WORD ASSOCIATION RESPONSES OF JAPANESE AND AMERICANS COMPARED WITH MORAN AND THE S-P SHIFT JUDY YONEOKA ABSTRACT A study was made of associative responses of 3-5-year-old and 8-10-year-old Japanese and American children as well as of adults to a list of 30 stimulus words, composed of 10 nouns, 10 verbs and 10 adjectives. Responses were classified according to grammatical form class and to a four-category system developed by Moran (1973). It was found that response mechanisms in Japanese and American adults differed radically, in that over 60% of American adult responses were paradigmatic (same form class as the stimulus word), and 47 % fell into Moran's logical category, whereas only 22.2 % of adult Japanese responded paradigmatically, and less than 5 % give logical category responses. Comparison data for various ages showed that 8-12-year-old response patterns were basically similar to those of the adults (with one exception), but that 3-5-year-old children of both cultures showed at least 66% syntagmatic (different form class) responses, resembling each other but differing greatly from the response patterns of their elders. Additionally, Japanese speakers of English in three classes of ability were tested in English. Intermediate and advanced speakers had essentially the same response patterns as the Japanese adults tested in Japanese. Conversely, expert speakers showed approximately the same overall percentage of paradigmatic responses as the American adults, although their use of logical responses was somewhat lower. INTRODUCTION In the 1960's, a great deal of literature appeared documenting a theory called the "syntagmatic-paradigmatic shift" (or "syntactic paradigmatic shift", hereafter referred to as S-P shift)1 in free-association tests. This S-P shift refers to a cognitive phenomenon occurring somewhere between 6 and 8 years of age. At this time, paradigmatic responses, or responses belonging to the same grammatical form class as the stimulus word (e.g. soft-hard, foot-shoe) replace syntagmatic responses, which belong to a form class other than that of the stimulus word (e. g. soft-blanket, foot-walk), as the most popular type of response in a word association test. Although this shift has been well-documented only for American studies, analysis of research of English, French, German and Italian primary responses shows that the tendency towards paradigmatic responses exists at least for French and German adults as well (See Table 1).2 However, according to a separate study done by Moran (1973), Japanese and Western adults show greatly differing word association response pat-terns. Moran found that Japanese and American children had identical response patterns, giving mostly enactive (usually verb-noun pairs) responses. However, adult responses differed in that Japanese preferred iconic (usually adj.-noun pairs) answers and Americans tended towards logical answers (same class with a categorical relation). Comparing Moran's work with the S-P shift theory, a contradiction appears. The American results from Moran's work are in accordance with the S-P shift theory, as the enactive responses preferred by children are syntagmatic (different form class) and the logical (adult) responses are paradigmatic (same form class). However, judging from Moran's findings, the S-P shift did not take place in Japanese responses, as the popular Japanese adult response category (iconic) is syntagmatic, not paradigmatic. In this study, a similar word-association test was administered to several Table 1. Comparison of syntagmatic vs. paradigmatic primary responses in English, French, German, Italian and Japanese. SYNTAGMATIC (Total) End. Ic. PARADIGMATIC Total Fun. Log. Total English(15) 0 0 0 4 11 15 French(15) 1 0 1 4 10 14 German(15) 0 0 0 4 11 15 Italian(15) 6 0 6 2 7 9 Data from table of primary responses in Rosenzweig (1961) to 10 nouns, 5 adjectives. English(100) 2 9 11 89 French(100) 1 11 12 88 German(100) 5 6 11 89 Data from table of primary responses in Russell and Meseck (1959) to 71 nouns, 29 adjectives. English(14) 1 1 2 3 9 12 Japanese(14) 7 1 8 6 0 6 Data from primary responses of present study to 9 nouns, 5 adjectives. ※Data from a separate study by Ervin and Landar (1961) on Navaho word associations may also be used for comparison. The total para-digmatic response rates per form class for Navaho adults were N -59% , A - 42%. V - 52%, which are basically comparable to the N 75.6%, A-57.8%. V-48.9% obtained in this study for American adults. Although the N-values are similar, the other values contrast dramatically with the N-55.6%, A-7.3%, V-4.4% obtained for Japanese adults in the present study. groups of subjects. Results were analyzed for consistency with both the S-P shift and Moran's findings, in an attempt to verify the lack of S-P shift in Japanese and discover possible explanations for it's cause. METHOD A list of 30 words was administered to some 170 subjects classified into one of the following 9 categories : 1. American 3-5-year-olds. 2. American 8-12-year-olds. 3. American adults (over 17). 