From Galton Points to ACE-V: One Examiner’s Journey An Editorial Perspective Disguised as a Research Paper By Sandra Wiese Like many other people I have met in the forensic professions, I was not fortunate enough to begin my career in a mentoring program or internship. I work for a small department and my in-house training consisted solely of learning how to use the computer system and being shown where the various offices were. Any and all training I did have came as a result of my own pursuit of it through courses put on by my local professional organization and through personal training with professional contacts. While I would not trade this experience for anything, I do admit it was spotty training at best and somewhat accidental at worst. I also found out that this is the norm in some jurisdictions, both near me and throughout the United States. Just as my colleagues in similarly small agencies do, I wear many hats: crime scene investigator, latent print developer and examiner, photographer, blood pattern analyst…butcher, baker, and candlestick maker. Not only do I have to report on these duties, I am frequently subpoenaed to testify to my examinations and analyses in these areas. The courts do not much care that I may not be experienced enough to write a book on any of these topics, they rightfully only care that I have performed my duties with honesty and integrity and that I have the proper training to perform them correctly. And here lies the rub. Without a proper mentoring program, I feel that I have not had the opportunity to develop a thorough enough knowledge in these areas as I might like. Despite countless hours of my personal time spent reading professional journals and books and talking with colleagues about the proper or best ways to accomplish my duties, I can never be sure just what I am missing. How could I be sure I was doing it “right?” The little personal instruction in latent comparison I received over the years went something like this: “See this point here in the latent? Now find that in the inked print. Now find any other points that match like that. If you don’t find them, you don’t have a match.” That was about it. I later found out I wasn’t the only person to receive this sort of instruction. This was or is the norm for a lot of the smaller agencies in my area. Then in 2001, I signed up for a course called “Ridgeology” because I heard it was a great class. I had previously learned to count “points” and also that there was no minimum number that was necessary for an identification. In the same breath, the more experienced examiners I knew would tell me how they never go into court with less than [fill in an arbitrary number here] “points” and how they once knew someone who got a conviction off a comparison with only [fill in an arbitrary smaller number here] “points.” Imagine my confusion with this “there is no minimum number but this is my minimum number and you should find a minimum number that you are comfortable with” philosophy. Well, I went to this Ridgeology class and found out that you can use the very pores in the ridges to make an identification! What would they think of next? We were shown pictures of comparisons with only three “points” and told they were an identification. Now I believe with all of my heart and mind that no two fingerprints or other areas of friction skin are alike, but there was no way I was ever going to testify to something like that. How could I possibly take that to a jury in my county and ask them to buy it? I believed it, yes, but I could not explain it if my very life depended on it. During this class, we learned about a “new” process for comparing latent prints called ACE-V. (Actually, we first tried to learn about “ayzvee”, which is what ACE-V sounds like coming from a fast-talking Canadian. Then we made him write it down and it all became clear as…well, you will see.) If my confidence in my abilities to compare prints was a little shaky before I came to class, after those “three-pointers” he showed us I was down right terrified. But I did understand that ACE-V was basically the same thing we had been doing all along, but it was a better way to articulate the process of comparison and identification. After this class, I added words like “level one detail” and “insufficient to effect a comparison” to my examination reports. I may have referred to the presence of level three detail in my reports, but I was still not remotely comfortable with the idea of ever using it myself. I was still comparing “just points,” but I was wording it in a more sophisticated way. About a year went by and I was still comparing points and only points and was eternally grateful that I did not have to ever testify to more than a negative identification. I did have confidence in my identifications and did get verification from examiners at other agencies, but I did not want to have to testify. Along came a Daubert symposium in 2002 and any confidence in my ability to testify crumbled like the Berlin Wall. How could I possibly be expected to know all of that and still perform at “expert” level in the other areas of my job? Shortly after the Daubert symposium, I had the opportunity to take an advanced comparison course and jumped at it. My hopes had been that this would be the class that made it all clear. My last chance, really. During this class we discussed the concept of “no minimum points” and “sufficiency.” I brought up that I did not understand how to respond to a defense challenge that “sufficiency” was subjective and therefore, not science. I left the class