Cultural Support for School: Contrasts Between Japan and the United States By: Robert D. Hess and Hiroshi Azuma From: Educational Researcher, December, 1991 J. Geffen 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 1. Although learning is a natural process, schools are not naturally conducive to learning. Children are compelled to attend; they have little choice in the content of a curriculum whose value may not be apparent; they must share the teacher’s time and other resources with peers; classmates differ from one another in ability and experience, requiring many of them to deal with an instructional tempo not suited to their interest or preparation; and they are governed by a set of rules about personal acts such as talking, moving around, and attending to physical needs. In short, schools are not user-friendly. 2. Some students are more ready than others to deal with these conditions – a readiness that comes from the attributes they bring to the classroom. These facilitate classroom learning by increasing motivation to achieve, promoting on-task behavior, and simplifying the task of the teacher. These dispositions are not primarily cognitive, in the usual sense, but a cluster of socio-emotional tendencies. They include, as a partial list, the following: (a) willingness to master skills and engage in tasks, whether appealing or not; (b) readiness to accept the curriculum of the school; (c) willingness to accept the rules of the school and authority of the teacher; (d) ability to concentrate and to persist in tasks and complete them on time; (e) readiness to monitor one’s own behavior and performance, giving attention to detail and to quality of work; (f) ability to work independently outside of school or in the classroom while the teacher is engaged with other students; and (g) willingness to accept rules of social behavior necessary for learning in groups. 3. Teachers can deal with the aversive conditions intrinsic in schooling by promoting adaptive dispositions or can mitigate their impact by using techniques that engage the student and make the learning situation more appealing, or both. Some cultures – in this discussion, Japan – seem to be particularly concerned about developing adaptive dispositions in students. Other cultures focus more on modifying the classroom environment. We suggest that international discrepancies in achievement arise in part from differences in the relative emphasis on these strategies. 4. The idea that there is an interaction between dispositions and the conditions of schooling follows Epstein’s (1983) elaboration of the ATI model to apply to interactions between attributes acquired outside the school and the classroom climate created by the teacher. We extend this frame to include cultures as sources of students’ adaptive dispositions. 5. The evidence we present is intended to be persuasive and illustrative rather than definitive. It draws primarily from studies of Japanese and American families and Cultural Support for Schooling: Japan and the U.S. / 2 40 45 50 55 60 65 70 75 schools, but the underlying principles apply to other comparisons both within and between nations. Much of the evidence offered here comes from our longitudinal study of socialization and education in Japan and the United States (Azumea, Kashiwagi, & Hess, 1982; Hess, Azuma et al., 1986). The purpose of the study was to analyze differences between the two countries in the way families prepared children for schooling. Our data are supplemented by results from opinion surveys, laboratory studies, and observation of families, included not as a full review of relevant research, but as evidence that our results are not idiosyncratic. 6. We worked with mothers and their four-year-old preschool children (58 families in Japan and 67 in the United States), gathering data when the children were four, five and six years of age and in a follow-up study when children in Japan were 11 years old (Grade 5) and when those in the United States were age 12 (Grade 6). In the first study, mothers were interviewed, children were given a variety of tasks to elicit school-relevant behavior, and mother-child pairs worked together on three tasks. At the follow-up period, both mothers and children were interviewed separately on their beliefs about the reasons why children did well or did not do well in school. Children were also given tests of mathematics skill and vocabulary. Although the research groups were samples of convenience, not representative of the two countries in a statistical sense, they came from comparable socioeconomic (SES) levels and urban/rural areas of residence in the two countries. Parents’ Goals in Japan and the United States 7. The belief of parents in Japan and the United States, although overlapping, sketch distinctive definitions of the kind of persons they want their children to become. In our study, data on the behavior that parents desire came from responses to the developmental Expectations Questionnaire (Hess, Kashiwagi, Azuma, Price, & Dickson, 1980), an instrument that asked mothers to indicate the age at which they expected their child to master specific skills. Expecting early mastery was considered to be an indicator of high priority. The largest divergence between the two groups was on items concerned with emotional maturity (e.g., “Does not cry easily.”) For a scale on which 1 = age six years or older, and 3 = younger than four years old, the mean for Japanese (J).) mothers was 2.49; for American (U.S.) mothers, 2.08, a significantly older age. The groups also differed significantly on items dealing with compliance: for example, “Comes or answers when called” (J. = 2.16; U.S. = 1.96). American mothers expected other types of competence at earlier ages: social skills, for example, “Takes initiative in playing with others” (J. = 1.86; U.S. = 2.18); and verbal assertiveness, for example, “Asks for explanations when in doubt” (J. = 1.73; U.S. = 2.18). The smallest differences appeared on items about school-related skills. 