (VISK) (Luteijn et al 2002) and psychiatric symptoms and syndromes (DISC and Auti-R) (Ferdinant & van der Ende 2002; Berckelaer-Onnes & Hoekman 1991), in order to describe possible psychomotor profiles of the specific groups of children. Consequences for psychomotor indication, therapy, evaluation and research will be discussed. Introduction From our point of view, the main task we are facing, in next years, is the development of a European, or even cross-Atlantic international, framework for psychomotor therapy. We hope this framework will go beyond national conventions and integrates specific psychomotor knowledge and practice. We also hope to contribute to ideas exchange, which will stimulate the effectuation of such framework. We will briefly discuss the present state of knowledge about psychomotor functioning in children with psychiatric disorders. The main themes are discussed in light of their practical relevance for psychomotor therapy. Than we will discuss psychomotor diagnostics, tests and instruments used in clinical practice and research. You may wonder why we focus on “psychiatric disorders”. After all, much work of psychomotor therapists in Europe takes place outside the medical field (e.g.: in schools). To explain this choice, we have to contradict our earlier proposition about going beyond national conventions. In fact, in The Netherlands most child practitioners are working within a psychiatric unit or centre. Therefore, Dutch psychomotor therapists are trained to use developmental psychopathology and psychiatric classification as a frame of reference, besides psychomotor theory. However, there is more. Especially in Anglo-Saxon research literature on child therapy, DSM-IV fourth edition, (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Psychiatric Disorders, APA 1994) is the rule. If we want to join the scientific debate on therapy effects on an international level, research oriented psychomotor therapists (just like psychiatrists and psychologists) have to be able to understand and discuss pathology in DSM-IV terms. From a clinical perspective, it is helpful to describe children who seek help by their psychiatric complaints. As a therapist, it is relevant to know whether a child has a “full blown” depression, a posttraumatic stress disorder or mixed features of both anxiety and mood disorders. Clear language about pathology is necessary to make good clinical decisions. As a matter of fact, the development of multidisciplinary clinical guidelines, in USA and The Netherlands, is based on psychiatric diagnostic categories. Psychomotor therapists participate in tasks forces to develop these national guidelines. However, a DSM-IV classification is too narrow for our discipline. We have to combine psychiatric classification with a more dimensional approach on psychomotor functioning. Therefore, we need scientific information of psychomotor functioning in relation to specific disorders and additional data regarding the specific psychomotor profile of each particular child. Subgroups Firstly, we will divide three distinguishable psychiatric subgroups of childeren with psychiatric disorders. 1. Emotional disorders: these children are characterized by internalisation of their problems; they are silent, anxious, sad or pondering; they rarely annoy their teachers because they keep their problems to themselves. In DSM-IV terms, we speak of depression, social phobia or separation anxiety. Sometimes these children also suffer from enuresis, eating disorders or somatic complaints. 2. Developmental disorders: these children suffer from mild to severe autism; many suffer also from mental retardation (about 75%, Lord & Rutter, 1994); but the main area of deficits concerns communication, social development, restricted and repetitive behaviours and interests. These are the odd kids in classroom who do not connect with their peers like others do. The Asperger subgroup is characterized by good language development and normal (or above) IQ, but show the same problems in social performance. 3. Behavioural disorders: these children are hyperactive, inattentive, oppositional, and aggressive or deny social, moral or legal rules - that is, they externalise their problems, causing others (especially teachers) a lot of trouble. By their way of behaving they provoke negative responses by others and exclusion of normal activities. This can lead to secondary problems like a negative selfimage, depression or alcohol and drug abuse. But what do we know about their psychomotor functioning? How can we discriminate between them? Do they show typical movement behavior or body image problems? And, if so, are they specific for the group? Do we find peculiarities in psychomotor functioning that cross the formal boundaries of psychiatric diagnosis? Emotional disorders Anxiety influences psychomotor behaviour in several ways. State