without an answer to this question. I drove home preparing in my mind how I would tell my boss that I could not do latent comparisons anymore because I obviously did not “get” it. I practiced saying, “Would you like to super size that?” As you might guess, my boss did not fire me and also did not “get” what the problem was. I was stuck: I still had to do the comparisons and I was still terrified of having to explain what I did to someone else (read: judge and jury). I felt like a fraud. Now comes the end of 2003 and I get a latent print in that is clear as a bell. The problem? Only about four “points” but also one very nice scar running down the center. Now I did not have suspect prints to compare at that stage, but I looked at the print and thought, “If I do get a suspect and I do make a match, how can I testify to it if most of my ‘points’ are all up and down the scar?” So I sent the prints to an IAI certified examiner I know (Mark Beck, CLPE, Alaska Department of Public Safety) and posed this same quandary to him. Oh sure he would make an identification off the print he tells me, no doubt. I wanted to know how? I couldn’t possibly testify to it. So he explained it to me and, a la Lucy Ricardo, I finally got it. It wasn’t the points, it was the way they were shaped and where they were in relation to everything else, not just how many ridges separated them. That this ridge goes down and to the right and not up and to the left and this other ridge is a little bulbous and this other ridge is pointy…Voila. It is not just the existence or location of various pores or the shape of the ridge edges. If anyone had ever explained that to me, I must not have been listening because that was the first time “level three” made sense to me. Holy God I could use level three in a comparison! There was hope. But what, I asked him, if this goes to court and that nasty “sufficiency” question raises its ugly head? Most of the areas of comparison in this latent were up and down the scar and I was still not prepared to use every single ridge characteristic (bye-bye “points”) to make my identification, even if I knew it was an identification. “Sufficiency,” I said to Mark, “is subjective. The subjectivity makes it an art or at the very least partially an art and not science and there is no way to counter that!” I have been wrong before, perhaps just never at this level. This was “level three” wrong thinking, but he made me finally understand. I will tell you how in just a bit… Now, this very well may be the longest introduction to a “research” paper ever written, but without knowing the history, it has little overall impact. I have been in this business for eight years, the last five as a full-time Criminalist with latent print comparisons as a primary duty. I have had over 250 hours of classroom instruction in latent development, recovery, classification and comparison. I just understood what is happening when I compare prints last month. On the off chance that I am not the dimmest bulb on the tree and that there are others just like me, you had to know the history. I would like to explain to you what I now know and maybe why and how this happened to me: There are many texts available to explain and illuminate the intricacies of friction ridge identification science. From Field’s Fingerprint Handbook to Ashbaugh’s Ridgeology with many more in between. Not to mention all of the professional publications and I won’t even start on the websites. There is a problem with every one of these sources: If you read only one, you are not getting a well-rounded education in the area of identification; if you read more than one, you risk complete confusion because the terminology is not all the same and the methods used to make an identification are not all the same. Couple these problems with the absence of an experienced examiner as mentor (or the presence of an indifferent mentor) and you get a recipe for perfectly poor confidence in the examiner’s ability to articulate the process involved in a comparison. One of the most glaringly obvious differences in terminology is the definition of the term characteristics. I would have thought that this one was a “gimme” because it is perhaps the most common word used when explaining friction ridge identification science. Once again, I was wrong. Field defines characteristics as, “the fusion of pores, length, course, breaks, directions, etcetera, of the ridges form the individual characteristics of fingerprints (Field, 11).” A pretty advanced definition for a text written in 1959. But this is a very different definition than the more modern one provided in a glossary of identification terms published in 2002 by the Scientific Working Group for Friction Ridge Analysis, Study and Technology (SWGFAST), which defines characteristics as, “features of the friction ridges. Commonly referred to as minutia(e), Galton detail, point, feature, ridge formation, ridge morphology (SWGFAST Glossary, 1).” This definition got me because I (and other people I know) had been going out of my way to not use the term “Galton details” or the word “points” anymore. I thought those terms were the “old way” and that they were not to be used now that we had all these “levels” in friction skin. So now I’m really confused, but I cannot find any explanation anywhere for all the different definitions. There doesn’t seem to be a text written just for me. No Fingerprinting for Dummies, no The Idiot’s Guide to Friction Ridge Identification. I did find out that I am not the first person to notice this. Scott’s Fingerprint Mechanics