1 Running head: PERCEPTIONS OF SEXUAL HARASSMENT Perceptions of Sexual Harassment: An Evolutionary Perspective Stephen M. Colarelli and Stephanie Haaland Department of Psychology Central Michigan University 2 Abstract Much of what we know about perceptions of hostile-environment sexual harassment remains unclear; therefore, scholars have called for greater theory development and theoryguided research. We present an evolutionary psychological framework for understanding sexual harassment perceptions and use that perspective to examine the effects of age, sex, status, and power on perceptions of hostile-environment harassment. In Study 1, we examined the effects of observer age, sex, and behavioral severity on harassment perceptions. Age had a significant effect, with older women viewing sexually-toned behaviors as more harassing than younger women. Observer sex also had a significant effect, with females perceiving sexually-toned behaviors—primarily the severe behaviors—as more harassing. Behavioral severity had a strong and significant effect on perceptions of both sexes. In Study 2, we examined how initiator status and power affected harassment perceptions. As expected, initiator power had a strong main effect on perceptions, whereas initiator status had no effect. Interactions revealed that harassment perceptions increased as power increased and as status decreased. The results were generally consistent with sexual selection theory. We conclude with suggestions for policies related to perceptions and accusations of sexual harassment in the workplace. With the increasing integration of women and men in the work place, the incidents of sexual harassment have increased—some surveys reporting that as many as 53% of women experienced sexual harassment at work (Gutek, 1985). The Equal Employment Opportunity Commission (EEOC) in the US defines two broad categories of sexual harassment (EEOC, 1980). The first is "quid pro quo" harassment, where sexual favors are solicited in exchange for 3 job-related rewards or to avoid negative consequences. For example, a supervisor propositions a subordinate and implies that unless she sleeps with him she will loose her job. The second type of sexual harassment is "hostile-environment" harassment. Hostile-environment harassment involves unwelcome behaviors of a sexual nature that are perceived as creating a hostile or offensive work environment. A person making a claim of hostile-environment harassment must show “the work environment is permeated with sexuality or ‘discriminatory intimidation, ridicule, and insult’….The plaintiff making a hostile-environment claim must show that she was subjected to ‘unwelcome’ conduct, based upon her sex” (Browne, 2002, p. 192). While the definition of quid pro quo harassment is clear, attempts to define hostile-environment harassment continue to generate controversy, and its definition remains ambiguous (Bennet-Alexander & Pincus, 1995; Browne, 2002; Lengnick-Hall, 1995).1 The courts in the US and scholars are divided over whether the standard for the perception of a hostile-environment should be that of a “reasonable person” or a “reasonable woman.” People frequently construe sexually ambiguous behaviors quite differently. Men are more likely to interpret a relatively innocuous behavior from a woman as a sign of sexual interest, whereas women interpret the same behavior as just being friendly (Abbey, 1982). Women are more likely than men to regard a sexual innuendo as unwelcome (Buss, 1994). Despite efforts by the courts and others to impose objectivity, definitive boundaries of what constitutes hostile-environment sexual harassment remain unclear (Lengnick-Hall, 1995). As a result, employees and organizations can have difficulty recognizing when sexual harassment has occurred. This ambiguity can be detrimental to targets of harassment who may be hesitant to report behavior they perceive as offensive because they are unsure that others will regard it as harassment. It can also be detrimental to those accused of harassment when there is little consensus that a behavior was offensive. Additionally, it can be 4 detrimental to organizations that do not recognize the problem and fail to develop clear definitions of hostile-environment harassment and procedures for dealing with it. Both men and women – but especially women – view severe sexually-toned behaviors as inappropriate and label them as sexual harassment (e.g., Hurt, Maver, & Hoffman, 1999). Unfortunately, most studies of severity included incidents of both quid pro quo and hostileenvironment harassment in their stimulus materials (e.g., Baker, Terpstra, & Cutler, 1991; Tata, 1993). Therefore, it remains unclear how perceptions are affected when behaviors are limited to gradations of hostile-environment harassment stimuli. People do not inevitably categorize sexually toned behaviors as sexual harassment. Saying “you look nice today” implies a perception of attractiveness and is mildly sexually-toned, but it does not necessarily mean that the comment is universally construed as sexually harassing. However, if it were perceived as unwelcome and offensive, then it could be considered sexual harassment. In addition to the ambiguity surrounding