anxiety, which is, by definition, short and foregoing, seems to impair motor performance (Hardy et al, 2001; Payne et al, 1995; Terelak, 1990; Pokrajc, 1982; Weinberg, 1976; Shepard & Abbey, 1958). And trait anxiety, a part of the character, seems to impair the performance especially on fine, attentionally demanding motor tasks (Calvo et al, 1988). In other words, anxious children are likely to perform worse on several motor tasks. Children with (psychiatric) anxiety disorders show high muscle tone, lack of performance-motivation and anxiety to fail in motor performance situations (Emck, 1997; Emck & Smit, 2000). Sometimes, these children are also deprived in their movement activities, which results in lagging behind in motor development. Children with trauma-related anxiety, as in posttraumatic stress disorders, are clinically characterized by problematic body experiences (like pain and feelings of discomfort) and negative body image (Lamers-Winkelman, 1997; Emck, 1997; Emck & Smit, 2000). In a screening profile for sexually abused children, Lamers-Winkelman (1997) describes amongst other a lack of spontaneous movement in play, a stiff, tense and wooden movement pattern, fear of body contact and dislike of own body. To what extend these psychomotor phenomena can be influenced is not sure. However, psychomotor interventions may improve body image, decrease muscle tone and stimulate spontaneous play behavior. In depression, several aspects of motor functioning seem to be impaired. This is possibly explained by neurological disturbances (e.g., Braun et al, 2000, found that patients with depression and psychomotor lethargy show left hemisphere lesions). Nevertheless, most studies on this subject concern adults (and some of them to adolescents). As there are developmental differences in affective disorders, we must be cautious to generalize these findings to children. Still, some features are likely to be shared at all ages. First, decreased energy and fatigue are common in depressed patients. For instance, Patton et al (2000) found that depressed adolescents were less energetic than normal controls. This is, even, more profound in severe episodes, when psychomotor retardation, and sometimes agitation, is seen. Yet, psychomotor agitation and retardation in adults are also associated with anxiety (Strain & Klicpera, 1984), which might also be case in children. Second, psychomotor retardation in depression is associated with reduced ability to experience pleasure (Lemke at al, 1999). This also concerns movement and body experiences normally perceived as nice and enjoyable. This is an important theme in psychomotor therapy for these patients (Bosscher, 1992). Belgian colleagues stress a third aspect. They found a relationship between physical fitness, perceived physical ability and body acceptance in depressed patients. In other words, depressed patients are likely to be in a bad physical condition, which stimulates the low physical abilities perception and low body self-esteem (Van der Vliet et al 1999). This could indicate support for psychomotor therapy because that may have a positive impact on all these features. We agree with Cloitre et al (1993), who found that recovery in depression is associated with a shift from impoverished and disjointed movement to a richer, more coherent non-verbal behavior. Again, this may also apply to children with depressive disorders, especially when considering that they seem to show less cognitive features (like guilt) and express themselves more in a non-verbal way (Harrington, 1994). Developmental disorders The second group, children with autism and pervasive development disorders, is characterized by deficits in communication, social development and restricted behaviour. Which psychomotor features do they show? In a beam-walking test, Kokobun et al (1996) found that motor performance is worse in autistic children than in mental retardation ones. Pry et al (2000) compared those children (aged 5-7 years) with children with psychomotor developmental problems. Autistic children showed impairment on postural regulation and balance. Primitive motor patterns and immature motor organisation are also described within a framework of disturbances of senso-motor development (Van Loon et al, 1997). Body scheme and body awareness are both disturbed (a non-integrated body image occurs in these children). Further, Taylor (1994) points out that autism can be associated with hyperactive behavior. No specific studies were found on physical fitness, perceived motor competence, but grounded on profound motor problems we expect these domains also to be impaired. And although autistic children show psychomotor impairments, it is still unclear if they are related to autism or associated with cognitive developmental problems. As developmental disorders are known to be “life time” conditions, it is hard to