states that “There is no common agreement among authorities…of the different types of ridge characteristics or their terminology (Olsen, 9).” And in 1983, Cowger wrote, “The terms used to describe these characteristics vary markedly among writers in the field and differ even among examiners (Cowger, 143)…” While I may know that a bifurcation and a dividing ridge are two words for the same thing, and while I may be able to tell you how this is so, up until recently I would not have been able to tell you exactly how “points” and “Galton details” were different from “level one, two and three details.” And I could not have referred to an authoritative text for this answer because it is not specifically addressed. If I am so confused, how can anyone blame a new examiner for being frustrated or an attorney (prosecutor, defense or judge) for being confused? We can talk shop with other latent print examiners all day every day and think we have it all in the bag. But unless we can make the rest of the world understand what we are talking about and doing, all of our talk is relatively worthless. Let’s look at that whole attorney problem while we are on the subject…If asked, I imagine most attorneys (and lay people for that matter) would tell you that they know only two things about fingerprints: 1) they can be used for positive identification, and 2) you have to match up at least [fill in a number between about 8 and 12] points. This is what they know. But wait! There are no minimum points, right? No court has ever said there was a number, right? An attorney might not get that idea if they used the Fingerprint Handbook as their reference source, which states this definition: “Number of Identical Characteristics: it is necessary to identify only the most distinctive characteristics, rather than each ridge. Most courts consider eight to twelve points of identity sufficient for convincing identification. (emphasis mine; Field, 127).” Okay, so if I were an attorney reading this book, I would want the examiner to come up with a number between 8 and 12 or higher. I might even mention this to the examiner in a pre-trial conference. Then that examiner might whip out Ashbaugh and read, “No scientific basis exists for requiring that a predetermined minimum number of friction ridge features must be present in two impressions in order to establish positive identification (Ashbaugh, 7).” Now because I am a lawyer, I am going to ask for an explanation of that one. As an attorney, I am going to want that concrete number. It is definitive, the jury understands it, and we can move along with the trial right quick: Wham, Bam, and Thank you Sam. They said there were ten, you’re off to the pen! Most texts do not seem to address the issue of the difference between having a minimum number and not having a minimum number. Several sources did mention that there was no minimum point requirement as a result of the 1973 International Association for Identification (IAI) pronouncement that this standard had no scientific basis. Other than reiterating this premise and possibly throwing in a little shameful “pooh-poohing” to the European countries still using a minimum point standard, most of the texts did not explain just what this meant in a real world application or how to explain it to a jury. Only Ashbaugh dares to even address this problem: “The number of points philosophy had been in use since the inception of the science just over 100 years ago. With this new doctrine, an opinion of identification could now be based on a varying number of points. At the time, the change in doctrine appeared to be only a minor adjustment in identification philosophy, but the consequences were far-reaching. In the beginning this new doctrine was not fully understood, leading to uncertainty and greater confusion as opposed to a solution (emphasis mine; Ashbaugh, 2).” He further states, “Most efforts to describe or defend the evaluative identification process were more an exercise in reciting rhetoric and dogma as opposed to describing a scientific process. Specific facts and logical interpretation were conspicuous by their absence (Ashbaugh, 3).” I’ll second that. My understanding of “no set minimum number” was limited to my having faith in those examiners in a leadership role in the professional organizations. They had to be smarter than me, right? So I would have to take their word for it. But since I did not “get” it, in practice I had to use the “there is no minimum number but this is my number” approach. It worked for me for the most part. Well, that philosophy combined with the constant crossing of my fingers and knocking on wood so I wouldn’t be called to testify about how to compare prints in court. That’s what really worked. So back to that advanced comparison class and the whole unanswered “sufficiency” question. After this discussion in class, several people came up to me and said that they felt the same way. We all had problems defining sufficiency as “enough” and none of us could explain how the subjectivity involved in our judgment of “enough” fit in with the concept of a science. The available literature covering this question is not always clear or even in agreement. Some texts advise that two impressions must compare identically (emphasis mine; Fields, 125). Others are not so exact “…an ‘identification’ is made when, in the judgment of the examiner the degree of similarity between two prints is sufficient to warrant that conclusion (emphasis mine; Cowger, 146).” SWGFAST failed to include a definition of “sufficiency” in their glossary of