8. These results are congruent with an opinion survey conducted by the Japanese Office of the Prime Minister (Sorifu, 1982). Five hundred mothers in Japan and 500 in the United States selected the three most important items out of 13 that described desirable characteristics of children. Words chosen most frequently by Japanese Cultural Support for Schooling: Japan and the U.S. / 3 80 85 90 95 100 105 110 115 mothers were basic habits (every-day self-care practices, 34.1%), compliance (37.8%), and patience (31.9%). In contrast, American mothers more often selected independence (41.7%), basic habits (34.1%), and tolerance of differences of opinion (31.1%). Compliance was selected by only 9.5% of American mothers, and patience was selected by only 19.7%. The Japanese mothers stressed diligence; American mothers were more concerned with independence and acceptance of diversity. 9. These distinctions are consistent with other studies, summarized by Yamamura (1986) and White and LeVine (1986), which conclude that in Japan a child is thought to be good if he or she is “obedient in good grace” (sunao), “mild and gentle” (otonashii), and “self-controlled” (jiseishin ga aru). In the United States, the “good child” is more likely to be assertive, independent, courteous, and socially competent with peers. 10. Thus Japanese mothers favor skills that promote group cooperation and compliance with authority; American mothers expect children to develop initiative and verbal assertiveness – skills that promote independence and effective interaction with peers. Qualities of self-control, patience, harmony, and compliance are more likely to facilitate a smooth transition to conventional school cultures than are qualities of verbal assertiveness, independence, and taking initiative. Preparing Children to be Students 11. Although such cultural goals for children refer to general behavior, it is no surprise that parents expect children to exhibit such characteristics in their encounters with teachers. Adults in Japan are particularly eager to prepare the child to be diligent and to cooperate with the teacher. In the United States, more concern is focused on acquiring academic (especially verbal) skills, independence, and self-reliance. One questionnaire study (Tobin, Wu, & Davidson, 1989) of 300 Japanese and 210 American administrators and parents on beliefs about what children should learn in preschools illustrates these contrasts: perseverance (J. = 16%; U.S. = 5%); sympathy/empathy/concern for others (J. = 80%; U.S. = 39%); beginning reading and math skills (J. = 1%; U.S. = 22%); self-reliance/self-confidence (J. = 44%; U.S. = 73%); communication skills (J. = 5%; U.S. = 38%); good health/hygiene/grooming habits (J. = 49%; U.S. = 7%); and creativity (J. = 30%; U.S. = 37%). 12. Parents also orient children towards their roles as students by instilling beliefs about the causes of failure and success in school and the degree to which the child is responsible for performance. Mothers and children in Japan tend to see the causes of performance as internal; their counterparts in the United States more often include sources that are beyond the child’s control and thus not the child’s responsibility. 13. Data supporting this contrast came from the follow-up phase of our study. Mothers and their children, who were then 11-12 years old, responded to questions about why students succeed or fail in mathematics by assigning 10 points among six possible causes for level of achievement. On reasons for failure, the responses of Japanese mothers peaked sharply on “lack of effort”. On a scale from 0-10, Japanese Cultural Support for Schooling: Japan and the U.S. / 4 120 125 130 135 140 145 150 155 mothers had a mean score of 6.17 and a median of 6. The next highest alternative was “lack of ability”, with a mean of 1.48. 14. American mothers assigned blame more often to sources outside the child or over which the child had little control. Lack of effort, while a major player, was less salient than in Japan (U.S. = 3.09 vs. 6.17 for Japan); lack of ability was given more weight (U.S. = 2.62 vs. 1.48 for Japan), and more blame was also placed on the schools (U.S. = 2.26 vs. 0.77 for Japan). 15. The two groups of children also showed contrasts, with even more emphasis by American children on lack of ability (U.S. = 3.38 vs. 1.93 in Japan) and less on lack of effort (U.S. = 1.98 vs. 3.58 in Japan). American children were also more likely to blame the school for the child’s lack of success. One consequence of students’ blaming failure on conditions that they cannot control (ability, external factors) is that they are then less likely to make an effort to improve their performance (Holloway & Hess, 1984). 16. Although parental behavior is guided by ideas about what they would like their children to become, these parental concepts are in turn shaped by cultural norms. In societies with considerable internal consensus, these expectations take on the force of public definitions of social behavior. In Japan, the child is trained to conform to the norm governing the role he happens to be playing. The role is first defined by sex, age, and birth order. Generally, younger children are more indulged than older children, but each child is told to behave properly “like” a male child or “like” a female child. The child may also be instructed to behave “like” the son of a farmer, a merchant, a doctor, and so forth. Such role discipline increases when the child reaches school age. He is told that now that he is a schoolboy he must obey his teacher. The parents go so far as to undermine their own authority by stressing the schoolteacher’s authority. The child learns his new role as a pupil in a rather abrupt way, and is motivated to concentrate