Disgust: Sensory Affect or Primary Emotional System? Disgust: Sensory Affect or Primary Emotional System? Cognition & Emotion, 2007, in press Judith A. Toronchuk Psychology and Biology Departments, Trinity Western University .and George F. R. Ellis Mathematics Department, University of Cape Town Running Head: Disgust: Sensory Affect or Primary Emotional System? Contact information: Psychology Department, Trinity Western University 7600 Glover Road, Langley, B.C. V2Y 1Y1 Canada. Phone: 604-888-7511 extension 3104 email address: toronchu@twu.ca 1 Disgust: Sensory Affect or Primary Emotional System? 2 Abstract We argue in this paper for the inclusion in the primary emotional systems enumerated by Panksepp of a neural system which organises disgust responses. The DISGUST system arose phylogenetically in response to danger to the internal milieu from pathogens and their toxic products. We suggest that the primitive emotive circuit which originally provided defence by regulating consummatory behaviours gave rise to a primary emotional system which facilitates evaluation of reinforcers. Unlike the sensory affect of distaste from which it is experimentally dissociable, disgust responses can involve flexible learned components triggered by several modalities. The anterior insula is implicated as playing a major role in the DISGUST system both in organizing disgust responses in the individual and recognizing disgust responses in others. Disgust: Sensory Affect or Primary Emotional System? 3 Introduction In this paper we present arguments for the inclusion of DISGUST 1 as a primary emotional system, to be included in the list of such systems in addition to those characterised as such by Panksepp (1998). Although numerous authors have established lists of criteria for characteristics of basic emotions, Panksepp’s criteria are particularly valuable in calling attention to the phylogeny of neural pathways. Using his differentiation between sensory affect and primary emotional system, we wish to argue that the complex, action-oriented affective response of the DISGUST system should be distinguished from the sensory affect of “distaste”. According to Panksepp (1998, p.48ff; see also Panksepp, 2000) six neurally-based criteria for primary emotional systems are: 1) genetically predetermined circuits accessible to various sensory stimuli, 2) the ability to organize diverse behaviours, 3) the ability to change the sensitivities of relevant sensory systems, 4) the use of feedback circuits to sustain arousal which outlasts precipitating events, 5) modulation by cognitive inputs, and 6) modification and channelling of cognitive abilities. In this scheme, a sensory affect is a sensation infused with affective qualities and more related to perception than to emotion, while primary emotions are action-orientated responses arising from “distinct emotional operating systems that are concentrated in subneocortical, limbic regions of the brain” (Panksepp, 2005). While distaste seems to us to belong to the former category, we suggest the DISGUST response belongs to the latter category and is a phylogenetic development arising from mechanisms which originally functioned to prevent disease. We support this claim below by arguing in turn for its phylogenetically ancient origins, activation by diverse sensory modalities, ability to organize diverse behaviours, distinct neural Disgust: Sensory Affect or Primary Emotional System? 4 circuitry, ability to produce changes in hedonic value, arousal which outlasts precipitating events, and modification and channelling of cognitive abilities Phylogenetically ancient origins While psychologists have long agreed in principle with the idea that emotions have evolutionary origins and facilitate adaptive actions, most emotion researchers (Panksepp is a refreshing exception) begin with human subjective experience and then search for ad hoc supporting data from the mammalian order. In our thinking a wider phylogenetic approach to the study of emotions such as that described by Lawrence and Calder (2004, p. 16) is appropriate, except that we would not simply advocate a mammalian perspective, but one which considers the entirety of vertebrate evolution. We propose that the basic emotion referred to in humans as disgust evolved from the reflexive distaste response of vertebrate ancestors, which in turn arose from primitive chemosensory mechanisms originally adapted to avoid pathogens and their toxins. Whether or not the use of the word `disgust’ should be limited to humans is not our concern in this paper; rather our purpose is to explicate the evolution of a basic neural operating system (DISGUST) which enables emotional responses to potentially infectious, or noxious material, in advance of actual contact with such material. In humans the basic emotion of disgust comes to be elaborated by higher cortical processing not available to other organisms to include social status and moral concerns. Natural selection requires that all organisms, no matter how simple, defend their bodies against others. The problem of defence requires two basic systems: one to defend against external threat and one to defend the internal milieu from toxins (Garcia, Disgust: Sensory Affect or Primary Emotional System? 5 Hankins, & Rusiniak, 1974; Garcia, Quick, & White, 1984, p. 49). All organisms must protect themselves not only from external predators but also from invasive microorganisms and parasites within their own bodies as well as ingested toxic substances. The learned avoidance of toxic or infected material before ingestion, as opposed to spitting or vomiting after ingestion, would thus serve a useful adaptive function. Throughout evolution danger to the internal milieu has been signalled by chemosensory detection mechanisms found in all multicellular animals. Even sponges and coelenterates, with rather limited behavioural repertoires, have within their bodies wandering phagocytic amoebocytes which play a dual role in both nutrition and defence from pathogens (Horton & Ratcliffe, 2001, p. 211). The avoidance of tastes previously paired with noxious substances or illness, first described by Garcia, has since been demonstrated in a wide range of mammals, birds, reptiles, fish, and various invertebrates (see Bernstein, 1999; Carew & Sahley, 1986; Garcia et al., 1974; Garcia et al., 1984). Even the bacteria-ingesting nematode Caenorhabditis elegans exhibits a chemoreceptor mediated response in which it selectively avoids in a radial maze specific pathogenic strains after 4 hrs. exposure (Zhang, Lu, & Bargmann, 2005). Garden slugs which previously ingested carrot juice will avoid it after one or two trials in which the juice is followed by poison (Garcia et al., 1984, p.48). Pond snails acquire differential conditioned taste aversion to either sucrose or carrot juice paired with lithium chloride (LiCl; Sugai, Shiga, Azami, Watanabe, Sadamoto, Fujito, et al., 2006). While we do not suggest that these invertebrate examples represent the emotion of disgust, we do wish to point out that they are the evolutionary precursors of the fully developed human emotion of disgust. Disgust: Sensory Affect or Primary Emotional System? 