Why should we feel so “Blue” sometimes? Hadjar Homaei March 2007 “Affirm that name, whoever thus repeat To him even death will taste soft & sweet Whoever once that face has kissed His bones are blessed in gravely mist.” Mawlānā Jalāl ad-Dīn Rūmī Introduction Synesthesia (also spelled synæsthesia or synesthesia, plural synesthesae or synaesthesae)1 from the Greek roots syn, meaning “together,” and aisthesis, or “perception”, is a neurological condition in which otherwise normal people experience the blending of two or more senses. [1] For a long time this phenomenon was often disregarded as fakery or unconscious experiencing childhood memories and associations. [1] But it has recently been shown to be real. The most probable theory is that it occurs because of cross activation between two normally separate areas of the brain when they educe activity in each other. Explorations in the mechanisms involved in synesthesia, not only helps to uncover mystic ideas behind this phenomenon, scientists are also learning about how the brain in general processes sensory information and uses it to make abstract connections between seemingly unrelated inputs.[1] Facts – Am I synesthetic? Since synesthesia does not, in general, interfere with normal daily functioning many synesthetes maybe not be aware of their condition or know it has a name. In reality most synesthetes report their synesthetic experiences to be neutral or even pleasurable, [4] or even in some cultures they think of it as a god given gift. Simner et al., (in press) conducted the first random population study, arriving at a prevalence of 1 in 23. Recent data suggests that grapheme → color, and days of the week → color variants are most common.2 1 2 Wikipedia – Synesthesia http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Synesthesia Simner et al. in press While cross-sensory metaphors are sometimes portrayed as "synesthetic", especially in visual art and literature, true neurological synesthesia is involuntary. It does not require attention and it involves a vivid perception in the secondary sense. It runs strongly in families. A very famous synesthete was Alexander Nabakov with grapheme → color synesthesia, whose mother and grandfather and son were also synesthetes.3 It is possibly inherited as an X-linked dominant trait. Maybe that is why it is more common in women; studies find a sex ratio of 1.1:1.2 Synesthesia is also sometimes reported by those under the influence of psychedelic drugs (often LSD), after a stroke or brain injury, or as a consequence of blindness or deafness. Interestingly there have also been reports of synesthetes who lost their condition after a brain injury.3 This form of synesthesia that arises from such non-genetic events is often referred to as “adventitious synesthesia” to discriminate it from the more common inherited forms of synesthesia. Adventitious synesthesia involving drugs or stroke (but not blindness or deafness) apparently only involves sensory linkings; there are few if any reported cases involving culture-based, learned sets such as graphemes, lexemes, days of the week, or months of the year. Neurologist Richard Cytowic who was one the first modern scientists to study synesthesia in 1980s, identifies the following diagnostic criteria of synesthesia:[3] 1. Synesthesia is involuntary and automatic. 2. Synesthetic images are spatially extended, meaning they often have a definite 'location'. 3. Synesthetic percepts are consistent and generic 4. Synesthesia is highly memorable. 5. Synesthesia is laden with affect. 6. Synesthesia is not easily forgotten. Synesthetes often report that they were not aware their experiences were strange until they realized other people did not have them. The automatic and ineffable nature of a synesthetic experience means that the pairing may not seem out of the ordinary. This involuntary and consistent nature helps us define synesthesia as a real experience which we can scientifically tested. Testing if someone is a synesthete is easy in most cases, and it can be very hard to fake. The simplest reliable test involves test-retest reliability over long periods of time. Synesthetes consistently score higher on such tests than non-synesthetes. Synesthetes may score as high as 90% consistent over test-retest intervals of up to one year, while non-synesthetes will score 30-40% consistent over test-retest intervals of only one month, even if warned that they are going to be retested.4 To determine whether an effect is truly perceptual, psychologists often use a simple test called pop-out or segregation. [1] When