his energy on performing the role. (Lebra, 1976, p. 151) 17. In Japan, such role definitions add a powerful external incentive for conformity because publicly defined roles are enforced by a wide range of adults in the child’s life, including strangers. Dispositions Begin in Preschool 18. If student characteristics are indeed culturally based rather than, or as well as, products of schooling itself, we would expect them to appear during the preschool years. This seems to be the case. Even casual observations of preschool classes reveal national differences in children’s behavior. For example, visitors to Japanese nursery schools from other countries are often puzzled at the sight of more than 30 three-yearold children sitting quietly on the floor of a classroom, listening attentively to the teacher, who is often a young and inexperienced girl. Frequently, the visitor’s first Cultural Support for Schooling: Japan and the U.S. / 5 160 165 170 175 180 185 190 195 guess is that the exuberance of students is held in check by strict discipline, but closer examination shows that this is not so (Lewis, 1984). 19. Cultural distinctions appeared in the problem-solving strategies of four-year-old preschoolers in our study. Japanese children took more time to solve tasks and made fewer errors than did their American counterparts. These differences were observed in three activities: the Matching Familiar Figures Test (MFF), a tactile-visual crossmodal instrument developed for the study; and the Draw-A-Circle-Slowly Task, a technique designed to assess impulsiveness. 20. The MFF presented the child with several pages of line drawings of familiar figures (e.g., kite, cat) on one page (the standard), while a facing page showed an array of five similar drawings, only one of which was exactly like the standard. The task was to select the figure that exactly corresponded to the model. Performance was measured by accuracy and delay-time to first choice. American children, as a group, waited 57.5 seconds across the set of drawings and made 11.4 errors. Japanese children took much more time, waiting 76.5 seconds, and also made fewer errors (9.9). Other studies show a similar pattern (Sallkind, Kojima, & Zelniker, 1978; Smith & Caplan, 1988). 21. In the cross-modal transfer task, children were shown a small box with a door covered by a cloth. The tester placed (out of view of the child) a small abstract plastic shape inside the box. The child could feel the object with her or his hand but could not see it. The child’s task was to explore the object in the box with his or her fingers and select the correct corresponding figure from an array displayed on a page next to the box. Again, the Japanese children took longer on this task (80.5 seconds) than did the children from the U.S. (55.3 seconds). They also made slightly fewer errors (4.8 vs. 4.4). The third task, Draw-A-Circle-Slowly, required the child to draw a circle as “slowly as you can”. Again, on average, Japanese children took more time to complete the task: Japan = 16.5 seconds to 11.5 seconds for children in the United States. 22. The difference in persistence and attention to instructions observed in these tasks indicate that ideals and values expressed by parents appear in the behavior of children before they reach the classroom. Cultivating Adaptive Dispositions 23. In both Japan and the United States, the interaction of parents with their children is consistent with the dispositions they want their children to acquire. We distinguish between two distinctive but overlapping cultural modes through which cultural transmission takes place: osmosis, in which nurturance, interdependence, and close physical proximity provide exposure to adult values and instil a readiness on the part of the child to imitate, accept, and internalize such values, and teaching, in which direct instruction, injunctions, frequent dialogue, and explanations are used. Although socialization in any culture may use both approaches, the emphasis differs from one Cultural Support for Schooling: Japan and the U.S. / 6 200 205 210 215 220 225 230 235 culture to another. In our view, parents in Japan are more inclined to use osmosis, and those in the United States are more inclined to use teaching. 24. The osmosis approach is best represented in Kadensho, a book written in the 13th century by a Noh play master, Zeami. Discussing how to rear children of a Noh family who are to succeed in the family business, he advised that children not be taught Noh dancing before they are six years old but that they should be allowed to visit freely the studio where their parents and relatives practice. They will begin to imitate parts of dancing spontaneously without being told. Parents are to observe them in these initial efforts but not make any evaluational comment, either positive or negative. Natural imitation will reveal each child’s strength. 25. Several conditions enhance osmosis: (a) a close physical proximity between the child and her or his socializing agent for long periods of time, (b) minimizing the difference between the child and the socializing agent, and (c) encouraging the child to be empathic to others, particularly the socializing agent. Under these conditions, it is as if the cognitive “film” that separates the child and the socializing agent becomes thin and permeable; feelings and behavior patterns of the socializing agent penetrate even without injunctions and intentional teaching. 26. A number of the differences between Japanese and American parenting reported in many studies over the past few decades may be understood by recognizing that Japanese practices are directed toward satisfying these conditions. The tendency of Japanese mothers to stay physically close to their infants for a long time has often been described. They also tend to encourage empathy by calling the child’s attention to feelings and relationships. For example, in a block-sorting task in our study, about half of the Japanese mothers used a family analogy (e.g., calling the tall blocks “parents” and the short blocks “little sister”) at least once in the interaction. American mothers rarely used such images. 