4. Japanese 3-5year-olds. 5. Japanese 8-12-year-olds. 6. Japanese adults (over 17). 7. Japanese intermediate speakers of English. 8. Japanese advanced speakers of English. 9. Japanese expert speakers of English. Groups 4-6 were tested in Japanese; all others were tested in English. Subjects for all groups were a mixture of sex and (in case of the adults) working status, with the exception of Japanese intermediate and expert English speakers. (The former were all female - mainly housewives, and the latter were all male university professors of English.) The stimulus word list consisted of 10 nouns, 10 verbs and 10 adjectives (see App. I for stimulus words and primary adult responses), and was administered orally with instructions to elicit “the first response that comes to your mind". All subjects except for 3-5-year-olds were tested in groups and gave their responses in writing. Due to their young age, the 3-5-year-olds were tested individually and their oral responses were recorded by the interviewer.3 ANALYSIS Analysis was performed on a data sample composed of five responses per subject group for each stimulus word, taken in such a way that unclassifiable and blank responses were avoided as much as possible. Three stimulus words, one in each grammatical form class, were disregarded due to various reasons (see Appendix). Therefore a total of 5 responses for 9 words in each form class category, or 45 responses x 3 form class categories=135 responses overall per subject group were used. The data was first categorized according to Moran's 4-category system as follows: (1) iconic, or quality-oriented (usually adj-noun, noun adj-pairs), such as apple-red, (2) enactive, or action-oriented (usually noun-verb,verb-noun pairs), such as apple-eat, (3) functional (typically responses of the same form class with a functional relation) such as apple-tree, and (4) logical (synonyms, antonyms, super-, sub- and co-ordinates), such as apple-orange or apple-fruit.4 The same data was then reanalyzed as syntagmatic or paradigmatic, and the percentages of each type were calculated. Syntagmatic response percent-ages were calculated from iconic and enactive class data, and paradigmatic responses from functional and logical class data. DISCUSSION Results of Present Data vs. the S-P Shift As can be seen from Table 2, there is a shift in American responses from 66.7% syntagmatic in 3-5-year-olds to 60.7% paradigmatic in adults, which is in full accordance with the S-P shift theory. Also, the American adult paradigmatic response rates according to grammatical form class (75% for nouns, 57% for adjectives and 49% for verbs) are in agreement with the findings of Fillenbaum (1965), in whose report the paradigmatic response Table 2. Percentages of syntagmatic vs. paradigmatic responses for each subject category according to form class of stimulus word. percentages were 79% for nouns, 65% for adjectives and 43% for verbs. As expected, the Japanese responses did not show a general S-P shift. In fact, this type of shift was present only in adult responses to nouns (55% paradigmatic). For verbs and adjectives, the responses remained syntagmatic throughout all age groups. It is interesting to note the slight trend towards P responses for verbs and adjectives (but NOT for nouns) in 8-12-year-old Japanese; however, this disappeared again in the adults. The total Japanese paradigmatic response percentage increased only slightly (+ 15.5%) with age. Japanese people tested in the English language showed a greater tendency towards P responses for nouns than those tested in Japanese. In fact, the P-response percentage for nouns is equivalent to or exceeds that of the native speakers, even for those students of only intermediate ability.5 As with the native language Japanese responses, however, no paradigmatic tendency was evident for verb and adjective responses in lower-ability subjects. The expert class, though, did show a tendency towards adjectival and verbal P-responses, as well as an overall paradigmatic response percentage rate which was equivalent to that of the American adults, seemingly indicating that they were learning to “think" with American response patterns. We will see that the Moran types for these paradigmatic responses are somewhat different, however. Results of Present Data vs. Moran's Findings The results of classification of the present data as iconic, enactive, function-al or logical according