Ramachandran and Hubbard (2001) conducted pop-out tests with grapheme → color synesthete that is now the standard test for this 3 4 Richard E. Cytowic, M.D. lecture about synesthesia at the Hirshhorn museum, Washington D.C. 2007 e.g., Baron-Cohen et al. 1996 form of synesthesia. They presented synesthetes and non-synesthetes with displays composed of a number of 5s, with some 2s embedded among the 5s. These 2s could make up one of four shapes; square, diamond, rectangle or triangle. (fig 1) Unlike normal subjects, synesthetes correctly reported the shape formed by groups of numbers up to 90 percent of the time, just as non-synesthetes would do when the numbers actually had different colors. This result proves that the induced colors are genuinely sensory because it is impossible for them to fake this. Succeeding studies based on this effect show that there is considerable variability among synesthetes of the same form5 and also while synesthesia is evoked early in perceptual processing, it does not occur prior to attention6. Figure 1. Pop up effect Different Forms of Synesthesia Synesthesia can happen between almost any two senses or perceptual modes. Because of the substantial number of forms of synesthesia, researchers have agreed on a convention of demonstrating the type of synesthesia by using the following notation x → y, where x is the "inducer" or trigger experience, and y is the "concurrent" or additional experience. [2] For example, perceiving letters and numbers as colored would be indicated as grapheme → color synesthesia. Also, when synesthetes perceive colors and movement as a result of hearing musical tones, it could be indicated as tone → (color, movement) synesthesia. Although very few but there are also some synesthetes who have bidirectional synesthesia, for example tone → color synesthetes who perceive different pitches as colors and also hear notes when they see colors or even hear a chord or music watching a painting or movie.7 5 Dixon, Smilek & Merikle 2004; Hubbard et al. 2005a e.g., Edquist et al. 2006; Sagiv, Heer & Robertson 2006a 7 Richard E. Cytowic, M.D. lecture about synesthesia at the Hirshhorn museum, Washington D.C. 2007 6 In many forms of synesthesia, the inducer set and concurrent set belong to distinct sense modalities. However, not all forms of synesthesia span two sense modalities.[2] For example grapheme → color synesthesia involves two different modalities of the same sense; visual. In addition, some forms of synesthesia involve inducers that are conceptual rather than sensory such as days of the week → color. Grossenbacher and Lovelace 2001, discriminate between two types of synesthesia, based on their inducers types. In Synesthetic Perception, concurrents are stimulated by perceiving particular sensory stimuli, but in Synesthetic Conception, concurrents are stimulated by thinking about particular concepts. The most common synesthesia forms or maybe the easiest ones to detect are synesthetic perceptions, like grapheme → color whereas in the time period → location synesthesia which is a form of synesthetic conception, periods of time are conceptualized in a spatial layout. (Figure 2.c) [3] Which I believe is very close to synesthetic metaphors that most people can understand the idea and it sounds natural to them but they do not necessarily experience the concurrent sensation. While almost every possible combination of experiences is logically possible, some forms of synesthesia are more common than others. Grapheme → color Grapheme → color synesthesia, is one of the most common and most studied forms of synesthesia, when individual letters of the alphabet and numbers (graphemes), are shaded with a color. (Figure 1.a) Although each particular synesthete’s consistent graphemecolor mapping can be different from others but there are also some commonalities across letters (e.g., A is likely to be red). [5] A grapheme → color synesthete reports, "I often associate letters and numbers with colors. Every digit and every letter has a color associated with it in my head. Sometimes, when letters are written boldly on a piece of paper, they will briefly appear to be that color if I'm not focusing on it. Some examples: 'S' is red, 'H' is orange, 'C' is yellow, 'J' is yellow-green, 'G' is green, 'E' is blue, 'X' is purple, 'I' is pale yellow, '2' is tan, '1' is white. If I write SHCJGEX it registers as a rainbow when I read over it, as does ABCPDEF."