27. Empathy is also encouraged in disciplinary encounters. In an interview about strategies that mothers would use to stop undesirable behavior or encourage desired responses, far more Japanese mothers appealed to feelings as a basis for gaining compliance (e.g., the pain the farmer who grew the vegetable would feel if the vegetable he grew were not eaten). In this interview, we used six hypothetical situations that would often elicit intervention (e.g., “Imagine that your child is here and that you found that she had been writing on the wall. What would you say?”). Other situations were refusing to eat vegetables at dinner, refusing to brush teeth, running around in a grocery market, and the like (Conroy, Hess, Azuma, & Kashiwagi, 1980). On these items American mothers relied more often on commands and status (U.S. = 50%; J. = 18%); the Japanese relied more on appeals to feelings (U.S. = 7%; J. = 22%) and consequences (U.S. = 23%; J. = 37%). The two groups were evenly divided on appeals to rules (U.S. = 16%; J. = 15%). Appeals to authority and rules, with the implicit threats of punishment or rewards, thus accounted for two- Cultural Support for Schooling: Japan and the U.S. / 7 240 245 250 255 260 265 270 275 thirds of mothers’ responses in the United States but for only one-third in Japan, where feelings and consequences were more favored. 28. In these situations, Japanese mothers tended to avoid explicit confrontation lest it damage the closeness with their child. In encounters where the child persisted in resisting the mother’s efforts (e.g., eat vegetables), about 20% of the Japanese mothers who started with a firm demand for compliance gradually moderated their demands, often yielding altogether. Except in situations that presented danger, few mothers attempted to gain compliance by using their authority as a rationale. More often they used a strategy of damashi – a kind of deception in which the child’s attention was diverted so that he or she would make the compliant response without being aware of the underlying dispute with the mother. American mothers were more willing to take a hard line with the child, following a familiar injunction in childrearing advice to parents to be consistent in their discipline. 29. Although a close relationship between mother and child is conductive to acquiring the mother’s behavior and values, teaching relationships, which tend to utilize explanations and instruction, are rooted in status distinctions. The superior knowledge and position of the teacher form the basis for persuasion. 30. Japanese mothers are much less willing than American mothers to take the role of teacher. When we asked mothers if they had taught or would teach their child several school-specific skills, Japanese mothers much less often reported that they taught or should teach preschool skills – for example, letters (characters), numbers, and shapes. However, Japanese children acquire such skills before they begin school and may be more likely to do so than are American children (Muraishi & Amano, 1972). Although some may have been learned from schooling outside the home, it is also likely that such skills were acquired incidentally from modeling, stimulation, and opportunities provided by the family. 31. One example of how Japanese mothers guide their children’s problem-solving activities was observed in a referential communication task (Dickson, Hess, Miyake, & Azuma, 1979). This technique requires one person to describe to another an object that the other person cannot see. Dickson used an inverted notebook with pages presenting four figures, one of which was the target. The mother was asked to describe this figure to the child, who was then to push a button under the correct pattern on her or his page. 32. Analysis of the transcripts, in both English and Japanese (Miyake, 1977), showed that American mothers focused more often on the need to produce a correct answer while Japanese mothers encouraged their children to be more deliberate. The task required the mother to do two things: describe the target and instruct the child to push the button to indicate his or her choice. An American mother might say to her child, “Can you push the red button under the picture that looks like a triangle?” A Japanese mother might say, “I see a shape that looks like a crooked triangle. Do you see something that looks a little bit like a triangle?” Cultural Support for Schooling: Japan and the U.S. / 8 280 285 290 295 300 305 310 315 33. The emphasis by mothers in the United States on the button-push drew the child’s attention to producing an answer promptly; indeed, sometimes the child pushed before the mother completed here description. In the Japanese group, however, the child was more likely to point to the picture before pushing the button, as if the emphasis were on analyzing the features of the figure rather than on a prompt physical response. 34. The distinction that Miyake’s analysis makes between “Can you …” and “Do you …” is not trivial; it casts the task in different terms – one as a test, the other as more of a game which, to be sure, has a goal but which also puts more emphasis on the process. The implication of evaluation was reinforced in the United States by the mother’s response, which was most often a signal of the correctness or incorrectness of the child’s action: “Right,” “OK,” or “Fine.” In Japan, however, the connotation was different. Mothers responded with Atari, meaning “hit.” While “Right” or “OK” refer to correctness, the Japanese term Atari refers to the probabilistic chance of a correct response – the effectiveness of efforts to reach a goal that one is approaching. In defining the task as a game, the Japanese mothers subtly showed their tendency to approve any response at some level. 