tell whether the associated psychomotor problems can be improved by training or therapy, or not. Instead of trying to focus the deficits, it seems plausible that helping child to deal with psychomotor problems is more helpful than trying to solve it. It could also be helpful to improve perceived competence in order to prevent additional emotional disorders like depression and anxiety (Lord & Rutter, 1994). Behavioural Disorders There are three subgroups in children with behavioural disorders: children with Attention-deficit / Hyperactivity Disorders (ADHD), Oppositional Defiant Disorders (ODD) and Conduct disorders. The ADHD group is well known for its striking movement behavior. But there is also a silent version: the inattentive subtype. Whereas the hyperactive child is characterized by disturbing motor behaviour* in the classroom, the inattentive child will not cause such problems. Problems with attention demanding tasks will influence schoolwork and social interaction in a less obvious way. We have to keep this in mind while focus on specific psychomotor behavior. Barkley (1997) states that 52% of ADHD children have motor problems (35% in the normal population). Fine motor skills, like writing and shoe lacing, are most problematic (as well in hyperactive and in inattentive subgroups – Whitmond & Clark, 1996; Piek et al, 1999). Moreover, many of these children show problems in lateralisation and are left-handed (Reid et al, 2000). For ADHD children (7-12 years, n= 19), Harvey & Reid (1997) found that gross motor skills and fitness condition are below average. And impulsivity, which is seen in most ADHD children, impairs performance in perceptual-motor tasks and is observable as a clinical feature in gross motor behaviour (Barret, 1984; Emck, 1997). Further, ADHD children show difficulties in regulating bodily distance and taking social perspective in sports and games, which leads to interpersonal conflicts with peers (Emck, 197; Flavell, 1985). They often show disturbing behaviour, like verbal and physical aggression, which is followed by social exclusion. Because they like to interact with their peers, rejection will have a negative impact on their self-image (Verhulst, 1997). From this perspective, it is not surprising that co-morbid depression occurs in a quarter of these children. For the psychomotor therapist, the question rises on which aspects to focus. Should we try to improve motor skills, boost fitness, increase control over impulsivity or stimulate adequate play behaviour and self-image? And what are the chances of a successful intervention? * fidgets with hands, leaves seat, runs / climbs, playing problems, talking, impulsive answering, not waiting turns, interrupting... The other behavioural disorders*, oppositional defiant (ODD) and conduct disorders (CD) often lead to antisocial behaviour in adulthood. These children are at risk to develop personality disorders and substance abuse (Earls, 1994). Some developmental psychologists suggest that children with these disorders do not show remarkable psychomotor behaviour (Netelenbos, 1998). But Aendekerk (1997), a Dutch psychomotor therapist, takes another view. She describes the movement pattern as characterised by rash, impulsive and unconscious action, motor restlessness, violence, agitation and hyper alertness. Also, bodily tension and a disturbed development of body awareness occur. Furthermore, these children overestimate their motor abilities (Aendekerk, 1997). Unfortunately, there is a lack of sound studies on this subject. An overview So far, we can identify some main problems in psychomotor behavior in each group: In emotional disorders, anxiety is associated with problems in perceived motor competence, a negative body image and a lack of spontaneous movement play (sometimes together with a fear of body contact). In depression, the same items are problematic. Although not much is known about play behavior, we expect that this will be negatively influenced by decreased energy and reduced ability to experience pleasure. In pervasive developmental disorders the immature motor organisation has its impact on gross motor functioning, body scheme and body image. By definition, play behavior with peers will also be disturbed. In behavioural disorders, ADHD children are below average in gross motor skills and physical fitness. Play behavior is characterized by impulsivity and interpersonal regulation problems. The risk of developing a negative self-image may have an impact on perceived competence as well on body image. In the ODD, play behaviour will be problematic because of the awareness and unrealistic positive perceived motor competence. * Behaviour disorders and mood disorders are often intertwined. According to Baker (1998), these so called mixed disorders are typically characterized by irritability, uncooperativeness and psychomotor