Impact of Repeated Reading on Reading Fluency, Reading Comprehension, and Reading Attitudes of Eighth-Grade Students Vickki R. Carter Valdosta State University An action research project submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements of the Education Specialist Degree in Exemplary Teaching at Valdosta State University. ABSTRACT: This study examined the impact of repeated reading on reading fluency, reading comprehension, and reading attitudes in eighth-grade students (N = 15). Regular education students were taught expository writing and guided reading using a novel for 4 ½ weeks. Repeated reading of grade-level passages for the first 10-15 minutes of class was then added to the curriculum. The AIMSweb R-CBM, AIMSweb Maze CBM, and an attitude survey were used to assess student performance. Statistically significant gains were made during the repeated reading intervention in comprehension (M = 25.53, p = *0.01). Improvements, though not statistically significant, were made in reading fluency (M = 132, p = 0.17) and in students’ attitudes toward reading (p = 0.30). Today’s society is like no other in history. Information is available at the touch of a key or the click of a mouse in the matter of a split second. However, is it really that simple? The act of reading information from the computer monitor requires two simple tasks: decoding words and comprehending the text. This notion seems as easy as placing one foot in front of the other and walking from one room to the next. However, schools are filled with students who can call, or decode words beautifully but simply do not understand what they read. Comprehension is missing. According to the results of the 2007 National Association for Educational Progress (NAEP), Georgia’s average score was 259 and the nation’s public schools average score was 261. (National Center for Education Statistics [NCES], 2007, Table 1). Critical to the quality education that children and adolescents seek is the ability to read and comprehend a variety of texts. However, the educational requirements do not simply stop at comprehension. Students must be able to respond thoughtfully to a text using inference as well as text-based response and critical response (Applegate, Applegate, & Modla, 2009). A key component of reading comprehension that researchers say is missing, especially in middle and high schools, is reading fluency (Applegate et al., 2009; Rasinski et al., 2005; Strong, Wehby, Falk, & Lane, 2004). Reading fluency is made up of three key elements: accuracy, rate, and prosody. Students should be able to read words accurately at a rate of a 1 Novus Scientia Vol. I No. 2 normal conversation with appropriate prosody, or expression, in order to gain meaning from a given text (Applegate et al., 2009). Based on information from several studies (Faver, 2008; Rasinski et al., 2005; Strong et al., 2004; Winn et al., 2006; Yurick et al., 2006), which outline the various formats for repeated reading (RR), RR is one of the most practical interventions to use with a variety of age ranges. In his book, The Fluent Reader: Oral Reading Strategies for Building Word Recognition, Fluency, and Comprehension (2003), Rasinski described RR as a powerful tool that, when practiced regularly and in interesting ways, lead to vast improvements in students’ word recognition, fluency, and comprehension. He also implied that RR is not used nearly enough in elementary and middle-grade classrooms. Rasinski made an analogy comparing repeated reading to basketball players practicing foul and jump shots on a daily basis or a pianist practicing scales to become a more proficient musician. Therefore, according to Rasinski, a reader who can automatically decode words can read with greater speed, accuracy, and comprehension. The purpose of this study was to test the impact of the RR strategy on the reading fluency, reading comprehension, and reading attitudes of eighth-grade students at a middle school in Southeast Georgia. Methods Participants in this study lived in a small, rural town in Southeast Georgia. According to the school improvement plan, the community is among Georgia’s poorest with a per capita yearly income of $22,031. The population of the county was approximately 17,900 and growing at a rate of approximately 14% from April 2000 through April 2008. At the end of the 2007-2008 school year, 766 students were enrolled in this middle school. The White population made up 83% of the total school population with the African-American population at 10%, the Hispanic population at 4%, and 3% other. The school improvement plan also reported that one of the factors impacting the community was that approximately 30% of the adult population had less than a high school education, and only approximately 16% of the adult population had some college education. Eighth-grade students in an Extended Learning Time (ELT) class, including 7 males and 8 females, participated in this study. All students were assigned to the teacher-researcher’s class by administrators at the beginning of the school year based on low CRCT scores in Reading from the previous school year and/or low scores on either a reading curriculum-based measurement (R-CBM) or a comprehension curriculum-based measurement (Maze CBM) (AIMSweb Progress Monitoring and RTI System, 2008). None of the students in this convenience sample had an Individual Education Plan (IEP). Intervention Because of the availability of only one group, a within-group/ time-series design was used with students in teacher-researcher’s ELT class, which met for approximately 1 hour each day. The control phase of the study took place during the first 4 ½ weeks of school. During this phase, students participated in expository writing instruction and group reading with discussion of Out of the Dust, a poem/novel by Karen Hesse, written at a grade-level equivalent of 5.1 (Scholastic, 2009). At the beginning