35. The Japanese method of socializing children typically relies on modeling, while Western methods predominantly use a hedonistically oriented (i.e., reward-based) training strategy. The keys for the former are attention to and close identification with others. The keys for the latter are to set one’s own goal, be clear about what one wants, and view socializing persons as “others” in a position of authority, able to offer punishments or rewards contingent on one’s behavior. Their influence depends on their explicit authority – the ability to deliver rewards or sanctions – not solely on their place in a social network. Efficiency versus Reflectiveness in Japanese Education 36. Teachers in both countries facilitate the child’s encounter with the school but in different ways. In Japan, they exploit and encourage adaptive dispositions that students bring to the classroom. In the United States, they are more likely to try to alter the conditions that students confront. These different strategies produce quite different educational climates in the two countries. 37. The common perception that instruction in Japanese classrooms consists primarily of drill and repetition deserves a note of historical context. In Japanese classroom instruction, two major, apparently conflicting, trends seem to coexist, with roots in an osmosis orientation of socialization. One is the traditional belief that repetition is a route to understanding – a tendency to mold the learner’s response into a required form prior to analytical understanding. 38. This ancient tradition, most typically seen in the training of classical performing artists (Noh plays are an obvious example), continues to influence behavior of teachers. In Japan, to defend the use of repetition, the teacher may quote old sayings and proverbs: “Read it one hundred times, and understanding will follow Cultural Support for Schooling: Japan and the U.S. / 9 320 325 330 335 340 345 350 355 spontaneously.” “First master the pattern. Then you can outgrow the pattern.” “Practice so it becomes automatic, rather than trying to learn intellectually.” 39. This custom of learning traditional knowledge and skills through repetition is not unique to the Japanese. It is a powerful socializing technique, designed to transmit adult norms unchanged, used in societies that are relatively closed and resistant to change. Japan was a closed state until quite recently, about 150 years ago, and much national tradition was carried forward into modern-school education. 40. Reading in chorus and answering in unison are techniques frequently used in Japanese classes. When the class answers in unison, even a child who does not quite understand can act as if he or she knows the right answer. This is a commitment to producing an accurate response, and it helps the child incorporate standards of quality. 41. When carried to an extreme that neglects the goal of understanding, such preoccupation with procedure may turn into what Hatano (1983) has characterized as efficiency orientation. Procedural orientation is adaptive to efficiency-driven situations. When a student is to take an entrance examination and has little time to prepare, memorizing algorithms is clearly a workable method for coping with conventional examination questions – a technique consistent with the Japanese belief in the utility of effort. 42. Efficiency orientation is, perhaps, a form of imitation that degenerated under the pressure of modernization in Japan and the emergence of “examination hell.” Traditionally, the use of imitation did not ignore the importance of understanding. On the contrary – procedural mastery was seen as an essential route to conceptual understanding. However, as Hatano pointed out, this secondary goal can easily be forgotten when the need for efficiency is urgent, as it was in Japan in the late 19th century. Convinced of a need to achieve quickly the level of productivity that had been attained in Western countries across two or more centuries, the Japanese felt that efficiency was imperative. The product took precedence over the basic process. “Sticky-Probing versus “Quick and Snappy” 43. The thoroughness that comes from repetition and the emphasis on understanding in Japan led to a unique teaching process when they interacted with the task-loyal receptive diligence of Japanese tradition. We call this the sticky-probing approach (Azuma, 1983). The modus operandi of this approach is to select a seemingly small problem that most of the children would not otherwise notice, probe into it through deliberative group discussion and teacher-pupil exchange, and thus spend considerable time on reflecting, examining, and digesting the problem. It is not unusual to find a fourth-grade teacher spending two class hours discussing only two haiku, that is, two short 17-syllable poems. Indeed, the reading textbook in Japan barely exceeds 100 pages even at the upper grade levels, but 300 classroom hours are allotted to study these pages – three hours to each page! This kind of “sticky probing” demands patience and compliance because children must resist feelings of satiation Cultural Support for Schooling: Japan and the U.S. / 10 360 365 370 375 380 385 390 395 400 and boredom. They are expected to stay with a topic, and to examine an issue from several perspectives, rather than to push quickly for a solution or “correct” answer. 44. The Japanese strategy differs sharply from a common American style that emphasizes dividing lessons into small steps or concepts, each of which is quickly mastered, promptly rewarded, and identified as a correct term, concept, or procedure. 