[2] Another reports a similar experience. "When people ask me about the sensation, they might ask, 'so when you look at a page of text, it's a rainbow of color?' It isn't exactly like that for me. When I read words, about five words around the exact one I'm reading are in color. It's also the only way I can spell. I remember in elementary school remembering how to spell the word 'priority' because the color scheme, in general, was darker than many other words. I would know that an 'e' was out of place in that word because e's were yellow and didn't fit." Fig 2. Some forms of Synesthesia Music → Color In music → color synesthesia, synesthetes experience colors in triggered by tones or other aspects of musical stimuli (e.g., chord, key, scale). Similar to grapheme → color synesthesia, although the mapping of inducer and concurrent is internally consistent for each individual synesthete but there is not much agreement among different music → color synesthetes. Synesthetes will consistently report the same experiences in response to the same inducer if tested again. Color changes in response to pitch may involve more than just the hue of the color. Brightness, saturation, and hue may all be affected to varying degrees. [9] In addition, unlike grapheme → color synesthetes, music → color synesthetes, often report that the color patterns move with the music, or flow inside and outside their visual field. Number form synesthesia A number form is a mental map of numbers in space, that involuntarily comes into mind whenever the synesthete person thinks of numbers.(figure 2.b) In particular, it has been suggested that number-forms are a result of "cross-activation" between regions of the parietal lobe that are involved in numerical cognition and spatial cognition. [1] This form of synesthesia has also been explored by numerical cognition researchers for the insights that it may give into the neural mechanisms of numerical spatial associations that is unconsciously present in everyone’s mind. Figure 3. 'Film for Music' interprets a piece of music from Brian Eno's album "Music for Films." Visually by a tone → color synesthete, Fred Collopy. Personification Ordinal-linguistic personification – OLD sometimes referred to as a type of animism, is a form of synesthesia in which ordered sequences, such as ordinal numbers, days of the week, months of the year and letters are associated with personalities. [10] Although it is harder to distinguish this type of synesthesia from non-synesthetic associations, however, recent research has shown that this form of synesthesia co-varies with other forms of synesthesia and cases that are automatic and maintain consistency, are required to be considered as a form of synesthesia. [10] Plausible Theories From the beginning of observing synesthesia, researchers have been contemplating about possible mechanisms underlying this phenomenon. As more research has been done, some theories can be ruled out, while others become more plausible. Associative learning Calkins (1893) was the first to suggest that synesthesia could be a form of associative learning possibly learned throughout early childhood. This hypothesis initiate several experiments that tried to “teach” adults one form of synesthesia using Pavlov conditioning.8 However, early experiments as well as recent works found that, although non-synesthetic individuals could be trained to learn these associations, there is no evidence that real conscious, involuntary synesthetic experiences accompanied these newly learned associations. [11,12] There are also many arguments against this theory. For example, in order to learn these associations, synesthetes should be exposed to hundreds or thousands of occasions with the same pairs of different stimuli. Marks and Odgaard [11] suggest that this does not seem to be very plausible; however I think that it can be plausible for some certain types of synesthesia tone → color and personification because there can be countless stimuli of these types in our environment in our everyday life. Rich and Mattingley[12] argue that for many synesthetes, a few letters consistently elicit colors in the same range. It is impossible that children all over the world consequently see these letters in the same colors, which is expected if these results depend on learned association. But I think there can be explanations for this so that if children learn these associations early in childhood by colored magnet alphabets that are used for training at schools, maybe the manufacturers and designers of these toys are also conditioned on the same set of similar mappings. Furthermore, behavioral experiments [12,1] showed that synesthesia is a more perceptual experience. These and other observations