and end of this phase, the teacher-researcher and the school’s Response to Intervention (RTI) coordinator administered the R-CBM, the Maze-CBM, and the Reading Attitudes Survey (Appendix A). The results were used as baseline data. The experimental phase of the study took place during weeks 4 ½ through 9 of the school year. During this phase, students continued participation in expository writing instruction and participated in a group reading and discussion of the novel My Brother Sam is Dead by James 2 Novus Scientia Vol. I No. 2 Lincoln Collier and Christopher Collier, which is written at a 5.7 grade-level equivalent (Scholastic, 2009). The students spent the first 10 to 15 minutes of class participating in RR. At the completion of the 4½ week intervention phase, the teacher-researcher and the school’s RTI coordinator administered the R-CBM, the Maze-CBM, and the Reading Attitudes Survey (Appendix A). At the beginning of each ELT class period, students were responsible for obtaining their repeated reading folders from the designated shelf in the classroom. Each folder included a table for students to record fluency rates each week (Appendix B) and two eighth-grade level reading passages for use during the week. Two new reading passages were added each week. On the first day of each week, the teacher introduced a new passage by reading it aloud to students as they read along silently from their copy. Next, students paired up, taking turns reading the same passage aloud at least twice to each other. During this time, the teacher-researcher and ESOL teacher monitored each pair of students while making field notes in a journal. On the second day of each week, students went back to the same partner, taking turns reading aloud the day 1 passage to one another at least three times. Again, the teacher-researcher and ESOL teacher monitored students while making field notes. The same process for days 1 and 2 was followed with a different passage for days 3 and 4. On day 5 of each week, each student selected his or her favorite passage from the week. After reading the passage aloud to the same partner one time, students conducted a mock RCBM on one another and recorded their own scores on the table located inside their RR folders. Students had been previously trained by the teacher-researcher to conduct an R-CBM. At the end of the 4 ½ week experimental phase, the teacher-researcher and the RTI Coordinator conducted the AIMSweb R-CBM and the Maze CBM as posttests for each participant. The Reading Attitudes Survey was also conducted at the end of the 4 ½ week experimental phase. Results During the time of year of administration, eighth-graders’ scores on the R-CBM should average 143 words correct per minute, and scores on the Maze-CBM should average 20 items correct (AIMSweb Progress Monitoring and RTI System, 2008). Table 1 Comparison of R-CBM and Maze CBM Mean Gains from Control and Intervention Phases Control Outcomes R-CBM Mean Gain 8.33 Maze CBM 2.06 Intervention SD SD N t-value p 12.30 Mean Gain 9.80 10.54 15 -0.35 0.36 5.31 7.13 5.24 15 -2.63 0.01* * p < .05. 3 Novus Scientia Vol. I No. 2 Mean gains and standard deviations along with t-test results from the control phase and intervention phase are given in Table 1. Although the gains in reading fluency were higher with the intervention than with no intervention, the results were not statistically significant (p > .05) over this period of time. In reading comprehension, the gains under the intervention were significantly higher (p < .05) than the gains under no intervention. The Reading Attitudes Survey was given to students prior to the repeated reading intervention and after the repeated reading intervention. After the intervention, students’ attitudes toward reading shifted positively in six areas of the eight measured on the survey. Student responses on the survey were similar on most survey items, indicating that there was not a significant shift in most students’ attitudes toward reading. There were two survey items where the responses were more positive than before the intervention, and where the differences approached significance. Those two items related to the excitement experienced when the teacher recommended a good book (Question 1) and getting so involved in reading a book it was difficult to put it down (Question 4). Although overall improvements in attitude toward reading were not statistically significant (p > .05), a more positive shift was reported regarding confidence in comprehension when reading alone (Question 6) and in attitudes toward reading at home (Question 8). Field notes were also used for data collection during the intervention phase. Students were randomly observed by the teacher and the ESOL teacher during repeated reading sessions. Discussion The RR intervention was beneficial to the students. The results of the current study are consistent with the findings from the Winn study (Winn et al., 2006) with adult subjects whose results were statistically significant. However, the Winn study differed from the current research in that it took place over a period of 5 months, as contrasted with a 4 ½ week intervention period. In addition, the current study reflected improvement similar to the Strong study (Strong et al., 2004) in which subjects utilized the repeated reading intervention along with an empirically valid reading program for a period of 5 weeks. Although conducted over a 4 month period in contrast to the 4 ½ week period of the current study, the results from the Yurick study (Yurick et al., 2006) are also consistent with the current study in the areas of reading fluency and reading comprehension. Findings from both studies are supported by conclusions from the Yurick study (2006) in which researchers noted that because students were able to focus more attention on reading and give less attention to decoding, they were able to gain more meaning from the