6 Rozin and his colleagues have suggested the phylogenetic origin of disgust lies in the distaste/oral rejection response of animals to bad tasting food (Rozin & Fallon 1987; Rozin, Haidt, & McCauley, 1999). They hold the specifically human emotion of disgust differs from the sensory distaste response of animals by the addition of concepts of offensiveness, in particular those aroused by reminders of our animal nature. Further human elaborations would include interpersonal and socio-moral disgust. More recently Curtis and Biran (2001) have argued for evolutionary origins in more general protection of organisms from infection. This latter theory is supported by the results of a massive international survey (Curtis, Aunger, & Rabie, 2004) showing that disgust is universally elicited by disease-salient contact stimuli such as bodily secretions, viscous substances, vermin and sick or dirty people. A recent study on odour also supports the disease model (Stevenson & Repacholi, 2005). Rubio-Godoy, Aunger and Curtis (2007) further suggest disgust is a neural system “evolved to detect reliable signals co-occurring with diseasecausing infectious agents, which stimulates avoidance responses and/or other behaviours that tend to decrease the risk of disease.” We propose touch, olfaction and taste were all involved in the evolutionary development of the DISGUST system as early aquatic vertebrates likely had in common with many modern fish wide-spread chemoreceptors on their body surface, an adaptation which allows not only avoidance of ingestion but even earlier avoidance of contact with infectious or noxious substances. Activation by diverse sensory modalities The first of Panksepp’s original criteria for an emotional system is that it be controlled by a distinct neural circuitry unconditionally accessed by various sensory stimuli (Panksepp, 1998, p. 48). Although taste may be the phylogenetically first, and Disgust: Sensory Affect or Primary Emotional System? 7 thereafter most prominent cue for aversion responses, other sensations can also be effective triggers. In humans taste, olfactory, auditory, tactile and visual cues are all capable of eliciting disgust (Curtis & Biran, 2001; Curtis et al., 2004). In mammals that hold food in their front paws, aversions can develop to tactile and olfactory features of the food (see Domjan, 2005). Visual cues such as the colour of monarch butterflies may be associated by birds with illness; and according to Garcia’s group (1984, p. 57), avians which eat relatively tasteless seeds also make excellent colour-illness associations, unlike mammals which tend to make primarily taste-illness associations. Using colour cues, birds avoid seeds associated with illness under conditions in which taste cues would be less useful. Miller (1997, p.169) notes that although the word disgust did not enter English until the 17th century along with the aesthetic notion of “good taste”, its etymological origin has likely biased English-speaking researchers to focus on the sensation of taste. The lack of gustatory connotations in German Ekel and widerlich may have inclined Freud to focus more on other sensory modalities (W. Miller, 1997 p.1; also S. Miller, 2004, p.12). Miller, rejecting a sole origin for disgust in taste elaborates, “Touch is the world of the slimy, slithery, viscous, oozing, festering, scabby, sticky, and moist” (W. Miller, p.19). Avoiding contact with infectious substances provides greater safety than merely avoiding ingestion. The selective activation of the DISGUST system (Garcia et al., 1974; Rozin & Kalat, 1971) by only certain stimulus categories may be similar to “belongingness” or “preparedness” of fear responses. Öhman and Mineka (2001) argue from the “preparedness” of humans to fear certain stimuli such as snakes, spiders and angry faces Disgust: Sensory Affect or Primary Emotional System? 8 for the existence of an evolutionarily adapted fear system that organizes human fear and fear learning. This system would respond more easily to stimuli which were important for survival in the environment of our ancestors than to modern dangers such as guns; and it would influence the ease with which fear associations are learned. The parallel manner in which tastes seem prepared to activate disgust and the ease with which disgust may be associated with taste suggests a similar argument for an evolutionarily adapted DISGUST system. Garcia and his colleagues (1984, p.51) have shown the visual appearance of a food pellet can be associated by rats with shock but not with illness, whereas flavour can be easily associated with illness but not with shock. The effectiveness of odour as a stimulus for rats depends on the context. Odour alone is a good cue for association with shock but not with illness, whereas odour plus taste can be easily associated with illness but not with shock. When odour has been paired with taste during acquisition, odour alone can then become a potent cue for aversion. It is commonly accepted that humans also readily associate tastes with nausea (see Bernstein, 1999; Rozin & Fallon, 1987). Ability to organize diverse behaviours Responses of the DISGUST system can involve flexible voluntary components, thereby satisfying another of the criteria for basic emotional systems (Panksepp, 1998, p.49). Rats do not just avoid food items after taste aversive conditioning—they gape, open their mouths, gag and retch, shake their heads, and wipe their chins on the floor. A coyote may retch, roll on the offensive food and then kick dirt on it, while a cougar may shake each paw. Monkeys may react to offensive objects by excessive sniffing and manipulation often followed by breaking and squashing the item, then dropping or Disgust: Sensory Affect or Primary Emotional System? 9 flinging it away and wiping their hands (Garcia et al., 1984, p.57). The complexity of these responses suggests they involve more than reflexes. Certain immune and endocrine responses may also be activated as part of the DISGUST system. Pairing of saccharine and a bacterial antigen can induce a conditioned taste aversion (CTA) in rats along with a conditioned increase in both immune function (interleukin-2 and interferon-γ) and corticosterone (Pacheco-López, Niemi, Kou, Härting, Del Rey, Besedovsky, et al., 2004; also Alvarez-Borda, Ramirez-Amaya, Perez-Montfort, & Bermudez-Rattoni, F. 1995; Ramirez-Amaya & Burmudez-Rattoni, 1999). This lends credence to Curtis’ disease theory, especially in light of the fact that it is the immune system that actually triggers malaise, fever, and other sickness behaviours. Although “each species reacts with its species-specific disgust pattern” (Garcia et al., 1984, p.57), individual organisms vary the pattern according to the external circumstances. An incident from the Garcia lab illustrates the extent of diversity during a procedure designed to produce conditioned taste aversion in rats (Garcia et al., 1984, p. 53). Animals were trained to drink from a tube protruding from a nose cone covered with filter paper impregnated with initially neutral odours. After becoming ill from LiCl paired with a novel odour and a novel taste in the water, rats later avoided plain water when it was presented with the conditioned odour. Garcia observed some of these rats claw out the filter paper containing this apparently offensive odour, push the paper through slots in the floor at the other side of the cage, and only then drink the water. While humans also give complex voluntary disgust responses, some behavioural components do have an involuntary nature. Disgust elicits specific autonomic responses in humans including reduced blood pressure, heart rate deceleration, increased skin Disgust: Sensory Affect or Primary Emotional System? 10 conductance (Stark, Walter, Schienle, & Vaitl, 2005; see also Critchley, Rotshtein, Nagai, O'Doherty, Mathias, & Dolan, 2005; Rozin & Fallon, 1987), and changes in respiration (Ritz, Thons, Fahrenkrug, & Dahme, 2005). The autonomic components may also be generated by voluntarily produced facial expressions of disgust (Levenson, Ekman, & Friesen, 1990). Disgust also elicits a unique, fairly universally recognized, facial expression (Ekman & Friesen, 1986; see also Wolf, Mass, Ingenbleek, Kiefer, Naber, & Wiedemann, 2005) which is even elicited in congenitally blind individuals (Galati, Scherer, & Ricci-Bitti, 1997) and correctly interpreted by children born deaf (Hosie, Gray, Russell, Scott, & Hunter, 1998). Some researchers (e.g. Ekman) have used the existence of a universal facial expression as one criterion for inclusion as a basic emotion. However because components of this particular expression can also be elicited in anencephalic neonates (see Steiner, Glaser, Hawilo, & Berridge 2001), it does not seem likely that the facial expression differentiates between the emotion of disgust and the sensory affect of distaste. While suggesting the existence of an innate mechanism, facial expression does not make an adequate criterion for an emotion. Distinct neural circuitry The existence across species of common neural structures involved in triggering disgust reactions suggests a unified affective system with ancient origins which has undergone recent elaboration in humans. Both the human disgust response and conditioned taste aversions in animals depend on allocortical regions of the insula consistent with the suggestion that primary emotions arise from emotional operating systems concentrated in subneocortical, limbic regions of the brain (Panksepp, 2005). Disgust: Sensory Affect or Primary Emotional System? 