to Moran's 4-category system can be seen in Table 3. We see here that, in accordance with Moran's findings, 3-5-year-olds of both cultures show a preference for enactive responses, differing in this respect from their elders. Indeed it is interesting to note that the totals for all response categories in the 3-5-year-old,'groups were quite similar, except Also in agreement with Moran's results and reflective of the paradigmatic shift in American adults is their overwhelming tendency to use logical responses. This turns out to be the dominant response category for American older children and adults irregardless of the form class of the stimulus word. There is one area in which, the present results do not agree with those of Moran, and that is in the lack of, an overwhelming preference-over 70% as reported by Moran-by Japanese adults towards iconic responses. Although the dominant response category for Japanese adults was iconic, the response rate was less than 41%, and it was "overwhelmingly" present only in the case of an adjectival stimulus word. The Japanese adult response rate for nouns was predominantly functional, meaning that the, overall iconic response .rate ,was due . mainly to responses, to adjectives.6 However. a preference for iconic responses to nouns is present in Japanese 8-12*year-olds. This will be discussed further below. Base Pattern and Deviant N-Patterns Upon closer observation of the results, we find a common bond between all t (primary , or secondary) responses to adjectives (= groups in the dominant (primary iconic) and verbs (=enactive), and all adult groups predominantly gave functional responses to nouns as well. From a grammatical standpoint, this would mean all noun responses, independent of the form class of the stimulus word. Although older American primary responses for all form classes Table 3. Comparison of responses using Moran 4-category system. were in the logical category, their secondary responses showed this same pattern of N-functional, A-iconic, V-enactive. Thus, it may be possible that this response style represents a sort of base pattern, which could be present universally.7 The only deviant form class was N, and it is worthwhile to take a closer look at the two deviant N-patterns found in the table. The first deviant N-pattern was present in both Japanese and American 3-5-year-old groups and favored enactive responses to nouns over the functional base pattern responses. This has already been shown to be in accordance with Moran's data, and is predictable from the S-P shift, since syntagmatic (i.e. enactive and iconic) responses should be predominant in this group. As with the adult patterns, the question arises as to whether this response style could be universal for small children. A similar test was conducted by Moran (1974) using Chinese children as subjects, and these were found to have the same enactive response style as in the present experiment.8 The other deviant N-pattern, present in the Japanese 8-12-year-olds, was mentioned briefly earlier. This is the iconic response pattern for nouns which was dominant in Moran's data for Japanese adults.9 The responses of this group actually show a tendancy away from the pattern of the adult group-not only in the deviant N-pattern but also in the relatively large numbers of functional or logical responses to adjectives and functional or enactive responses to verbs. There is no clear-cut explanation for the appearance of this response style in this age group, and no emergence of any comparable pattern in the data of the American children. Spontaneity response mechanism Another interesting observation from Table 3 is the much more frequent usage of echo answers by all American groups as compared with Japanese. This suggests the possibility of a more “spontaneous" response mechanism in Americans. It could well be that this “spontaneity" mechanism also accounts in part for the shift towards logical responses in older children and adults, allowing the possibility of repeating, without thinking, what was learned in school days, i.e. synonyms, antonyms, classifications, etc. Striking evidence for this possibility comes from the study of Harry W. Crane (1915) regarding reaction time. In that study, 30 American subjects were asked to undergo a word-association experiment under stressful conditions; i.e. they were informed that they could be required to justify their responses or response interval at any time. In addition, some of the participants were requested to deliberately avoid disclosing certain information in their responses. Under these circumstances, it is understandable that responses would tend to be less spontaneous and more <"guarded" than usual. The results show a marked preference for the