45. Some examples illustrate this contrast. In a study of science education, Azume and Walberg (1985) compared Japanese and American teaching processes. Fifthgrade teachers, four in each country, were asked to teach their pupils about the dissolution of substance in water and to ask students to conduct an experiment. The classroom processes were tape-recorded and video-taped. 46. The topics discussed were usually different in the two countries. All four American classes discussed what they planned or were going to do, the procedures that would be followed, and precautions that they should take. The teacher asked questions that could be answered in short sentences. Clear feedback or acknowledgment from the teacher immediately followed each child’s response. The exchange moved at a brisk pace. Convergence of ideas was not a goal. Often the teacher allowed the discussion to end without a summary statement or conclusion. The American classes that we observed were clear and snappy, encouraging divergent fluency. “Anything else? Anything different?” was the stimulus to which children kept responding. The discussion kept moving on, as if to linger on the same phase or idea might threaten fluency or lose the students’ attention. 47. All four Japanese classes, however, spent considerable time lingering over a substantive question, such as how to ascertain the density of a salt solution without testing it, whether or not some of the weight of the substance would get lost when it dissolved in water, whether salty taste is evidence of salty substance, and the like. The teacher probed and focused, seldom gave clear feedback, and kept his own position vague. At the same time he made sure that the discussion concentrated on the problem he posed at the beginning. After a lengthy discussion that divided the class into groups holding different opinions, an experiment was planned and executed as a means of resolving the conflict. These experiments were carefully designed to persuade students to adopt a conclusion that the teacher approved. 48. The Japanese classes were generally “sticky.” The core of the sticky impression was taking time on the same topic, looking at it from varied perspectives and in a variety of conceptual frameworks. Teachers were successful in making children think, often to an impressive degree. Success, however, depended heavily upon the cooperation of the children with the teacher. Children accepted the problem set by the teacher as their problem, concentrated on the topic, and took a long time to discuss and probe deeply. These observations are consistent with the findings of Stigler and Perry (1988), who report that teachers of mathematics in Sendai, Japan, dealt with fewer topics within a given time span than did teachers in Chicago and that in Japan more time segments included explanations from teachers and students. Cultural Support for Schooling: Japan and the U.S. / 11 405 410 415 420 425 430 435 440 49. Thus molding-through-repetition and the sticky-probe approach, two traditions of Japanese educational practice that otherwise look quite dissimilar, share one feature in common. Students must be ready with facilitating response tendencies: Be compliant rather than self-assertive, work hard without immediate feedback, persist, tolerate satiation, and the like. 50. In Japan, teachability is an internalized receptive diligence that does not depend primarily on the attractiveness of the curriculum or the teacher’s presentation. The term “internalized” is of particular significance: Overt control by the teacher is minimal, and feedback is scarce. Students perceive the situation, realize what is expected of them, and concentrate on living up to the teacher’s expectation. 51. Teachers in Japan begin to encourage these adaptive dispositions in the early grades. Lewis’s (1988) observations of 15 Japanese first-grade classrooms in Tokyo project an image of Japanese teachers who emphasize several supportive qualities: social and emotional development, such as praising other students’ performance, making friends, getting along in a group, including other children, feeling relaxed and emotionally stable; procedures that make for smooth operation of the class – arranging desk contents, using the bathroom, walking in the halls, sitting properly, handwashing; and encouraging responsibility for self and for the class – leading the class in opening and closing greetings, introducing the class period, announcing the subject to be studied, following goals and schedules posted on the walls. Establishing their own authority is not the teachers’ primary goal. One teacher said, in response to a question about the most important thing to be learned during the first months of school, “Following rules? I don’t want to create children who obey (just) because I’m here. I want children who know what to do themselves and who learn to judge things themselves” (Lewis, 1988, p. 164). Early training at school thus supports and continues the socialization experiences of the preschool and out-of-school worlds. Motivation, American Style 52. The two styles – sticky-probing and quick-and-snappy – rely on different concepts of motivation. The internalized sense of diligence and receptiveness fits uncomfortably into the more familiar American concepts of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. In the United States, the teacher often provides specific stimuli or a stimulus environment that appeals to the children, engages them with encouragement and feedback, and moves on briskly. 53. One version of what constitutes effective teaching in American classrooms is described by Brophy and Good (1986) in their chapter in the Handbook of Research on Teaching. These excerpts illustrate our point: General Principles. [italics added] 3. The difficulty level of questions and tasks should be easy enough to allow the lesson to move along at a brisk pace… Cultural Support for Schooling: Japan and the U.S. / 12 445 450 455 460 465 470 475 480 Programming for Continuous Progress. 