argue strongly against memory associations. So, it is assumed that synesthesia has a neural basis. Limbic activation, mediated by cortical inhibition Cytowic [3] suggested that synesthesia is actually a normal brain function generated in subcortical areas, mainly in the limbic system but its activations only reach consciousness level in little number of people, mediated by cortical inhibition. His theory was based on a fMRI experiment with xenon-133 inhalation in which a Shape → Taste synesthete showed extensive decrease in cerebral blood flow which indicates a decrease of activity in neocortex. This result sounds to be in line with the results of an ERP study of grapheme → Color synesthetes. Schiltz and his colleagues observed positivity, which indicates an inhibition, over frontal and prefrontal structures. Nevertheless, no further evidence is found for this theory, and no other imaging study has confirmed these results. On the contrary, activation of frontal areas is present when there is a mismatch between the grapheme surface color and the induced synesthetic color.9 Cross-activation Baron-Cohen suggests another theory based on the assumption that synesthesia represents a breakdown in modularity of sensory areas. The hyperconnectivity theory indicates that brains of synesthetes differ from normal brains in the way that they have extensive crosswiring between two or more different sensory modalities. The reason for these extra connections could either be due to additional unnecessary growth or a failure of natural pruning throughout development. Grossenbacher and Lovelace [2] proposed that dis8 9 eg. Kelly, 1934 and Howells, 1944 eg. Weiss et al. 2001 and 2005 inhibited feedback can be the mechanism that causes cross-activation. The dis-inhibited feedback theory assumes no abnormal neural connections, but proposes that synesthesia is entirely mediated by neural connections that exist in normal human brains. This theory says that in normal individuals, the inhibitory mechanism prevents the activation between different modules, while in synesthetes this inhibitory mechanism is not functioning properly and this leads to a cross-activation of concurrents in a subordinate modality. [11] The main support of this theory comes from primate studies which show that conscious sensory perceptions are characterized by feedforward and feedback information flow, mainly in the visual system. These theories have led to an updating of the cross-wiring theory of Ramachandran and Hubbard, and the developing of possible models. [11] For example, Grossenbacher and Lovelace [2] proposed a mechanism characterized by feedforward and feedback connections, in which three different routes can lead to an activation of the concurrent representation, either through hyperconnectivity, or through dis-inhibition. [11] Smilek et al. [13] suggested a re-entrant model based on dis-inhibited feedback. In his model, feedforward connections from V1, through V2, to V4 take color and form information of a grapheme, which is processed and sent to Anterior fusiform and Posterior Inferior Temporal (PIT) cortex. From here the synesthetic processing departs from normal processing. Feedback connections from these areas back to V4 carry information about the synesthetic meaning of the grapheme which influences the perception of an externally presented figure. Although producing slightly different results, neuroimaging studies support this idea that synesthesia results from cross-activation between different modalities, for example between grapheme/auditory and color selective brain areas. Studies that studied the response to hearing words in phoneme → color synesthetes, reported an activation of areas ranging from visual association areas such as parietooccipital junctions and the posterior inferior temporal (PIT) cortex [15], to early visual areas V4 and V8 [14], and even V1 10 that were not present in control experiments. Other studies that examined grapheme → color synesthesia found activation in extra striate cortex [18], intra-parietal cortex, fusiform gyros [16] and V4 [17], after perceiving words. These activation patterns appear to be related to the synesthetic experience. V4 is associated with color perception, while the PIT cortex is concerned with the integration of color with shape. It is known that both areas show strong attentional modulation which is consistent with cognitive studies showing that graphemes can not induce synesthetic colors when presented peripherally, and that synesthetic experience highly depends on attention. [1] Also, it seems that the color percepts of synesthetes can be generated without primary visual area activation. This finding matches with the report of a synesthete who experienced ‘uncanny’ colors that he had not experienced in real world since he has scone weakness, and also the description of blind people that still perceive synesthetic ally evoked colors. [20] This implies that if synesthesia works because of dis-inhibited feedback, the feedback connection may not go as far back as primary visual areas. 