basic response pattern (i.e. the Japanese pattern) in 22 subjects. Only 4 subjects showed the normal American logical response patterns, while the remaining 4 showed a mixture of the two. CONCLUSIONS We see from the present data that, as suspected, the S-P shift as document-ed in previous literature does not occur in Japanese cognitive development, except to some extent for nouns. Even in this case, however, the trend is more toward functional responses than the logical responses preferred by Americans. This is basically true irregardless of the test language, although expert speakers of English show a tendency towards paradigmatic responses. Two questions arise from these results. First of all, even though small children of both cultures show the same cognitive patterns, these patterns became markedly different in the respective adult subjects. Why should this happen? The answer to this question could lie in any of (or in a combination of) many possible fields of study. A possibility of learned spontaniety as a factor has already been suggested. Cultural and/or educational differences could provide a plausible surface explanation for the difference in response patterns (i.e. Eastern people are taught to think and respond to life emotionally, whereas Westerners value logic and reason more highly). However, more consideration would have to be given here in light of the large number of syntagmatic responses made by Italians (mentioned in the introduction of this report and in Table 1) and for the likelihood (based on hearsay through informants of that language) that Korean responses would be similar to American ones, although both the Korean language and culture have strong similarities and connections with Japanese. The second question arises concerning the possibility of a "basic response pattern" of N-functional, A-iconic, V-enactive for adults, and of N-enactive, A-iconic, V-enactive for young children. Although further study is needed, it seems rather safe to conclude that the childrens' enactive response style to nouns is to some extent universal. Could it be that the adult pattern is also universal and is foreshadowed by the - possibly more spontaneous - logical response in some cultural or linguistic groups? If so, why should this foreshadowing occur? This is a possibility which would require further experimentation to answer satisfactorily. NOTES 1. The distinction between syntagmatic-paradigmatic (also called "heterogenous-homogenous") and syntactic-paradigmatic has been discussed widely (McNeill, 1963; Entwisle, 1964). It will suffice to say that since most comparable studies have been analyzed using the simpler heterogenous-homogenous distinction (based on form class only), the same distinction has been used in the present study. 2. The similarity between the low rate for paradigmatic primary responses (40-60% vs. 86-100% in other languages) and the wide use of enactive responses in both Italian and Japanese in Table I is interesting and merits confirmatory study and analysis. 3. Much attention has been given to the possibility of differences in response due to method of administration (oral or written). Palermo (1963) found a higher degree of paradigmatic responses associated with the oral response procedure in the case of 5th graders, which would indicate that the S-P shift would be even more pronounced if an oral procedure were used for all ages across the board. According to other studies (Entwisle and Forsyth, 1963; Entwisle. 1966) there seems to be no major effect. 4. A fifth <'echo" response group was included in this study for a type of response typical of 3-5-year-olds in which gestures, motions, sounds, and nonsense-or rhyming-words (such as apple-papple) were used. This <<echo" group is similar to the <'clang" response category used by Ervin (1961), who found that this category was widely used by kindergarten-age children, but decreased rapidly with age. 5. This tendency does not reflect a true correlation with the cognitive process in English, as the Moran-classification type for these P responses is mostly functional, not logical as in the case of the American responses. 6. The stimulus words used by Moran were not balanced between form classes, consisting of 68.2% nouns, 21.2% adjectives and only 10.6% verbs. Judging from the different values obtained for responses according to form class, this factor could have had a significant effect on the data. 7. Indeed, since there is a general tendency to use nouns more than other form classes overall in responding, it is common sense that most responses to verbs would be enactive (V-N pairs), to adjectives, iconic (A-N pairs), and to nouns, logical or functional (N-N pairs). 