Some Basics of Selayarese Pronominal Clitics This study makes an effort to explain basic pronominal cliticization in the Austronesian language Selayarese in terms of the Minimalist Program (Chomsky 2001, 1995). Prior work on the language omits the basics of pronominal cliticization, jumping to more complex phenomena (Finer 2002). My aim in this work is to answer the question why certain bound pronominal forms pattern as clitics on the verb. Singular forms are given in (1). Three dimensions of the verb phrase are relevant to all syntactic structures in Selayarese. These are, clitic doubling, resemblance to the ‘Philippine-type focus system’ (Wouk and Ross 2002), and ergativity (noted by Mithun and Basri 1985). Verbs canonically require pronominal clitics to mark their overt arguments, as in examples (2–5). However, personal pronouns do not show doubling (3). This means that a pronominal clitic doubles a non-pronominal overt argument, located elsewhere in the structure, by way of being fixed onto the verb. Notable approaches to clitic constructions, base-generation versus movement to surface position are reconciled by adopting Sportiche’s (1996) conclusion that pronominal clitic constructions are derived from movement of a clitic associate to a base-generated clitic category. Based on a number of related ordering constraints, it appears that these categories sandwich the verb. Although the presence of the ‘Philippine-type focus system’ of Wouk and Ross is faint in Selayarese, it is apparently responsible for a good deal of derivation in the syntax. In Philippine-type focus systems, a morpheme in the verb co-references one of the entities it licenses, to the effect that that entity, whether argument or non-argument, accrues some kind of elevation above other arguments. Terzi (1999) discusses contrastive information structure versus new information structure. This opposition could provide a way to determine which entities are drawn into co-reference with Selayarese verb morphology. Particularly, while the ergative clitics are arguably proclitic and the absolutive clitics enclitic, any one of a variety of intransitive ‘focus’ morphemes precedes the verb root. By nature, these are in complementary distribution with the ergative proclitic, e.g., (5); however, some interesting combinatorial variation involving the ‘focus system’ does occur. It provides a strategy to increase transitivity, resulting in structures such as causatives and ditransitives. Mahajan (2005a, b) provides an insightful way to render ergativity, by assigning case to the specifier of vP, without the problematic ergativity parameter. It is striking that Selayarese allows ergative marked agents if the object of the verb is definite (4) but not in other cases (5). This is evidence that information structure is responsible for definiteness. Moreover, it is the definiteness feature of the determiner which dictates the distribution of pronominal clitics and their associated noun phrases. It does this by effecting which entities enter specifier position of vP. My presentation argues that bound pronouns reside in verb adjacent clitic categories. They arrive there because of verb ‘focus’ morphology and definiteness, which is determined by information structure. On another level, bound pronouns occur phonologically as clitic because of their location in a clitic functional category. (1) Selayarese Bound Pronouns and Verbalizers Ergative Absolutive Verbalisers 1 ku-a a’Intransitive, [-def] object 2familiar mu-ko naintransitive 2honorific ri-ki taPatient (unergative) 3 la-i ang- Intransitive/transitive paCausativizer (2) Noai ’i Baso. noa-i ’i Baso greedy-3Abs Prsn.Mrkr Baso ‘Baso is greedy.’ (3) Po:rea. po:re-a skillfull-1Abs ‘I am skillfull.’ (4) Lataroi doe’ injo ri lamari injo ’i Baso. la-taro-i doe’ injo ri lamari injo ’i Baso 3Erg-put-3Abs money the in cupboard the Prsn.Mrkr Baso ‘Baso put the money in the cupboard.’ (5) ’Amu:noa ’a:su. ’a-mu:no-a ’a:su Intr-kill-1Abs dog ‘I killed a dog.’ REFERENCES CHOMSKY, NOAM. 2001. Derivation by phase. In Michael Kenstowicz (ed.) Ken Hale. A life in language, pp: 1–52. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. ——. 1995. The Minimalist program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. FINER, DANIEL. 2002. Phases and Movement in Selayarese. In Andrea Rackowski and Norvin Richards (eds.), MIT Working Papers in Linguistics 44, Proceedings of AFLA 8, 157–69. MAHAJAN, ANOOP. 2005a. Ergatives, Passives and other Oblique Subject Constructions. Seminar handout. University of Hawaii: Honolulu. ——. 2005b. The Syntactic Basis of Ergativity. Seminar handout. University of Hawaii: Honolulu. MITHUN, MARIANNE AND HASAN BASRI. 1986. The Phonology of Selayarese. Oceanic Linguistics 25:1–2. pp: 210–54. SPORTICHE, DOMINIQUE. 1996. Clitic constructions. In J. Rooryck and L. Zaring (eds.), Phrase structure and the lexicon, pp: 213–277. Dordrecht: Kluwer. TERZI, ARHONTO. 1999. Clitic combinations, their hosts and their ordering. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 17: 85–121. WOUK, FAY AND MALCOLM ROSS. 2002. The history and typology of western Austronesian voice systems. Pacific Linguistics 518: 17–62. Canberra: Research School of Pacific and Asian Studies, Australian National University.