terms, however they do attempt to define it in their Examination Methodology, “sufficiency is the examiner’s determination that adequate unique details of the friction skin source area are revealed in the impression.” Sounds great, but what exactly is “adequate”? When I have raised this question in the past in classes and at conferences, I have been told that maybe I was just being “argumentative” or that I “obviously” did not believe in the science. I was told, on more than one occasion, that if I believed in the science then I would know what “enough” was and would I just “let it go.” Well, I did and do believe in the science. Believe enough to know it can withstand questioning by little ol’ me. And that these questions mean there is more work to be done to develop a better understanding for everyone. If I can have these questions after eight years in the field, imagine the questions of people newer in the field or (gasp!) of those people on the jury. I would not expect a jury member to believe my testimony just because I told them it was so, but that is what I would have been doing if I had ever testified to a “sufficient number of matching characteristics” without being able to explain what exactly this means. Maybe the question shouldn’t be so much what is “sufficient,” but how the determination of “sufficiency” is in fact scientific. The question here is always some variation of: If I use my judgment to determine sufficiency, then it is a somewhat arbitrary and subjective decision and therefore inconsistent with the idea that friction ridge identification is a science instead of an art. I do believe that friction ridge identification is a science, not an art, but if I am going to testify to it, I have to be able to explain it and counter those people who came before me who have characterized it as an art. Cowger writes that T. Dickerson Cooke, one of the most respected workers in this field, wrote in 1973 that, “Pronouncing that two friction skin impressions…were or were not made by the same area of friction skin is an art, not an exact science. It is entirely a matter of judgment based on training and experience (Cowger, 146-147).” How can I counter that on the stand? Have I been led down the wrong path? My confusion must be contagious and it may be spreading to the courts. More than one defense attorney and more than one defense expert has attacked fingerprint evidence on this basis alone. District Court Judge Michael had this to say in his dissenting opinion in U.S. v. Crisp: “One forensic expert (Stoney) contends that there are no standards; there are no minimum point requirements. The movement away from point requirements ‘is not based on scientific study. (Epstein)’ and there is disparity in the field regarding the use of level 3 detail for id. (because of distortion) ‘One dissimilarity’ in two impressions is thought to be a universal standard, but if an examiner believes the prints match they explain away the difference rather than discounting the match. Verification is considered to be essential, but cases exist where no verification took place; and even verification that does take place is not independent and objective. All of this leads (Stoney and Cole) to the belief that ‘[t]he criteria for absolute identification in fingerprint work are subjective and ill-defined.’ (Wertheim/Weekly Detail 123)” Judge Michael was not convinced that friction ridge identification is a science under the rules of the court. Although the previous excerpt was from a dissenting opinion, Judge Michael obviously feels strongly that the friction ridge identification field has not made its case and he is in a position to influence decisions in similar cases. Perhaps James Cowger was a little overly optimistic when he wrote, “that this element of judgment exists as a necessary element of the comparison is certainly not seriously questioned (Cowger, 148).” I could find no explanation of how this judgment is not subjective or how this subjectivity relates to friction skin identification being a true science in any of the professional friction skin comparison sources. So I took it a few steps outside the fingerprint realm to find my answers. First, I looked up the terms “subjective” and “objective.” All of the dictionaries I looked at basically broke down the differences as being dependent on personal feelings (subjective) or being independent of personal feelings (objective). Fair enough. Then I looked into what makes any study a science. The most consistent litmus test seemed to be the application of the scientific method. On to looking up what exactly constitutes the scientific method. Guess what? Scientific method does not include objectivity by definition in any reputable source I could find. To me, this couldn’t be a mere coincidence, but it took a little more thought to fully understand. It finally hit me: Scientists in every field do use their judgment when they are applying scientific method and principles. Take medicine for instance: Doctors use their judgment every day, yet they are still simultaneously applying scientific methods and principles. And just because they do so does not make medicine an art or even any less of a science. What a concept. The judgment of a doctor is based on his or her training and experience and this is why it is not purely subjective and this is why medicine is a science even with the application of judgment. That is also why different friction skin examiners can both look at the same print and come up with differing amounts that they consider “sufficient.” The difference is because