11 In mammals reflexive vomiting and retching is organized by central pattern generators in the medulla under the control of the nucleus tractus solitarius (NTS) (see Hornby, 2001; Lawes, 1990). There is however no single “vomiting centre” that may be activated by electrical stimulation, suggesting that emesis, like feeding, is under multiple levels of neural control (Lawes, 1990). . Caudal NTS receives visceral and immune system input from the vagus (X th) nerve and input regarding blood-borne substances from area postrema, while the rostral NTS receives taste input from cranial nerves VII, IX and X. Taste and interoceptive inputs enter separate areas of NTS forming two pathways which first come together in the insula (reviewed in Bermudez-Rattoni, 2004; Craig, 2002; Saper, 2002; Spector, 2000). Some differences between primates and other mammals are evident in the inputs to the insula which Craig (2002, 2005) argues allow the development of a greater degree of self-awareness in primates. In rodents both taste and visceral input is relayed via the pons to the thalamus and then to the insula (see Saper, 2002). Visceral input reaches the amygdala via the pons (see Bermudez-Rattoni, 2004). Formation of a CTA in rats requires both the anterior insula (AI) and the amygdala to be intact (Lasiter, Deems, Oetting, & Garcia, 1985; Bermudez-Rattoni, Grijalva, Kiefer, & Garcia, 1986). However decorticate rats still prefer sucrose and reject quinine (Kiefer & Orr, 1992) consistent with the idea that distaste is a sensory affect requiring only lower levels of the brainstem, while the DISGUST programme requires subneocortical limbic structures. The insula appears to be required for rodents to recognize the taste as novel (Berman, Hazvi, Neduva, & Dudai, 2000), while the amygdala signals the aversive visceral components of the nausea/illness (Miranda, Ferreira, Ramirez-Lugo, & Bermudez-Rattoni, 2002). Lesions in the insula not only Disgust: Sensory Affect or Primary Emotional System? 12 disrupt acquisition and evocation of CTA, but also of immune enhancement or suppression conditioned by either taste or olfactory cues, while leaving normal immune functioning intact (Ramirez-Amaya & Bermudez-Rattoni, 1999; Ramirez-Amaya, Alvarez-Borda, & Bermudez-Rattoni, 1998). In primates the input to AI is more direct than in rats and the amygdala appears to receive both taste and visceral input from the insula (Mesulam & Mufson, 1982; Mufson, Mesulam, & Pandya, 1981), rather than through subcortical connections from NTS as in rodents (see Rolls, 1989). Craig (2002, 2005) has proposed that this more direct pathway allows for greater cortical integration in humans of visceral components. The insula itself has three cytoarchitectural regions—agranular (named for its lack of layered granular cells), dysgranular and granular. The anterior insula, consisting of agranular and the anterior portion of the dysgranular regions, developed phylogenetically as a centre for processing exteroceptive chemosensory input. More posterior dysgranular and granular regions act as visceral sensory cortex (Saper, 2002). Although the cytoarchitecture of the insula in humans and other primates seems comparable (see Flynn, Benson, & Ardila, 1999), the human insula may be involved in a wider range of emotional behaviours as suggested by its richer interconnections to orbitofrontal cortex. The primate agranular AI is relatively undifferentiated allocortex (rather than neocortex) containing two or three layers which are continuous with agranular orbitofrontal cortex (see Flynn et al., 1999; Gottfried & Zald, 2005). This region, like the adjacent orbitofrontal region, receives olfactory input and responds to olfactory stimuli (Critchley & Rolls, 1996). In humans the homologous olfactory area is further rostral in Disgust: Sensory Affect or Primary Emotional System? 13 the orbitofrontal cortex (Gottfried & Zald, 2005), although the agranular AI is also activated by olfaction and taste (de Araujo, Rolls, Kringelbach, McGlone, & Phillips, 2003). Dorsal and caudal to the agranular area is the five or six-layered dysgranular region which is intermediate in appearance between agranular allocortex and fully formed mammalian isocortex (neocortex). In rodents the taste area is found here (Cechetto & Saper, 1987) and electrical stimulation of dysgranular insula produces gastric motility (Yasui, Breder, Saper, & Cechetto, 1991). Monkey neurons in AI respond to taste, olfaction, vision, texture or temperature of food (see Rolls, 2005a). Human neuroimaging studies show that AI is involved in coding taste sensation, olfaction, the reward value of taste (Royet, Plailly, Delon-Martin, Kareken, & Segebarth, 2003; Small, Gregory, Mak, Gitelman, Mesulam, & Parrish, 2003; Small, Zatorre, Dagher, Evans, & Jones-Gotman, 2001), and participates in more comprehensive mapping of other aspects of interoceptive bodily states (see Saper, 2002). For example AI is activated by conscious awareness of: thirst (Egan, Silk, Zamarripa, Williams, Federico, Cunnington et al., 2003), hunger (Tataranni, Gautier, Chen, Uecker, Bandy, Salbe et al., 1999), sexual arousal (Stoléru, Grégoire, Gérard, Decety, Lafarge, Cinotti et al., 1999), and heartbeat (Critchley, Wiens, Rotshtein, Öhman, & Dolan, 2004). The posterior insula consists of granular six-layered isocortex which receives primarily somatosensory and enteroceptive inputs. In rats neurons responding to gastrointestinal sensation are found just caudal to and overlapping with the taste area, and a few cells respond to both taste and gastrointestinal sensation (Cechetto & Saper, 1987). Imaging studies show the human posterior insula responds to visceral and muscle sensations along with pain, itch, temperature, hunger, thirst, etc. (see Craig, 2002; 2003). Disgust: Sensory Affect or Primary Emotional System? 14 Lesions of human AI impair both the experience of disgust and the recognition of disgust in others (Calder, Keane, Manes, Antoun, & Young, 2000). Penfield found that stimulation of insula in humans invokes unpleasant tastes, nausea, and gastrointestinal sensations (cited in Flynn et al., 1999). Recordings with depth electrodes from ventral AI (agranular and anterior dysgranular) produced responses to pictures depicting facial expressions of disgust but not to other emotional expressions (Krolak-Salmon, Henaff, Isnard, Tallon-Baudry, Guenot, Vighetto, et al., 2003). Stimulation through these electrodes produced unpleasant sensations in the throat, mouth, lips and nose described by one patient as “difficult to stand”. Numerous imaging studies provide evidence for the role of AI in elicitation of disgust either by odours or visual stimulation with disgusting objects, and also in recognition of disgust in facial expressions of others (Hennenlotter, Schroeder, Erhard, Haslinger, Stahl, Weindl, et al., 2004; Phillips, Young, Senior, Brammer, Andrew, Calder et al., 1997; Schienle, Stark, Walter, Blecker, Ott, Kirsch et al., 2002; Shapira, Liu, He, Bradley, Lessig, James, et al., 2003; Wicker, Keysers, Plailly, Royet, Gallese, & Rizzolatti, 2003). However the insula is also activated during other negative emotions such as fear, anger or sadness (Damasio, Grabowski, Bechara, Damasio, Ponto, Parvizi et al., 2000; Lane, Reiman, Ahern, Schwartz, & Davidson, 1997; Liotti, Mayberg, Brannan, McGinnis, Jerabek, & Fox, 2000; Morris, Friston, Buchel, Frith, Young, Calder et al, 1998; Schienle et al., 2002). Although AI is clearly involved in human disgust, other regions of the forebrain are also implicated in the DISGUST system. Not surprisingly given the role of the amygdala in rodent CTA and its anatomical proximity to and rich connections with the Disgust: Sensory Affect or Primary Emotional System? 