8. It is regrettable that in that study adults were not tested as well, as it would have been quite informative to know what type of response pattern was dominant in Chinese adults. 9. Moran himself suggests that the response patterns of older children and adults are roughly equivalent, leading us to question the average age of his “adult" subjects, which is not made completely clear in that study. References Brown, R. and Berko, J. Word Association and the Acquisition of Grammar. Child Development. 1960, 31, 1-14 Carroll, J. B., Kjeldergaard, P. M. and Carton. A. S. Number of opposites vs. Number of primaries as a response measure in free association tests Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1962, 1, 22-30. Clark. E. V. On the Child's Acquisition of Antonyms in two Semantic Fields. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1972, Il, 750-758. Crane, H.W. A study in association reaction and reaction time. Psychological Monographs, 1915, 18, No. 80. Deese, J. Form class and the determinants of word association. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1962, l, 79-84. Emerson, H.and Gekoski, W. Interactive and Categorical Grouping Strategies and the Syntagmatic-Paradigmatic Shift. Child Development. 1976, 47, 1116-1121 Entwisle, D. The Syntactic-Paradigmatic Shift in Children's Word Associations. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1964, 3, 19-29 Entwisle, D. R. Form class and children's word associations. Journal of Verbal Learnileg aud Verbal Behavior. 1966, 5, 558-565. Entwisle, D. R. Developmental sociolinguistics : inner-city children American Jour-nal of Sociology, 1968, 74, 37-49. Entwisle. D. R and Forsyth, D. F. Word associations of children : the effect of method of administration, Ps_vchological Reports. 1963, 13, 291-229. Ervin. S M. Changes with Age in the Verbal Determinants of word-association, American Journal of Psychology. 1961, 74, 361-372. Ervin. S.M. and Landar H. Navaho Word Associations. American Journal of Psychology. 1963, 76, 49-57. Fillenbaum. S. and Jones, L V.Grammatical contingencies in word association. Journal of Verbal Learnileg aud Verbal Behavior. 1965, 4, 248-255. Francis, H. Toward an explanation of the syntagmatic-paradigmatic shift. Child Development. 1972, 43, 949-958. Heidenheimer, P. Logical relations in the semantic processing of children between six and ten: emergence of antonym and synonym categorization. Child Development, 1978, 49, 1243-1246. Jenkins, J. J. and Russell, W. A Systematic changes in word association norms: 1910-1952. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1960, 60, 293-304. Lippman, M.Z. Correlates of Contrast Word Associations: developmental trends. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1971, lO, 392-399. McNeill, D., The origin of associations within the same grammatical class Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1963, 2, 250-262. McNeill, D. A Study of word Association Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1966, 5, 548-557. Moran, L. J Generality of Word Association Response sets. Psychological monographs. 1966, 80, 4, Whole No. 612. Moran, L. J Comparative Growth of Japanese and North American Cognitive Dictionaries. Child Development. 44, 862-865. Moran, L.J. and Huang, I. N Note on cognitive dictionary structure of Chinese children. Psychological Reports, 1974, 34, 154. Moran, L. J., Mefferd, R. B, and Kimble, J. P Idiodynamic Sets in Word Association. Psychological Monographs. 1964, 78, 2, Whole No. 579. Murphy, R. An experimental study of literary vs. scientific types. American Journal of Psychology. 28, 238-26-?theory. Psychological Bulletin, 1977, 84, 93-1l6. Palermo. D. S. Characteristics of word association responses obtained from children in grades one through four. Developmental Psychology, 1971, 5, 118-123. Palermo. D. S. and Jenkins, J. J. Frequency of superordinate responses to a word association test as a function of age. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal, Behavior, 1963. 1. 378-383. Penk. W. E. Developmental changes in idiodynamic sets of children’s word associations. Developmental psychology, 1971, 5, 55-63. Petrey. S. Word associations and the development of lexical memory. Cognition, 1977, 5, 57-71. Rosenzweig. M. R. Comparisons among word-association responses in English, French, German and Italian. American Journal of Psychology. 1961, 74, 347-360. Rosenzweig, M. Word associations of French workmen : comparisons with associations of French students and American workmen and students. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1964, 3, l7-59. Russell, W. A. and Meseck O. R. Der Einfluss der Assoziation auf das Errinern von Worten in der Deutschen, Franzosischen und Englischen Sprache, Z. exp. angew. Psychool, 1959,6, 191-211.