of the differences in training and experience, not because the science is invalid. That simple. The problem? The texts in this field do not explain it so simply. Mark Beck helped me to understand subjectivity versus objectivity and “sufficiency” with a very simple story. This is as good a time as any to share that story with you: A car runs a red light at a high rate of speed causing a fatal accident. The car speeds away from the scene. There are four witnesses to the entire incident. All four were standing on the same street corner, the same distance from the accident. The first witness is a 16-year-old high school football player. He tells the responding officer that the run vehicle was an “old, shiny red and white convertible.” He doesn’t know a Chevy from a Ford, knows even less about vehicle years, he doesn’t know about different license plates and he did not pay attention to this one anyway. The second witness is an average 32-year-old male. He reports to the officer that the run vehicle was an older model Corvette convertible, red over white and that the vehicle had Wyoming plates, but he did not get the number. The third witness works at a Chevy dealership and is an antique car buff. He tells the cop that the vehicle was a cherry red over white 1958 Corvette convertible with Wyoming plates. He further advised that he has attended antique car shows in the Rocky Mountain Region for over 30 years and that he has only ever seen one vehicle like that and he knows the owner’s name is John Smith and that he lives somewhere in Laramie, Wyoming. The fourth witness is an off-duty Wyoming State Patrol officer. She tells the responding officer that the run vehicle was a cherry red over white 1958 Corvette convertible with Wyoming plates. She also advised that she had recently checked Department of Motor Vehicle records for a similar vehicle that was involved in a separate traffic incident she was currently investigating. Her investigation to date in the other case revealed that there was only one vehicle of this make, model, year and color registered in Wyoming and that this vehicle was registered to John Smith of 123 Chestnut Ave. in Laramie, Wyoming. All four witnesses saw the same thing from the same vantage point. All four therefore had the same objective observation (facts is facts). All four witnesses had the visual information available to them to identify the run car, but only two of the witnesses had the training and experience to individualize the vehicle. This story did more to enhance my understanding of the whole subjectivity and sufficiency argument than anything I have read or heard to date. It is a very simple way to explain what I thought was a difficult concept. Maybe whoever writes the next friction ridge comparison “Bible” would put in some easy examples like this for the dummies like me? This field went from a “counting points” standard to a “there is no scientific basis for a minimum number of points” standard with little debate or explanation as to the practical applications of this change. The practical result has been that too many examiners of the day could not articulate the reasons behind the change and ended up counting their points while telling everyone else that point counting is passé. Then these examiners taught the new examiners and one of those new examiners was me. More has changed in this field than just the way we count or don’t count characteristics. The very explanation of how a comparison is conducted has changed. But the literature on the subject is not written with readily understandable explanations in mind. Most of the literature I have read does not explore friction skin comparison at the lowest level, a level necessary to give every examiner a sound and consistent understanding of the basics of friction skin identification. A “new basics” if you will. As more challenges to friction ridge identification science are occurring regularly, the need to address these issues should be paramount. The science needs to develop consistent terminology and publish articles on the basics so we don’t have any more examiners like me, who can make the identifications but not necessarily explain them. I am not the only one with these experiences, but the chances of everyone else in this situation suddenly having it all become clear to them, too, are slim to none. After all, where can they look to get an explanation? As Ashbaugh so succinctly states the problem on the very first page of his book, “While the ability to identify is inherent, an understanding of the process and the ability to describe it is not.” BIBLIOGRAPHY Ashbaugh, D. (1999). Quantitative-Qualitative Friction Ridge Analysis: An Introduction to Basic and Advanced Ridgeology. Baca Raton, Florida: CRC Press. Cowger, J. (1983). Friction Ridge Skin: Comparison and Identification of Fingerprints. New York: Elsevier Science. Field, A. (1959). Fingerprint Handbook. Springfield, Illinois: Charles C. Thomas. Olsen, R. (1978). Scott’s Fingerprint Mechanics. Springfield, Illinois: Charles C. Thomas. Scientific Working Group for Friction Ridge Analysis, Study and Technology (SWGFAST). (2002, August). Friction Ridge Examination Methodology for Latent Print Examiners. 1. Retrieved January 19, 2004, from www.swgfast.org Scientific Working Group for Friction Ridge Analysis, Study and Technology (SWGFAST). (2002, August). Glossary – Identification. Retrieved January 19, 2004, from www.swgfast.org Wertheim, K. (2003, December). U.S. v. Crisp Dissenting Opinion. The Weekly Detail. 123. Retrieved December 15, 2003, from www.clpex.com