15 insula, several imaging studies have reported activation of the human amygdala by both pleasant and unpleasant tastes (O’Doherty, Rolls, Francis, Bowtell, & McGlone, 2001) and also during evocation of disgust (Phillips et al., 1997; Schafer, Schienle, & Vaitl, 2005; Schienle et al., 2002; Schienle, Schafer, Stark, Walter & Vaitl, 2005). Studies of Huntington’s, Parkinson’s and obsessive compulsive patients also suggest a possible role for the basal ganglia in the DISGUST system (Sprengelmeyer, Young, Calder, Karnat, Lange, Hömberg et al., 1996; Sprengelmeyer, Young, Mahn, Schroeder, Woitalla, Büttner et al., 2003; Sprengelmeyer, Young, Pundt, Sprengelmeyer, Calder, Berrios et al., 1997). The AI is therefore only one region involved in the processing of human disgust; it is apparently a necessary region, but one which is also involved in other negative emotions. Other functions of the human insula will be considered below. Changes in hedonic value The DISGUST system is able to produce changes in the hedonic value of tastes consistent with the third criterion outlined above (Panksepp, 1998, p. 49; Panksepp, 2000). CTA can be elicited by tastes that would otherwise be pleasant, showing that CTA is more than just distaste. CTA, as further discussed below, can be dissociated from avoidance and can also be elicited by tastes that would otherwise be pleasant. This suggests a change occurs in the hedonic value of the taste. Pelchat, Grill, Rozin and Jacobs (1983) trained groups of rats to avoid sucrose either when paired with illness, shock or lactose (rats are lactose intolerant). All groups learned to avoid sucrose, but only those in which sucrose was paired with illness showed the orofacial aversion response (gaping) indicating the preferred taste actually became aversive. Disgust: Sensory Affect or Primary Emotional System? 16 Experiments by Kiefer and Orr (1992) confirm that CTA involves changes in hedonic value. Lesions in the insular taste area (agranular and dysgranular) do not lead to a failure of rats to dislike quinine or to like sucrose, but do lead to impaired learning of taste avoidance accompanied by lack of aversive reaction to a taste paired with illness. This suggests that mere avoidance does not entail a palatability shift whereas the insular taste area is necessary for the shift in palatability that occurs in learned taste aversions. Furthermore electrical stimulation of insula in anesthetised rats can modulate both positively and negatively neural taste responses in the parabrachial nucleus (PBN) of the pons (Lundy & Norgren, 2004), showing that there is a mechanism located in the insula for downward modulation of gustatory responses. When a novel taste is paired with LiCl, c-Fos expression is greater in the NTS, PBN, amygdala and insula than when a familiar taste is paired with LiCl, whereas a novel taste alone only increases expression in the amygdala and insula (Koh & Bernstein, 2005). Lesioning the insula eliminates the differences shown by rats exposed to these three taste conditions (novel taste + LiCl, familiar taste + LiCl, novel taste alone) suggesting changes in hedonic value are influenced by the insula. The insula modulates both PBN and NTS, providing a mechanism whereby illness can affect the response of these lower nuclei to taste (see Burmudez-Rattoni, 2004). In humans an fMRI study by Small and colleagues (2003) showed that although the amygdala and mid-insula respond to taste intensity rather than valence, the anterior insula responds to valence regardless of intensity. In this study the left AI was preferentially activated by unpleasant tastes. The dissociation of valence from intensity Disgust: Sensory Affect or Primary Emotional System? 17 and of pleasant from unpleasant is consistent with the notion that separate neural substrates underlie the pleasure and disgust systems. Arousal that outlasts precipitating events As discussed above the insula provides feedback to lower taste centres producing changes in hedonic value which appear to outlast the immediate effects of illness. The DISGUST system organizes the long-term adaptive responses of conditioned aversions and immune alterations, thereby fulfilling Panksepp’s (1998) criterion that a basic emotion have consequences which outlast the initiating stimuli. In humans the emotion of disgust is accompanied by autonomic changes (Levenson, et al., 1990) and autonomic changes tend to be longer-lasting than the sensory stimuli which trigger their production. The human insula is also activated by self-induced disgust generated by recall and reexperience of salient personal situations (Fitzgerald, Posse, Moore, Tancer, Nathan & Phan, 2004) indicating that disgust and its neural correlates can occur without the presence of external stimuli. Modification and channelling of cognitive abilities Long-lasting behavioural plasticity is a characteristic not found in a reflexive response such as distaste, although it is seen in both primary and secondary emotions. While distaste is a sensory affect and may give rise to reflexive responses, responses of the DISGUST emotional system give rise to primary emotions which may be affected by learning. In humans the blending of primary DISGUST and complex cognitions gives rise to secondary emotions such as moral disgust. Even though some tastes (e.g. quinine) are intrinsically noxious giving rise to distaste, activation of the DISGUST system usually involves learning. Furthermore a Disgust: Sensory Affect or Primary Emotional System? 18 taste need not have negative hedonic value to produce disgust, as shown by the ease with which CTA can be elicited to sweet tastes paired with illness. This again supports the notion that the disgust response of humans arose not specifically as a reaction to the hedonic properties of taste but as a reaction to cues associated with increased likelihood of illness (see Curtis & Biran, 2001; Curtis et al., 2004). In rats CTAs may form when taste is associated with nausea either due to gastrointestinal toxins (LiCl, nicotine etc.), X-ray, or body rotation but not when taste is paired with shock or lactose, neither of which gives rise to upper gastrointestinal distress (Pelchat et al., 1983; see Parker, 2003). In humans there is a close but imperfect correlation between nausea and disgust (Rozin & Fallon, 1987). The initial formation in an organism of a taste aversion seems to involve gastrointestinal distress, just as the formation of a conditioned fear response to a neutral stimulus may require pairing with shock, but once the CTA is formed aversion proceeds without illness suggesting that the DISGUST system is activated as a learned response. CTAs also form in one trial to a novel taste paired with bacterial antigens which produce an immune response normally associated with illness (e.g. Pacheco-Lopez et al., 2004) suggesting that these bacterial antigens may also produce nausea in the rat. A number of experiments have shown that learned taste avoidance (measured in amount drunk) can be dissociated from learned taste aversion (measured by gapes, chin wiping etc.). In her review of this work Parker (2003) uses the term “disgust reaction” to refer to oral gaping by which rats reject fluid infused into the mouth. She suggests that reduced drinking from a spout measures both appetitive and consummatory responses not necessarily involving disgust, whereas gaping or retching produced by intraoral infusion Disgust: Sensory Affect or Primary Emotional System? 