agitation. However, Kashani et al (1983) did not find differences in psychomotor agitation between depressive and non-depressive delinquent boys. In short, it remains unclear if we can characterize these children by their psychomotor behaviour. But if in future we can, psychomotor therapy may also contribute through community based models of intervention with sports and games. However, the presence of nondeviant peers in the group is an essential component for a good outcome (Feldman et al, 1983). Here, two questions rise. First, how this global characterization applies to individual psychomotor performance? For instance, do all children of a sub-group show the same features? How is the variation between subgroups? And what impact has a certain psychomotor feature on the overall functioning of a particular child? Second, do we have good instruments to measure the relevant items of an individual level? An explorative field study In our study, we focus on psychomotor functioning of children with psychiatric disorders (N=90). We are investigating which psychomotor profiles occur in the three groups earlier mentioned. Our test battery covers two areas of functioning: 1. Psychiatric diagnosis, complaints and behaviour problems. Those instruments include systematic interviews and questionnaires for parents and teachers. 2. Psychomotor functioning, including motor development, fitness, perceived competence, psychomotor diagnoses and therapy indication. We are especially keen on broad-spectrum psychomotor diagnosis, including several domains besides basic gross motor functioning, like perceived competence, body awareness and play behaviour. For this second domain we use amongst other the Psychomotor Diagnostic Construct (PMDC – Hammink, 2003). This construct includes a procedure to choose psychomotor activities, a checklist with instructions for observation and interview and a list of instruments for progressive diagnostics. After implementation, therapists and psychiatrists were enthusiastic about the systematically derived psychomotor diagnosis. For our present study we are developing a new scoring system, which will give us the opportunity to investigate reliability and validity, and at the same time allows us to look for scores of individual children. The PMDC is based on the ICIDH-model and contains several dimensions and domains. The first dimension - structure and function, refers to anatomical, physiological and psychological qualities. Second dimension concern activities that the child is able to perform in an ideal situation. In participation, the third dimension, we see whether the child is actually involved and participating according to his or her age. The fourth and last dimension – context, refers to external factors influencing the other three. These dimensions are divided in domains, wich refer to the developmental areas: physical, emotional, cognitive and social development. An example: the case of JJ JJ is a boy of eleven years old. His new schoolteacher suspects him of ADHD. His mother, recently divorced, is not convinced by this. Nevertheless, she is willing do participate in our research project in order to ground or reject possible psychiatric diagnosis and determine if some form of treatment for JJ is advisable. Table 1. PMDC scores of JJ Dimension Str/F Act Part Context Total Bodily 0.02 0.01 0.06 0.020 Cognitive 0.05 0.03 0.00 0.040 Emotional 0.00 0.00 0.000 0.19 0.160 Domain Social 002 0.05 0.04 0.00 0.005 Let’s have a look at the example of JJ’s scores on the PMDC. The domains are easily understood. They refer to the child’s bodily, cognitive, emotional and social functioning within the context of psychomotor activities. In this case, it’s possible to observe that all scores are near zero on a scale of 0-2; 0 means no abnormalities, 1 means slightly abnormal and 2 is clearly abnormal. If a child has a score of 2 there may be an indication for psychomotor therapy, which was not the case for JJ. Lets have a look at the other scores. Are they in the same line? Table 2. JJ’s overall results Intrument Outcome DISC No DSM-IV classification AutR/Visk No autism or PDD CBCL Internalisation (at home) TRF Internalisation (at school) CBSK-m Perceived motor competence above average CBSK School.76; social .88; sport .80; physical appearance.40 behavioural attitude.78; global self worth .57 TGMD Good motor development MOPER Fitness below average (inadequate norms?) PMDC No indication PMT On the psychiatric instruments, JJ does not meet the criteria for any classification. However, there are symptoms of separation anxiety (fearful thoughts and dreams) and restlessness, together with a state of some emotional distress, and in school JJ has trouble to stay focussed. Looking at functioning at home, we see internalisation scores (anxiety/depression) just in the clinical range. In school functioning, the scores on externalisation were in the clinical range. Apparently, JJ’s behaviour at school differs from home. JJ shows average results in perceived competence at all domains (clinical range >. 