10 Aleman et al. 2001 Finally, there appears to be cross-activation in synesthetic perception, but unfortunately, we’re not sure if this is because of inhibition or excessive cross-wiring, as areas of brain activity do not point to a certain mechanism. Some important questions Despite the similarities that let us define the general phenomenon of synesthesia, it should also be noted that experiences of individual synesthetes vary in many ways. Some grapheme → color synesthetes report that the colors seem to be "projected" out into the world, while most report that the colors are experienced in their "mind's eye".11 Maybe that is why no research has demonstrated a consistent association between synesthetic experience and other neurological or psychiatric conditions yet, although this is an active area of research. It might be difficult to study on synesthetes for whom this experience is involuntary, so we can not turn the phenomenon on or off and study the change in the mechanism that controls their perception; however there are other situations that normal people can experience synesthetic perception. Many people that have used hallucinogens claim to have "seen sounds" or "heard colors" and this is clearly synesthetic experience which has been “turned on” by a drug. Using hallucinating drugs is not the best way to experience such marvel, but there also have been many reports on people experiencing the same phenomena during meditation. Although in meditation one makes an attempt to minimize perceptions through senses by detaching mind from sense organs in the brain to help to silent the mind but in fact it increases consciousness. Also studies show that it causes clearer perception. [8] My personal experience for several years of meditation gives me the idea that not only the consciousness is increased but it’s quite different level because during meditation and a little while after you can have a total consciousness of every part of your body and every stimulus from environment somehow that it feels like a whole new body or even your feet on the floor although you shouldn’t be conscious of this perception because of adaptation. So are the forms of synesthetic experiences based on altered consciousness, similar when caused by drugs such as LSD and when caused by long meditation? Are there different types of mechanisms that can cause synesthesia? If there is only one mechanism it should be found in the mechanisms that Meditation and Hallucination share. We know that meditation can cause some rewiring in the brain where hallucination in a short term does not. [8] But they both cause changes in the level of neurotransmitters in the brain. It is determined that hallucinogenic drugs structurally resemble serotonin and cause an increase in the level of brain serotonin, but they inhibit the rapid firing of neurons containing serotonin. This is a negative feedback system in which as the serotonin level rises, the activity of serotonergic neurons decreases. On the other hand meditation has no proven effect on serotonin level but long meditations dramatically 11 Dixon, Smilek & Merikle 2004 increase the level of melatonin in the brain which is a hormone secreted by the pineal gland in proportion to the body's exposure to light. Considering the fact that serotonin is a precursor of the hormone melatonin, hallucinogenic drugs must also increase the melatonin level. I believe that study of this similarity, the melatonin level, might be a worthwhile idea. One can compare the melatonin level of synesthetes and normal people or to test the effect of harmless melatonin supplements that in low dose can help regulate sleep but in higher dose can bring vivid dreams and nightmares – if taken at night before sleep. Time release melatonin pills can help people with depression but can it cause normal people to experience synesthesia? As described earlier Cytowic suggests that cross-modal associations are a normal part of our thinking that happen at an unconscious level. On the other hand synesthetes are aware of these associations and discern them as “experiences”. All people