19 provides a measure of consummatory response alone. Measuring aversion by gaping and retching in rats should therefore give an indication of activation of the DISGUST system. Work by Parker and her colleagues shows that in rats, treatments pairing taste and a drug that produces conditioned taste aversion (as measured by gaping during intraoral infusion) also produce avoidance of the paired taste (as measured by voluntary drinking), but not all treatments producing avoidance of the taste lead to conditioned aversion (see Parker, 2003). Antinausea treatments reduce taste aversion as measured by gaping in rats, but not taste avoidance, suggesting that nausea or malaise is necessary for the establishment of a conditioned taste aversion. Of course conditioning is not necessary to produce all taste aversion responses, as some inherently bad-tasting substances produce distaste aversion which is not eliminated by antiemetics. Innate distaste responses are found in normal and decerebrate infant mammals including anencephalic human infants (Steiner et al., 2001). A learned response to a tasteillness pairing, however, is not a reflexive sensory affect but involves an affective behavioural programme, i.e. the disgust reaction. A case study reported by Adolphs, Tranel, Koenigs and Damasio (2005) illustrates this dissociation in a human patient with bilateral damage to the insula, amygdala, hippocampus and parts of the neocortex. This man has normal taste preference when sequentially comparing two fluids, although he appears to have no conscious explanation for his preference. He prefers strong sucrose solution over strong salt solution when presented sequentially but completely lacks taste recognition of the stimuli presented separately, appearing to have no conscious recognition of either. In other words because the brainstem gustatory areas are all intact he shows the sensory affect, but not the full disgust component. This also suggests that at Disgust: Sensory Affect or Primary Emotional System? 20 least in humans the disgust response is a conscious affect whereas distaste need not be conscious. The sensory affect-like distaste response of earlier vertebrates has been elaborated on with the unique development of the insula in humans and the concomitant development of subjective awareness. Thus the role of the insula is not limited to providing an anatomical substrate for disgust. The indirect visceral pathway between insula and amygdala which enabled the formation of CTA in rodents was replaced in primates with a direct thalamocortical pathway allowing greater cortical integration of sensory inputs from multiple modalities. The human insula, as described by Craig (2002, 2003, 2005) has come to play a major role underlying the distinctively human experience of consciousness, a form of consciousness which according to Burgdorf and Panksepp (2006) arose out of ancient forms of core consciousness supported by brain stem structures. We are thus suggesting that the primitive emotive circuit which originally functioned to defend the self by regulating consummatory behaviours gave rise to a primary emotional system which facilitates evaluation of reinforcers. This is consistent with Rozin and Fallon’s view (1987) of the elaboration of the disgust mechanism in humans. The disgust-associated circuitry became further enriched in primates by development of a direct thalamocortical pathway that interacts with multimodal inputs as described by Craig (2003). In addition the unique spindle cells found in deep layers of the cingulate cortex of humans and great apes but no other mammals, are also found in human agranular and dysgranular AI (Nimchinsky, Gilissen, Allman, Perl, Erwin & Hof, 1999). Heimer and Van Hoesen (2006) go so far as to suggest that the right AI is not only Disgust: Sensory Affect or Primary Emotional System? 21 crucial for emotional awareness, but “the final breeding ground for subjective feeling states”. Facilitated by abundant reciprocal connections to the adjacent orbitofrontal cortex, this evaluative emotional system acquired a role in self-consciousness and complex-decision making. Core representations of the body in the proto-self arising from structures such as the insula (Damasio, 1999, p.156) may play a role not only in selfawareness, but also in an “as-if” loop system that allows evaluation and anticipation of events. So the human insula rather than providing a mere module for disgust, participates together with other areas in the instantiation of a variety of conscious feeling states. The AI plays a role in the acquisition of the “gut feeling” developed by subjects during the Iowa gambling task (Bechara & Damasio, 2005; see also Dunn, Dalgleish, & Lawrence, 2006). The right AI mediates subjective awareness of heart rate timing; and the activity and volume of AI are correlated with the accuracy of this awareness (Critchley et al., 2004). The right insula is also activated by anticipation of emotionally aversive visual stimuli (Simmons, Mathews, Stein, & Paulus, 2004). In individuals who practice meditation, the number of years of practice correlates with the volume of the right AI (Lazar, Kerr, Wasserman, Gray, Greved, Treadway et al., 2005). Probably related to activation by disgusting stimuli, right AI is also somewhat activated by unpleasant moral scenarios (Moll, Oliveira-Souza, Eslinger, Bramati, Mourão-Miranda, Andreiuolo, et al., 2002). Many behaviours integrating higher cognitive processing with “gut level” feelings also activate AI, especially on the right. For example right AI activation is correlated with the degree of risk during decision-making as well as the degree of harm avoidance and Disgust: Sensory Affect or Primary Emotional System? 22 neuroticism in individuals (Paulus, Rogalsky, Simmons, Feinstein, & Stein 2003). Unfair offers of monetary reward which the authors suggested to be a form of emotion-based disgust activate AI more strongly on the right (Sanfey, Rilling, Aronson, Nystrom, & Cohen, 2003). While the right AI seems to play a special role in other subjective states, studies using unpleasant tastes, odors, or evaluating disgust often report more involvement on the left (e.g. Calder et al., 2000; Hennenlotter et al., 2004; Royet et al., 2003; Schienle et al., 2005; Small et al., 2003; Wicker et al., 2003). Goldman and Sripada (2005) have pointed out that the reading of basic emotions in the faces of others may have unique survival value involving specialized neural programs which attribute a state in another based on simulation of that state in oneself. They note this may be accomplished by a reverse simulation which would require an as-if loop if it were not to depend on actual facial feedback, or alternatively observation of the target’s face might trigger activation of the same neural substrate in the observer. The somatic marker hypothesis of Damasio proposes the role of the insula is primarily to provide sensory representation of the bodily state as part of an as-if mechanism. On the other hand Gallese, Keysers and Rizzolatti (2004) propose the key activation of the insula in social cognition is as a viscero-motor centre which simulates the activity of the other person in a manner similar to the “mirror” neurons which they previously found in monkeys. Hence Wicker and colleagues (2003) were able to show, using the same subjects, that activity in left AI subserves both the experience of disgust and its recognition in others. In the same manner both the bodily state of pain as well as anticipation of pain in a loved one activated the right AI, and individual empathy scores correlated with the level of activity in the right AI while the loved one was experiencing Disgust: Sensory Affect or Primary Emotional System? 