text. Factors that Influenced Implementation During the first week of the intervention, 9 of the 15 participants were absent for a 2 days due to an outbreak of influenza in the school. During the third week of intervention, the teacher-researcher was out of the classroom for one day for a meeting; however, the ESOL teacher was present during class. The third factor was the fact that the hallways were being decorated for homecoming activities during the final week of implementation and the day the final R-CBM was administered, causing the teachers to note distractions among several students. Findings from this study affirm the credibility of previous research studies in relation to the positive impact of repeated reading on reading fluency and reading comprehension. The practice of reading accurately through repeated reading allows students to improve their rate of reading and word recognition skills causing them to spend less time decoding words. As a result, students gain more meaning from the text. In addition, RR provides a viable, inexpensive intervention to help students increase their fluency and comprehension without the purchase of 4 Novus Scientia Vol. I No. 2 an expensive program. Any text of any length can be used to practice repeated reading. Despite the positive findings from this study, a few limitations should be addressed. Because the participants were students of the researcher, teacher bias may have made an impact on the students’ achievement. Other factors that could have influenced the results were the small class size, the presence of two teachers, and the impact of reading instruction received at other times of the day through language arts. The brevity of the study (4 ½ weeks) also limited the results. Clearly, further research is required to validate the findings of this study. Although the use of repeated reading is well-documented in previous research literature, a larger sample of eighth-grade students from various school settings and demographics would serve to further evaluate the effectiveness of this strategy. Further research should be conducted on the repeated reading strategy using a variety of text types and genres as opposed to the one-page informational passages provided in this study. 5 Novus Scientia Vol. I No. 2 References AIMSweb Progress Monitoring and RTI System (2008). Reading-CBM. Retrieved June 29, 2009, from http://www.aimsweb.com Applegate, M. D., Applegate, A. J., & Modla, V. B. (2009). “She’s my best reader; She just can’t comprehend”: Studying the relationship between fluency and comprehension. Newark, DE: International Reading Association. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. EJ833705) Faver, S. (2008, December). Repeated reading of poetry can enhance reading fluency. Newark, DE: International Reading Association. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. EJ820651) National Center for Education Statistics (2007). The nation’s report card: Reading 2007 state snapshot report [White paper]. Retrieved February 7, 2009, from http://nces.ed.gov/nationsreportcard/pdf/stt2007/2007497GA8.pdf Rasinski, T. V. (2003). Repeated reading. In The fluent reader: Oral reading strategies for building word recognition, fluency, and comprehension (pp. 75-100). New York: Scholastic. Rasinski, T. V., Padak, N. D., McKeon, C. A., Wilfong, L. G., Friedauer, J. A., & Heim, P. (2005, September). Is reading fluency a key for successful high school reading? Newark, DE: International Reading Association. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. EJ718844) Scholastic (2009). My brother Sam is dead. Retrieved July 6, 2009, from http://www2.scholastic.com Scholastic (2009). Out of the dust. Retrieved July 6, 2009, from http://www2.scholastic.com Strong, A. C., Wehby, J. H., Falk, K. B., & Lane, K. L. (2004). The impact of a structured reading curriculum and repeated reading on the performance of junior high students with emotional and behavioral disorders. Bethesda, MD: National Association of School Psychologists. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. EJ683549) Winn, B. D., Skinner, C. H., Oliver, R., Hale, A. D., & Ziegler, M. (2006, November). The effects of listening while reading and repeated reading on the reading fluency of adult learners. Newark, DE: International Reading Association. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. EJ750751) Yurick, A. L., Robinson, P. D., Cartledge, G., Lo, Y., & Evans, T. L. (2006, August). Using peer-mediated repeated readings as a fluency-building activity for urban learners. Morgantown, WV: West Virginia University Press. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. EJ744818) 6 Novus Scientia Vol. I No. 2 Appendix A Reading Attitudes Survey Thank you for your participation in this voluntary survey. Your completion of this survey lets me know you agree to participate. Using the following 1-5 scale, please circle the number that best represents how you feel about the statement. 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 3 Neutral 4 Agree 5 Strongly Agree 1. I get excited when the teacher recommends a good book. 1 2 3 4 5 2. I can read just as well as my classmates. 1 2 3 4 5 3. Knowing how to read well is important. 1 2 3 4 5 4. Once I start a book, I can’t put it down. 1 2 3 4 5 5. When my teacher asks me a question about what I have read, I can give an accurate answer. 1 2 3 4 5 6. When I am reading by myself, I understand what I am reading. 1 2 3 4 5 7. I like to read out loud in class. 1 2 3 4 5 8. Reading a book at home is something I like to do. 1 2 3 4 5 Appendix B Student # ______ 7 Novus Scientia Vol. I No. 2 Weekly Fluency Rates Week # WCPM (Words Correct Per Minute) Student Reflections Week 1 Week 2 Week 3 Week 4 Notes: Appendix C Teacher Field Notes Action Research Field Notes Week: _______ Monday: Tuesday: Wednesday: Thursday: 8 Novus Scientia Vol. I No. 2 Friday: 9 Novus Scientia Vol. I No. 2