85 and <. 15). His motor development is very good, according to TGMD, but surprisingly, the MOPER fitness score is below average. Based on data we collected so far, we think this may be due to inadequate norms for the present population. Finally, very low scores on the PMDC give no indication for psychomotor therapy. JJ’s psychomotor test results (his psychomotor profile) were very useful in this diagnostic process. The conclusion was that, although JJ has some emotional and school problems, psychiatric treatment is not indicated. We advised his mother to arrange some support for JJ to make a successful transition to high school. A school counsellor would be sufficient. Mother, JJ and the school counsellor were all satisfied with this recommendation. Some remarks for the future To become a discipline no longer divided by national conventions, we must look for a common research framework. In this, several elements are important, specially the use of sound instruments, which has to be a major priority. From this point of view, we will end with a few statements: 1. International psychomotor research must be based on the use of similar instruments. Otherwise, we can never compare our interventions on an international level. 2. Many instruments are of unknown validity: we do not know exactly what they measure. This is difficult, because our outcome measures are broader than motor performance. That’s why we need instruments covering whole psychomotor domain. 3. Instruments must be, also, reliable. When observational procedures are completely idiosyncratic and stem from the experience of a single therapist, they are not yet suitable for research. 4. Instruments should be, also, informative for clinical practice. When a therapist is trained in the use of instruments, they have to experience that valuable information about the case is provided. Only in this way, this experience is successful. 5. And last but nor least, instruments must be manageable for practitioners. We don’t want an unwieldy battery of tests and procedures that burden our therapist. A second priority is establishing a research-oriented journal, which is also of clinician’s interest. We need to exchange ideas on a more frequent level. We would like to invite the scientific committee to brood on the possibilities of giving birth to such a journal. References AENDEKERK, E.W.C. & VERHEIJ, F. (1997), The psychodynamics of psychomotor therapeutic process of children with conduct disorders. In Vermeer, A., Bosscher, R.J. & Broadhead, G.D. Movement Therapy across the Lifespan. Amsterdam: VU University Press, pp 76-96 ACHENBACH, TH.M. RESCORLA, L.A. (2001), Manual for the ASEBA School-Age Forms & profiles, An Integrated system of multi-informant assessment, Aseba: Burlington. AMERICAN PSYCHIATRIC ASSOCIATION (APA) (1994), Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition (DSM-IV). Washington: APA. BARKLEY, R.A., (1998), Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a handbook for diagnosis and treatment. New York: Guilford Press. BARRAT, E.S. (1984), The biological bases of impulsiveness: The significance of timing and rhythm disorders. Personality and Individual Differences, 4(4): 387-391. BERCKELAER-ONNES, I.A. & HOEKMAN, J. (1991), Auti-R Schaal, Ten behoeve van de onderkenning van vroegkinderlijk autisme. Lisse: Swets. BOSSCHER, R.J. (1991), Running therapie bij depressie [Running therapy for depression]. Amserdam : Thesis. BRAUN, C.M.J., LAROCQUE, C., DAIGNEAULT, D., MONTOUR-PROUX, I. (1999), Mania, pseudomania, depression, and pseudodepression resulting from focal unilateral cortical lesions. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neurology, 12(1): 35-51. CALVO, M., ALAMO, L. (1988), Test anxiety and motor performance, International Journal of Psychology, 22(2): 165-178. CLOITRE, M., KATZ, M., VAN PRAAG, H.M. (1993), Psychomotor agitation and retardation, IN COSTELLO, CH.G. (ED) Symptoms of depression, pp 207-226, New York: John Wiley and Sons. EARLS, F. (1994), Oppositional-Defiant and Conduct Disorders, In: RUTTER, M., TAYLOR, E. & HERSOV, L. Child and adolescent psychiatry, p 308-330.. London: Blackwell Science. EMCK, C. (1997), Een diagnostische procedure voor psychomotorische therapie. Bewegen & Hulpverlening, 14: 127-140. EMCK, C. & SMIT, CH. (2000), Wat beweegt hen? Over adolescenten en psychomotoriek. In J.VANDEPUTTE, J.BUITELAAR, P.COHEN KETTENIS & W.MATTHYS, Uit de kinderschoenen. 60 jaar kinder en jeugd-psychiatrie UMC-Utrecht, pp112-126, Assen: Van Gorcum. FERDINANT, R.F. & VAN DER ENDE, J. (2002), Nederlandse vertaling NIMH-DISC-IV. Rotterdam: Aseba / Erasmus MC. FLAVELL, J. (1985), Cognitive development. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. HAMMINK, M.N. (2003), Psychomotorische diagnostiek binnen het kinder- en jeugdpsychiatrisch zorgveld. Maastricht: Shaker Publishers. HARDY, L., MULLEN, R., MARTINI, N. (2001), Effect of task-relevant cues and state anxiety on motor performance. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 92(3 Pt1): 943-946. HARRINGTON, R. (1994), Affective disorders. In Rutter, M., Taylor, E. & Hersov, L. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, pp 330-351, London: Blackwell Science. HARVEY, W.J. & REID,G. (1997), Motor performance of children with attention-deficithyperactivity disorder, A preliminary investigation. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 14(3):189-202. KOKOBUN, M., HAISHI, K., OKUZIMI, H., HOSOBUCHI, T., KOIKE, T. (1996), Predictive value of age of walking for later motor performance in children with mental retardation, Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 40(6): 529-534. LAMERS-WINKELMAN, F. (1997), Recognition and treatment of sexually abused children, In VERMEER, A., BOSSCHER, R.J. & BROADHEAD, G.D. Movement Therapy across the Lifespan. Amsterdam: VU University Press, pp 107-116. LEMKE, M.R., PUHL, P., KOETHE, N., WINKLER, T. (1999), Psychomotor retardation and anhedonia in depression. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavia, 99(4): 252-256. LEYTEN, C., KEMPER, H., VERSCHUUR, R. (1982), De Moper fitheidstest. Haarlem: De Vrieseborch. LOON, H. VAN, AENDEKERK, E.C.W. & VERHEIJ, F. (1997), Children with pervasive developmental disorder: a non-integrated body image. In Vermeer, A., Bosscher, R.J. & Broadhead, G.D. Movement Therapy across the Lifespan. Amsterdam: VU University Press, pp 96-107. LORD, C. & RUTTER, M. (1994), Autism and pervasive developmental disorders, In: Rutter, M., Taylor, E. & Hersov, L. Child and adolescent psychiatry, p 569-594. London: Blackwell Science. LUTEIJN, E., MINDERAA, R. & JACKSON, S. (2002), VISK Handleiding. Vragenlijst voor inventarisatie sociaal gedrag bij kinderen. Lisse: Swets. NETELENBOS, J.B. (1998), Motorische ontwikkeling van kinderen, deel 1 en 2. Amsterdam: Boom. PATTON, G.C., COFFEY, C., POSTERINO, M., CARLIN, J.B., WOLFE, R. (2000), Adolescent depressive disorder: a population-based study of ICD-10 symptoms. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 34(5): 741-747. PAYNE, R.B., CORLY, T.J. (1995), Motivational effects of anxiety on psychomotor performance. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 79(3 Pt2): 1507-1521. PIEK, J.P., PITCHER, T.M., & HAY, D.A. (1999), Motor coordination and kinasthesis in boys with attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder, Developmental Medicine and Child,Neurology, 41, 159-165. POKRAJAC, B. (1982), Usefulness of a self-report measure of anxiety in prediction of motor performance. School Psychology International, 3(4): 213-218. PRY, R., GUILLAIN, A. & PERNON, E. (2000), Regulation postural et conduits autocentrees chez les infants autiste. Enfance, 52(2), 149-167. REID, H.M. & NORVILITIS, J.M. (2000), Evidence for anomalous lateralisation across domain in ADHD children as well as adults identified with the Wender Utah rating scale. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 34, 311-316 ROSSUM, J.A. & VERMEER, A. (2000), Competentiebelevingsschaal voor kinderen; Motorische Competentiebeleving (supplement). Lisse: Swets. SHEPHARD, A.H. & ABBEY, D.S. (1958), Manifest anxiety and performance on a complex perceptualmotor task. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 8: 327-330. STRIAN, F., KLICPERA, C. (1984), Anxiety and depression in affective disorders, Psychpathology, 17(1): 37-48 TAYLOR, E. (1994), Syndromes of Attention Deficit and Overactivity, In: Rutter, M., Taylor, E. & Hersov, L. Child and adolescent psychiatry, p 285-308. London: Blackwell Science TERELAK, J. (1990), Individual differences in anxiety level and psychomotor performance, Personality and Individual Differences, 11(8): 771-775. ULRICH, D.A. (2000), Test of gross motor development (2e druk). Austin:PRO-ED. VAN DER VLIET, P., VAN COPPENOLLE, H., KNAPEN, J. (1999), Physical measures, percieved physical ability, and body acceptance of adult psychiatric patients, Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 16(2): 113-125 VEERMAN, J.W., STRAATHOF, M.A.E., TREFFERS, PH.D.A., VAN DEN BERGH, B.R.H. & TEN BRINK, L.T. (1997), Competentiebelevingsschaal voor kinderen, Lisse: Swets. VERHULST, F.C. (1994), Inleiding in de kinder-en jeugdpsychiatrie. Assen: van Gorcum. WEINBERG, R.S. & HUNT, V.V. (1976). The interrelationship between anxiety, motor performance and electromyography, Journal of Motor Behavior, 8(3): 219-224 WHITMOND, S. & CLARK, CH (1996), Kinaesthetic acuity and fine motor skills in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a preliminary report. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 38, 1091-1098