23 pain (Singer, Seymour, O'Doherty, Kaube, Dolan, & Frith, 2004). A recent follow up study showed that the AI activity in response to pain in another person was modulated by the perceived fairness of that person (Singer, Seymour, O'Doherty, Stephan, Dolan, & Frith, 2006). Perhaps the two sides of the insula have become specialized for divergent roles in self-awareness, with the left side playing a greater role in signalling and perceiving danger to the internal milieu and the right in processing other forms of self and other awareness. Conclusion We have argued here the case that disgust is a primary emotional system, with roots deep back in our pre-mammalian ancestors. There are reasons to believe it might have indeed been the first such system, arising out of links between the immune system and the central nervous system, indeed possibly being the origin of the system of Neural Darwinism. In a companion paper (Toronchuk & Ellis, forthcoming) we propose a complete set of primary emotional systems, including DISGUST. Completion of a list of such primary emotions, developing from the work of Darwin, Ekman, Panksepp, and many others, is a key step in identifying the psychological factors guiding development of higher level brain functioning. This in turn has implications for psychology, learning, economics, and many other aspects of human behaviour. It is for this reason that we believe that the kinds of argument presented in this paper are important. Some have argued that the concept of basic emotions is not useful (e.g. Ortony & Turner, 1990; Rolls 2005b, pp.31-32), proposing in essence that there is a onedimensional basic evaluation system guiding brain development, with the (apparent) Disgust: Sensory Affect or Primary Emotional System? 24 basic emotions evolving out of this system in a systematic way (but not themselves being genetically determined). We believe rather that a multi-dimensional genetically determined system as envisaged by Panksepp (1998) and ourselves (Ellis & Toronchuk, 2005) is needed because a 1-dimensional system simply does not have the ability to structure a brain in the developmental time available, and indeed even if slower were acceptable, probably can't do the job with sufficient refinement and accuracy to meet the brain's developmental and functional needs in any case. Thus we see basic emotions playing a key role in brain evolution and development (as discussed in Ellis & Toronchuk, 2005). If this is correct, in understanding brain development it is crucial to determine which emotional systems are primary (that is, mainly genetically determined) and which are secondary (that is, determined mainly by developmental processes as the mind interacts with the local social environment). Investigating this may generate useful future research on evolutionary and developmental processes. We see two further significant reasons to discuss which emotions should be considered basic. Firstly, focusing attention on the biological functions of these systems in the evolutionary past has led psychology to a greater awareness of biological complexities of human emotions. This has led and will continue to lead to new treatments and diagnostic techniques for psychiatric disorders. Secondly because of the complex interaction of genetic and environmental components in the ontogeny of basic emotions, the consideration of developmental mechanisms may lead to new ideas on the promotion of healthy emotional development in human children. Disgust: Sensory Affect or Primary Emotional System? 25 FOOTNOTES 1 We follow Panksepp in the use of capitals to denote an emotional organising system rather than an emotion per se. Disgust: Sensory Affect or Primary Emotional System? 26 REFERENCES Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Koenigs, M., &, Damasio, A. R.(2005). Preferring one taste over another without recognizing either. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 860-861. Alvarez-Borda, B., Ramirez-Amaya, V., Perez-Montfort, R., & Bermudez-Rattoni, F.(1995). Enhancement of antibody production by a learning paradigm. Neurobiology of Learning & Memory, 64 (2), 103-105. de Araujo, I.E.T., Rolls, E.T., Kringelbach, M.L., McGlone, F., & Phillips, N. (2003). Taste-olfactory convergence and the representation of the pleasantness of flavour, in the human brain. European Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 2059-2068. Bechara, A., & Damasio, A.(2005). The somatic marker hypothesis: A neural theory of economic decision. Games and Economic Behavior 52, 336–372. Berman, D.E., Hazvi, S., Neduva, V., & Dudai, Y. (2000). The role of identified neurotransmitter systems in the response of insular cortex to unfamiliar taste: activation of ERK1–2 and formation of a memory trace. The Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 7017–7023. Bermudez-Rattoni, F.(2004). Molecular mechanisms of taste recognition memory, Nature Reviews: Neuroscience, 5, 209-217. Bermudez-Rattoni, F., Grijalva, C.V., Kiefer, S.W., &. Garcia, J. (1986). Flavor-illness aversions: the role of the amygdala in the acquisition of taste-potentiated odor aversions. Physiological Behavior, 384, 503-508. Bernstein, I.L. (1999). Taste aversion learning: a contemporary perspective. Nutrition, 15, 229-234. Disgust: Sensory Affect or Primary Emotional System? 27 Burgdorf, J., & Panksepp, J. P.(2006). The neurobiology of positive emotions. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 173–187. Calder, A. J., Keane, J., Manes, F., Antoun, N., & Young, A.W.(2000). Impaired recognition and experience of disgust following brain injury. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 1077-1078. Carew, T. J., & Sahley, C. L.(1986). Invertebrate learning and memory: from behaviour to molecules. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 9, 435–-487. Cechetto, D.F., &. Saper, C.B. (1987) Evidence for a viscerotopic sensory representation in the cortex and thalamus in the rat. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 262, 2745. Craig, A. D. (2002). How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience, 3, 655-666. Craig, A. D. (2003). Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 13, 500–505. Craig, A.D. (2005). Forebrain emotional asymmetry: a neuroanatomical basis? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 566-571. Critchley, H. D., & Rolls, E.T.(1996). Olfactory neural responses in the primate orbitofrontal cortex: Analysis in an olfactory discrimination task. Journal of Neurophysiology, 75, 1659-1672. Critchley, H .D., Rotshtein, P., Nagai, Y., O'Doherty, J., Mathias, C. J., & Dolan, R. J.(2005). Activity in the human brain predicting differential heart rate responses to emotional facial expressions. NeuroImage, 24, 751-762. Disgust: Sensory Affect or Primary Emotional System? 28 Critchley, H. D., Wiens, S., Rotshtein, P., Öhman, A., & Dolan, R. J. (2004). Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 189-195. Curtis, V. A., & Biran, A.(2001). Dirt, disgust and disease: is hygiene in our genes? Perspectives in Biology and Medicine, 44, 17–31. Curtis, V., Aunger, R., & Rabie, T.(2004). Evidence that disgust evolved to protect from risk of disease. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences, 271, 131-133. Damasio, A.R. (1999). The feeling of what happens: body and emotion in the making of consciousness. New York, NY: Harcourt. Damasio, A. R., Grabowski, T. J., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Ponto, L. L., Parvizi, J., et al. (2000). Subcortical and cortical brain activity during the feeling of selfgenerated emotions. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 1049-1056. Domjan, M.(2005). Pavlovian conditioning: a functional perspective. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 179–206. Dunn, B. D., Dalgleish, T., & Lawrence, A. D.(2006). The somatic marker hypothesis: A critical evaluation. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 239-271. Egan, G., Silk, T., Zamarripa, F., Williams, J., Federico, P., Cunnington, R. et al. (2003). Neural correlates of the emergence of consciousness of thirst. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, U.S.A., 100, 15241-15246. Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V.(1986). A new pan-cultural facial expression of emotion. Motivation and Emotion, 10, 159-168. Ellis, G. F.R., & Toronchuk, J. A.(2005). Neural development: Affective and immune system influences. In N. Newton, & R. Ellis (Eds.), Consciousness and emotion: Disgust: Sensory Affect or Primary Emotional System? 29 Agency, conscious choice and selective perception (pp.81-119). Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins. Fitzgerald, D. A., Posse, S., Moore, G. J., Tancer, M. E., Nathan, P. J., & Phan, K.L.(2004). Neural correlates of internally-generated disgust via autobiographical recall: a functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation. Neuroscience Letters, 370, 91-96. Flynn, F. G., Benson, D. F., & Ardila, A.(1999). Anatomy of the insula--functional and clinical correlates. Aphasiology, 13, 55-78. Galati, D., Scherer, K.R., & Ricci-Bitti, P.E. (1997) Voluntary facial expression of emotion: comparing congenitally blind with normally sighted encoders. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1363-1379. Gallese, V., Keysers, C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2004).A unifying view of the basis of social cognition. Trends in Cognitive Science 8, 396-404. Garcia, J., Hankins, W. G., & Rusiniak K. W.(1974). Behavioral regulation of the milieu interne in man and rat. Science, 185, 824-831. Garcia, J., Quick, D. F., & White, B.(1984). Conditioning disgust and fear from mollusk to monkey. In D. Alkon, & J. Farley, Primary neural substrates of learning and behavioral change (pp. 47-61).Cambridge University Press. Goldman, A. I. & Sripada, C. S.(2005). Simulationist models of face-based emotion recognition. Cognition, 94, 193-213. Gottfried, J. A., & Zald, D. H.(2005). On the scent of human olfactory orbitofrontal cortex: meta-analysis and comparison to non-primates. Brain Research Reviews, 50, 287-304. Disgust: Sensory Affect or Primary Emotional System? 30 Heimer, L.,& Van Hoesen, G. W. (2006). The limbic lobe and its output channels: Implications for emotional functions and adaptive behaviour. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 30, 126-147. Hennenlotter, A., Schroeder, U., Erhard, P., Haslinger, B., Stahl, R., Weindl, A., et al. (2004). Neural correlates associated with impaired disgust processing in presymptomatic Huntington's disease. Brain, 127, 1446-53. Hornby, P. J. (2001). Central neurocircuitry associated with emesis. The American Journal of Medicine, 111 Supplement 8A, 106S-112S. Horton, J., & Ratcliffe N.A.(2001) Evolution of Immunity In I. Roitt, J. Brostoff, & D. Male, Immunology 6th ed. Toronto, ON: Mosby. Hosie, J.A., Gray, C.D., Russell, P. A., Scott, C., & Hunter, N.(1998). The matching of facial expressions by deaf and hearing children and their production and comprehension of emotion labels. Motivation and Emotion, 22, 293-313. Kiefer, S.W., & Orr, M. R.(1992).Taste avoidance, but not aversion, learning in rats lacking gustatory cortex. Behavioral Neuroscience, 106, 140-146. Koh, M. T., & Bernstein, I. L.(2005). Mapping conditioned taste aversion associations using c-Fos reveals a dynamic role for insular cortex. Behavioral Neuroscience, 1192, 388-398. Krolak-Salmon, P., Henaff, M.-A., Isnard, J., Tallon-Baudry, C., Guenot, M., Vighetto, A., et al.(2003). An attention modulated response to disgust in human ventral anterior insula. Annuals of Neurology, 53, 446-453. Disgust: Sensory Affect or Primary Emotional System? 31 Lane, R. D., Reiman, E. M., Ahern, G. L., Schwartz, G. E., & Davidson, R. J. (1997). Neuroanatomical correlates of happiness, sadness, and disgust. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 926-933. Lasiter, P. S., Deems, D.A., Oetting, R. L., & Garcia. J. (1985). Taste discriminations in rats lacking anterior insular gustatory neocortex. Physiological Behavior, 352, 277-285. Lazar, S. W., Kerr, C. E., Wasserman, R. H., Gray, J. R., Greved, G. N., Treadway, M. T., et al. (2005). Meditation experience is associated with increased cortical thickness NeuroReport, 16, 1893-1897. Lawes, I.N. (1990). The origin of the vomiting response: a neuroanatomical hypothesis. Canadian Journal of Physiological Pharmacology, 682, 254-259. Lawrence, A. D., & Calder, A. J.(2004). Homologizing human emotions. In D. Evans & P. Cruse (Eds.), Emotion, evolution and rationality (pp. 15-47). Oxford University Press. Levenson, R. W., Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V.(1990). Voluntary facial action generates emotion-specific autonomic nervous system activity. Psychophysiology, 27, 363384. Liotti, M., Mayberg, H. S., Brannan, S. K., McGinnis, S., Jerabek, P., & Fox, P. T. (2000). Differential limbic-cortical correlates of sadness and anxiety in healthy subjects: implications for affective disorders. Biological Psychiatry, 48, 30-42. Lundy, R. F., & Norgren, R. (2004).Activity in the hypothalamus, amygdala, and cortex generates bilateral and convergent modulation of pontine gustatory neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology, 91, 1143-1157. Disgust: Sensory Affect or Primary Emotional System? 32 Mesulam, M.-M., & Mufson, E. J.(1982). Insula of the old world monkey. III: Efferent cortical output and comments on function. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 212, 38-52. Miller, S.B. (2004) Disgust: The gatekeeper emotion. Hillsdale, NJ.: The Atlantic Press. Miller, W.I. (1997). The anatomy of disgust. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Miranda, M. I., Ferreira, G., Ramirez-Lugo, L., & Bermúdez-Rattoni, F.(2002). Glutamatergic activity in the amygdala signals visceral input during taste memory formation. Proceeding of the National Academy of Science, USA, 99, 11417– 11422. Morris, J.S., Friston, K.J., Büchel, C., Frith, C.D., Young, A.W., Calder, A.J., et al. (1998). A neuromodulatory role for the human amygdala in processing emotional facial expressions. Brain, 121, 47-57. Moll, J., Oliveira-Souza, R., Eslinger, P. J., Bramati, I. E., Mourão-Miranda, J., Andreiuolo, P.A., et al.(2002). The neural correlates of moral sensitivity: a functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation of basic and moral emotions. The Journal of Neuroscience, 22, 2730-2736. Nimchinsky, E. A., Gilissen, E., Allman, J. M., Perl, D. P., Erwin, J. M., & Hof, P. R.(1999). A neuronal morphologic type unique to humans and great apes. Proceeding of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 96, 5268-5273. Mufson, E. J., Mesulam, M. M., & Pandya, D. N.(1981). Insular interconnections with the amygdala in the Rhesus monkey. Neuroscience, 6, 1231-1248. Disgust: Sensory Affect or Primary Emotional System? 33 O’Doherty J., Rolls, E.T., Francis, S., Bowtell, R., & McGlone, F. (2001). The representation of pleasant and aversive taste in the human brain. Journal of Neurophysiology, 85, 1315–21. Öhman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychological Review, 108, 483-522. Ortony, A., & Turner, T.J.(1990). What's basic about basic emotions? Psychological Review, 97, 315-331. Pacheco-López, G., Niemi, M.-B., Kou, W., Härting, M., Del Rey, A., Besedovsky, H. O., & Schedlowski, M.(2004). Behavioral endocrine immune conditioned response is induced by taste and superantigen pairing. Neuroscience, 129, 555562. Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. London, UK: Oxford University Press. Panksepp, J. (2000). Emotions as natural kinds within the mammalian brain. In M. Lewis & J.M. Haviland-Jones, (Eds.), The handbook of emotions, 2nd ed. (pp.137-156). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Panksepp, J. (2005). Affective consciousness: core emotional feelings in animals and humans. Consciousness and Cognition, 14, 30–80. Parker, L. A. (2003). Taste avoidance and taste aversion: evidence for two different processes. Learning & Behavior, 312, 165-172. Paulus, M. P., Rogalsky, C., Simmons, A., Feinstein, J. S, & Stein, M. B.