3. Pace. Both progress through the curriculum and pacing within specific activities should be brisk, producing continuous progress achieved with relative ease (small steps, high success rate). 4. Error rate. Expect to get correct answers to about 80% …. Continue with practice and review until smooth, rapid, correct performance is achieved …. (p. 328) 54. Although not without critics, this approach represents one of the prevalent views of teaching in the United States. Children are enticed into working by presenting tasks in easy steps, by assuring prompt success, and by moving briskly from a completed problem to a new one. The teacher does not count on internalized diligence; the stimulus environment is designed to induce interest and create a motivation to work on the task. 55. The spirit that organized the “new curricula” movement in the United States in the 1960s encouraged discovery and deeper problem solving. To render a discovery “meaningful,” the solution of the problem should not be immediately obvious nor the step size too small. The goal was to utilize intrinsic motivation, tapping an inherent curiosity and desire for knowledge. The construct of intrinsic motivation expanded the concept of motivation beyond reward and punishment. Teachers made material more meaningful, relevant, exciting, briskly paced, and individualized to sustain the intrinsic motivation of learners. In the ideal case, the atmosphere produced was free, colorful, and pleasantly noisy. 56. We suggest, however, that when these ideas are applied in classrooms, it appears that curiosity, exploration, and involvement are usually induced by the stimulus, not brought to the situation by students. Perceived dissonance stimulates an inclination to find a solution to reduce the apparent inconsistency. Indeed, new science classes often start with a demonstration of an unusual, interest-catching phenomenon on which involvement could be based. Thus “intrinsic motivation” is to some degree also stimulus regulated. From a more traditional strategy of teaching by reward and punishment (still often used) to the recent trends, the primary method of motivating students in the United States has become the managing of the social context of learning to provide stimulation. 57. This approach acknowledges the individuality, separateness, and independence of the student. The teacher and curriculum must earn the student’s attention. Students are less likely to be blamed for inattention if the topic and presentation are not appealing. However, it also adds to the task of the teacher, who not only attends to the details and strategies of presenting subject matter but also takes responsibility for engaging the interest of the class. The success and quality of schools are judged by their ability to arouse interest in the student. 58. Given this difference between Japanese and American classrooms in motivational styles, we would expect that students’ dispositions in each country Cultural Support for Schooling: Japan and the U.S. / 13 485 490 495 500 505 510 515 520 would respond selectively to the nature of specific tasks. Data from our study suggest that this is indeed the case. 59. Five-year-old children who could work carefully and diligently on a relatively uninteresting task were more able to profit from Japanese schooling than those who could not. This disposition did not correlate with success in American schools. The children in the United States who worked effectively on tasks that aroused curiosity gained more from schooling than those who did not. This relationship in Japan was similar but less conspicuous. 60. Behavior of these children on a relatively unstructured task showed congruent patterns. Ratings of persistence correlated with later achievement for Japanese children (r = .56), but not for American children (r = .13); ratings of independence/ originality showed significant correlations for the American group (r = .39) but not for Japanese (r = .11). Discussion 61. The suggestion that adaptive dispositions are prerequisite to successful schooling in formal settings raises a question about the role of congruence between the culture of the home and that of the school. We suggest that home-school congruence is relevant primarily where it touches the dispositions that formal schooling requires – a topic that we believe deserves systematic, serious attention, including more formal analysis of the demands that different kinds of educational settings present (e.g., lecture, small group, computer-based, open schools). 62. If questions of similarity were paramount, students who are taken from their own culture and placed in other distinctly dissimilar cultures would usually encounter difficulty. However, this is not necessarily what happens. Students from Japan adapt readily to American classrooms; Japanese and some other Asian students find favor with teachers in the United States (Muro, 1988; Wong, 1980), and Japanese students achieve very well at all levels in schools in the United States (see Sue & Okazaki, 1990, for a discussion of evidence and alternative explanations). The reverse, however, seems not to be the case. We know of no studies of American students in Japanese schools, but the experience of many Japanese children who were educated for a time outside of Japan is relevant. After a few years abroad, upon returning to Japan they often experience difficulty with the scripts of school life – how to deal with peers and teachers (Azuma, Nakazawa, &Yamawaki, 1980; Muro, 1988). 