experience what they hear and see as distinct events - but we integrate these diverse experiences in forming our thoughts based on the sensations they bring to our brains and we call them sensations and feelings because we are not exactly aware of how they integrate. That is where synesthete metaphors come from. Language metaphors are part of our everyday life and they are trivial to every normal person but why a sound is sour when it’s unpleasant? How can person be sweet if she’s nice? Cytowic goes on to state that though we discriminate what we hear and see as distinct events, experience shows that we can integrate them in forming thoughts about what these sensations bring to our brains. The integration occurs at a level that we are not accessible by our consciousness while the synesthetes’ experiences imply that there is a conscious mixing of these sensory channels. This is the theory that sounds the most interesting to me. I think I can design some experiments to compare synesthetes and normal individuals based on their characteristics of their conscious experiences in their concurrent modality. For example on the minimum duration of stimulus presentation that evokes the conscious experience. My hypothesis is that grapheme → color synesthetes’ time threshold for perceiving color might be lower than normal people. Also another very simple experiment can involve measuring the effect of unconscious synesthete process - if it exists by forcing nonsynesthetes to pick colors for graphemes although they do not have a conscious perception and check if there is any commonality among them and if that matches of synesthetes. References [1] Vilayanur S. Ramachandran, Edward M. Hubbard,” Hearing Colors, Tasting Shapes”, Scientific American, 2003, p 53-59 [2] Peter G.Grossenbacher, Christopher T. Lovelace, “Mechanisms of synesthesia: cognitive and physiological constraints”, Trends in Cognitive Sciences Vol.5 No.1 p 3641, 2001 [3] Richard Cytowic, “The Man Who Tasted Shapes”, New York: Tarcher/Putman, 2003 [4] Sean Day, "Some Demographic and Socio-cultural Aspects of Synesthesia", in L. Robertson & N. Sagiv, Synesthesia: Perspectives from Cognitive Neuroscience, Oxford: Oxford University Pres, 2005 [5] Julia Simner, Jamie Ward et al., "Non-random associations of graphemes to colors in synesthetic and non-synesthetic populations", Cognitive Neuropsychology 22(8): 10691085 [6] B.L. Jacobs. “How Hallucinogenic Drugs Work.”, American Scientist 75:3 p 85-92, 1987 [7] Alicia Ebbitt, “The Effects of Hallucinogenic Drugs on the Brain” [8] M.P.Bhattathiry, “Neurophysiology of Meditation”, [9] Sean Day, “Synesthesia and Synesthetic Metaphors“, in Simon Baron-Cohen, Synesthesia: Classic and Contemporary, 1996 [10] Simner, J.; C. Mulvenna & N. Sagiv et al. , "Synaesthesia: The prevalence of atypical cross-modal experiences", Perception 8(35): p 1024-1033, 2006 [11] Marks, L.E., & Odgaard, E.C. “Developmental constraints on theories of synaesthesia. “ In L. Robertson and N. Sagiv (Eds.), Synesthesia: Perspectives from Cognitive Neuroscience. New York: Oxford University Press. 2005 [12] Rich, A.N. & Mattingley, J.B.“Anomalous perception in synaesthesia: a cognitive neuroscience perspective.” Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, p 43-52. 2002 [13] Smilek D., Dixon M.J., Cudahy C., Merikle P.M. “Synesthethic colour Experiences Influence Memory” Psychological Science 13(6), p 548-552, 2002 [14] Nunn J.A., Gregory L.J., et al, “Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging of Synesthesia: Activation of V4/V8 by Spoken Words.” Nature Neuroscience 5(4), p 371375, 2002 [15] Paulesu E., Harrison J., Baron-Cohen S., et al. “The Physiology of coloured Hearing: A PET Activation Study of colour-Word Synaesthesia.” Brain 118, p 661-676, 1995 [16] Weiss P.H., Zilles K., Flink G.R. “When Visual Perception Causes Feeling: Enhanced Cross-Modal Processing in Grapheme-colour Synesthesia.” Neuroimage 28, p 859-868, 2005 [17] Hubbard, E. M.; A. C. Arman & V. S. Ramachandran et al. "Individual differences among grapheme-color synesthetes: Brain-behavior correlations", 45(6) Neuron, p 975985, PMID 15797557, 2005 [18] Weiss P.H., Shah J., Toni I., Zilles K., Fink G.R. “Association colours With People: A Case of Chromatic-Lexical Synesthesia.” Cortex 37, 750-753, 2001 [19 ] Mondloch C.J., Maurer D. “Do Small White Balls Squeak? Pitch-object Correspondences in Young Children.” Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience 4(2), p 133-136, 2004 [20] Steven M.S., Blakemore C. “Visual Synesthesia in the Blind.” Perception 33, p 855868, 2004