(2003). Increased activation in the right insula during risk-taking decision making is related to harm avoidance and neuroticism. NeuroImage, 19, 1439–1448. Disgust: Sensory Affect or Primary Emotional System? 34 Pelchat, M. L., Grill, H. J., Rozin, P., & Jacobs, J.(1983). Quality of acquired responses to tastes by Rattus norvegicus depends on types of associated discomfort. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 97, 140-153. Phillips, M. L., Young, A. W., Senior, C., Brammer, M., Andrew, C., Calder, A. J., et al. (1997). A specific neural substrate for perceiving facial expressions of disgust. Nature, 389, 495-498. Ramirez-Amaya, V., Alvarez-Borda, B., & Bermudez-Rattoni, F.(1998). Differential effects of NMDA-induced lesions into the insular cortex and amygdala on the acquisition and evocation of conditioned immunosuppression. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 12, 149-160. Ramirez-Amaya, V., & Bermudez-Rattoni, F.(1999). Conditioned enhancement of antibody production is disrupted by insular cortex and amygdala but not hippocampal lesions. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 13, 46-60. Ritz, T., Thons, M., Fahrenkrug, S., & Dahme, B.(2005) Airways, respiration, and respiratory sinus arrhythmia during picture viewing. Psychophysiology, 42, 568578. Rolls, E. T.(1989). Information processing in the taste system of primates. Journal of Experimental Biology, 146, 141-164. Rolls, E. T.(2005a). Taste, olfactory, and food texture processing in the brain, and the control of food intake. Physiology & Behavior, 85, 45-56. Rolls, E. T.(2005b). Emotions explained. (Oxford University Press, Oxford). Disgust: Sensory Affect or Primary Emotional System? 35 Royet, J.-P., Plailly, J., Delon-Martin, C., Kareken, D. A., & Segebarth, C.(2003). fMRI of emotional responses to odors: Influence of hedonic valence and judgment, handedness, and gender. NeuroImage, 20, 713–728. Rozin, P., & Fallon, A. E.(1987). A perspective on disgust. Psychological Review, 94, 23-41. Rozin, P., Haidt, J., & McCauley, C. R.(1999). Disgust: the body and soul emotion .In T. Dalgleish & M. J.. Power, (Eds.),The handbook of cognition and emotion, 2nd ed. (pp. 429-445). Chichester, UK: John Wiley. Rozin, P., & Kalat, J. W.(1971). Specific hungers and poison avoidance as adaptive specializations of learning. Psychological Review, 78, 459-486. Rubio-Godoy, M., Aunger, R., & Curtis, V.(2007). Serotonin—a link between disgust and immunity? Medical Hypotheses, 68, 61-66. Sanfey, A. G., Rilling, J. K., Aronson, J. A., Nystrom, L. E., & Cohen, J. D. (2003). The neural basis of economic decision-making in the ultimatum game. Science, 300, 1755-1758. Saper, C.B. (2002). The central autonomic nervous system: conscious visceral perception and autonomic pattern generation. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 25, 433-469. Schafer, A., Schienle, A., & Vaitl, D.(2005). Stimulus type and design influence hemodynamic responses towards visual disgust and fear elicitors. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 57, 53-59. Schienle, A., Schafer, A., Stark, R., Walter, B., & Vaitl, D.(2005). Relationship between disgust sensitivity, trait anxiety and brain activity during disgust induction. Neuropsychobiology, 51, 86-92. Disgust: Sensory Affect or Primary Emotional System? 36 Schienle, A., Stark, R., Walter, B., Blecker, C., Ott, U., Kirsch P., et al.(2002). The insula is not specifically involved in disgust processing: an fMRI study. Neuroreport, 13, 2023-2026. Shapira, N. A., Liu, Y., He, A. G., Bradley, M. M., Lessig, M. C., James, G. A., et al.(2003). Brain activation by disgust-inducing pictures in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 54, 751-756. Simmons, A., Mathews, S. C., Stein, M. B., & Paulus, M. P.(2004). Anticipation of emotionally aversive visual stimuli activates right insula. NeuroReport, 15, 22612265. Singer, T., Seymour, B., O'Doherty, J., Kaube, H., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D.(2004). Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. Science, 303, 1157-1162. Singer, T., Seymour, B., O'Doherty, J. P., Stephan, K. E., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D. (2006). Empathic neural responses are modulated by the perceived fairness of others. Nature, 439, 466-469. Spector, A. C. (2000), Linking gustatory neurobiology to behavior in vertebrates. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 24, 391–416. Sprengelmeyer, R., Young, A. W., Calder, A. J., Karnat, A., Lange, H., Hömberg, V., et al. (1996). Loss of disgust. perception of faces and emotions in Huntington's disease. Brain: a Journal of Neurology, 119, 1647-1665. Sprengelmeyer, R., Young, A. W., Mahn, K., Schroeder, U., Woitalla, D., Büttner, T., et al.(2003). Facial expression recognition in people with medicated and unmedicated Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychologia, 41, 1047-1057. Disgust: Sensory Affect or Primary Emotional System? 37 Sprengelmeyer, R., Young, A. W., Pundt, I., Sprengelmeyer, A., Calder, A. J., Berrios, G., et al. (1997). Disgust implicated in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences, 264, 1767-1773. Small, D. M., Gregory, M. D., Mak, Y. E., Gitelman, D., Mesulam, M. M., & Parrish, T.(2003). Dissociation of neural representation of intensity and affective valuation in human gustation. Neuron, 39, 701-711. Small, D. M. Zarotte, R. J., Dagher, A., Evans, A. C., & Jones-Gotman, M.(2001). Changes in brain activity related to eating chocolate: from pleasure to aversion. Brain, 124, 1720-1733. Stark, R., Walter, B., Schienle, A., & Vaitl, D.(2005). Psychophysiological correlates of disgust and disgust sensitivity. Journal of Psychophysiology, 19, 50-60. Steiner, J. E., Glaser, D., Hawilo, M. E., & Berridge, K. C.(2001). Comparative expression of hedonic impact: Affective reactions to taste by human infants and other primates. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 25, 53-74. Stevenson, R.J., & Repacholi, B.M.(2005). Does the source of an interpersonal odour affect disgust? a disease risk model and its alternatives. European Journal of Social Psychology, 35, 375–401. Stoléru, S., Grégoire, M.-C., Gérard, D., Decety, J., Lafarge, E., Cinotti, L., et al.(1999). Neuroanatomical correlates of visually evoked sexual arousal in human males. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 28, 1-21. Disgust: Sensory Affect or Primary Emotional System? 38 Sugai, R., Shiga, S., Azami, S., Watanabe, T., Sadamoto, H., Fujito, Y., et al.(2006). Taste discrimination in conditioned taste aversion of the pond snail Lymnaea stagnalis. The Journal of Experimental Biology 209, 826-833. Tataranni, P. A., Gautier, J. F., Chen, K, Uecker, P. A., Bandy, D., Salbe, A. D., et al.(1999). Neuroanatomical correlates of hunger and satiation in humans using positron emission tomography. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A., 96, 4569-4574. Toronchuk, J. A. & Ellis, G.F.R.(in preparation). Affective neuronal Darwinism: the nature of the primary emotional systems. Wicker, B., Keysers, C., Plailly, J., Royet, J. P., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G.(2003). Both of us disgusted in my insula: The common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust. Neuron, 40, 655-664. Wolf, K., Mass, R., Ingenbleek, T., Kiefer, F., Naber, D., & Wiedemann, K.(2005). The facial pattern of disgust, appetence, excited joy and relaxed joy: an improved facial EMG study. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 46, 403-409. Yasui, Y., Breder, C. D., Saper, C. B., & Cechetto, D. F.(1991). Autonomic responses and efferent pathways from the insular cortex in the rat. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 303, 355-374. Zhang, Y., Lu, H., & Bargmann, C.I.(2005). Pathogenic bacteria induce aversive olfactory learning in Caenorhabditis elegans. Nature, 438, 179-184. ----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------