63. The value placed on performance in schools plays a powerful mediating role and may be the most important of the adaptive dispositions. The qualities of autonomy and initiative, so prized in American culture, do not in themselves interfere with learning in group situations. Many reports of computer-based learning indicate high involvement of students. Also many of the constricting features of schooling are also present in team sports, where compliance with the authority of coach and referee and cooperation with peers are required. Cultural Support for Schooling: Japan and the U.S. / 14 525 530 535 540 64. In sports, however, students value achievement and even within constraints, qualities of autonomy and initiative contribute to success. Students in the United States have little difficulty in adapting to the restrictive rules of team sports. Their difficulty with academic achievement, we suggest, comes in part from the discrepancy in importance placed by the public on sports and that placed on performance in the classroom. The importance of achievement overrides values of separateness and individuality. 65. Where education is valued, as in professional and graduate schools, dispositions of initiative, independence, and verbal assertiveness in the United States are no barrier to success. It is at the early grade levels that students’ sense of the importance of education is dependent on community support. Without such support for achievement, an emphasis on independence and initiative makes the teacher’s task more difficult. Engaging students becomes a major goal of instructional strategy, a formidable task for teachers in classes that encompass extreme diversity of culture and income. 66. Some schools in the United States build effectively on qualities of independence and initiative. To do so, we suggest, they have created microcultures encompassing the community and school in which education is given extraordinarily high value. Creating such educational cultures requires extreme effort; it is not likely to be achieved solely by administrative reforms or changes in the curriculum. Cultural Support for Schooling: Japan and the U.S. / 15 Answer in your own words in English, unless otherwise indicated. 1. What do the first five paragraphs suggest about the connection between the overall culture in which learning is carried on and educational norms prevailing in the said culture? Answer : ____________________________________________________________ 2. Judging by the information provided in paragraphs 7-8 (mainly 8), what do the Japanese and American mothers expect of their offspring? Answer : ____________________________________________________________ 3. Explain how the attitudes of the American mothers, as indicated in paragraphs 12-14, are not necessarily consistent with independence and self-reliance. Answer : ____________________________________________________________ Answer the question below in Hebrew. 4. How would you – in the terms of this article – account for American acceptance of diversity on the one hand and Japanese insistence on conformity on the other hand? (Think in terms of population composition.) Answer : ____________________________________________________________ 5. Answer the question below in English. What does the example of the three-year-old nursery school children – paragraph 18 – suggest? Answer : ____________________________________________________________ Answer the question below in Hebrew. 6. What characteristics did the Japanese children on the one hand and the American children on the other hand – paragraphs 19-22 – display in their problem solving games? Answer : ____________________________________________________________ Cultural Support for Schooling: Japan and the U.S. / 16 7. 8. Answer the question below in English. Japanese society is commonly perceived as subjecting its children to harsh discipline and regimentation; in what sense can the behaviour of Japanese mothers – paragraphs 23-33 – towards their children be said to contradict these perceptions? Answer : ____________________________________________________________ Answer the question below in English. Describe the way Japanese mothers treat their unruly children – paragraphs 2730 – and compare it with that of their American counterparts. Answer : ____________________________________________________________ Answer the question below in Hebrew. 9. How could the Japanese insistence on repetition and learning by rote – paragraphs 36-40 – be attributed to either past or present political or social circumstances? Answer : ____________________________________________________________ Answer the question below in Hebrew. 10. What particular circumstances may have led – paragraph 42 – to the educational system in Japan turning into an “examination hell”? Answer : ____________________________________________________________ Choose the best answer. 11. The methods of instruction practised in American and Japanese schools – paragraphs 45-60 – may suggest that the latter are not necessarily conducive to a. discipline. b. good manners. c. social harmony. d. individual creativity. Cultural Support for Schooling: Japan and the U.S. / 17 12. Answer the question below in English. Which of the two schooling systems - American or Japanese – paragraphs 52 – 57 – is likely to make the teacher’s job both more difficult and more challenging? (Elaborate) Answer : ____________________________________________________________ Answer the question below in Hebrew. 13. What qualities – paragraphs 61-66 – are encouraged and enhanced by the American schooling system? Answer : ____________________________________________________________ 14. Answer the question below in English. How could the qualities emphasized in team sports – paragraph 64 – conceivably conflict with those stressed in the American schools? Answer : ____________________________________________________________ Summary Question – Answer in Hebrew. 15. What are the qualities fostered in the Japanese schooling system on the one hand and in the American school system on the other hand? Answer : ____________________________________________________________ Cultural Support for Schooling: Japan and the U.S. / 18