hostile-environment harassment, two other areas associated with the perception of sexual harassment are problematic: individual differences in perception and the influence of initiator status and power on perceptions. Early research tended to treat sexual harassment as a unitary phenomenon in which all women were assumed to perceive and experience harassment in a similar manner. However, more recent research suggests that individual differences among women affect perceptions of sexually-toned behaviors (Ellis, Barak, & Pinto, 1991). Such results raise the questions of what individual differences are most likely to account for differences in same-sex perceptions and why? Similarly, the power and status of male initiators have been widely associated with sexual harassment perceptions. Power and status have different meanings, and it is not clear that they have similar effects on harassment perceptions. Although several studies suggest that their effects on harassment 5 perceptions are less clear than previously assumed (Hurt, et al., 1999; Jones, Remland, & Brunner, 1987; Littler-Bishop, Seidler-Feller, & Opaluch, 1982), relatively little research exists on the comparative effects of male power and status and on the mechanisms that might influence how they harassment perceptions. Because of these and other complexities, scholars have called for more theory development and theory-guided research (Browne, 1997; Fitzgerald & Shullman, 1993). The objectives of this paper are to: (1) examine how perceptions of sexual harassment are affected when sexually-toned behaviors are limited to gradations of hostile-environment harassment stimuli; (2) present an evolutionary psychological framework for understanding hostileenvironment sexual harassment perceptions, and (3) use that perspective to examine the effects of target age and initiator status and power on perceptions of hostile-environment harassment. Sexual Harassment Perceptions from an Evolutionary Perspective Evolutionary psychology provides a useful framework for studying perceptions of sexually-toned behaviors (Studd & Gattiker, 1991). Just as natural selection shaped human morphology and physiology, it also influenced people's behavioral and psychological repertoires. This may be particularly evident in human sexual behavior, where evolved psychological mechanisms influence the occurrence and perceptions of sexual behavior (Hrdy, 1999; Low, 2000). Parental investment and female choice are two concepts from evolutionary psychology that are particularly relevant to sexual harassment perceptions. Parental investment and female choice. Parental investment refers to the effort and resources devoted to an offspring that improves its chances of survival and that also limit the parent’s ability to invest in other offspring (Trivers, 1972). Because of the physiological and psychological demands associated with gestation, birth, lactation, and attachment, human 6 females invest more in offspring than males. In some species, however, males are the more investing sex—for example, the pipefish seahorse, Mormon cricket, and Panamanian poison arrow frog. Female choice refers to females’ dominant role in mate selection. Because of the effort required to care for young, it is in the reproductive interests of the more investing sex to be selective in mate choice (Trivers, 1972). It is, therefore, in a human female’s reproductive interests to mate with “high quality” males (Cronin, 1991; Small, 1992). It is to the female’s advantage to choose a mate who is committed to her and who has the ability to assist in providing for her offspring. This helps ensure that the female will have adequate resources and security to rear her young. Women have more to lose from mating with someone whom they do not choose to mate with. They may end up pregnant without a mate to help care for the child and possibly with a child who may be sickly and difficult to care for. Sexual coercion, therefore, has potentially more negative consequences to women than men, and therefore women are more likely than men to react negatively to coercive sexual strategies (Petralia & Gallup, 2002). Women also pay more attention to and notice more details of sexually-toned behavior that they perceive as threatening. This follows from research on emotion and cognition that has found that people react more emotionally to negative information (Taylor, 1991) and are more likely to process negative information in an effortful, systematic, and detailed manner (Clore, Schwarz, & Conway, 1994). It is also consistent with an evolutionary interpretation that people’s propensity to react more strongly to negative than positive information is an evolved adaptation. Over evolutionary history, it has been more difficult for people to reverse the consequences of severe assault than a missed opportunity (Cacioppo & Berntson, 1999). Figure 1 presents a general 7 heuristic of how biological, personal, and cultural factors may influence the perception and labeling of sexually-toned behaviors. Insert Figure 1 about here Study 1: Age, Sex, and Perceptions of Sexual Harassment One of the most critical features of a woman’s reproductive biology is age. Age is a biologically important individual difference, and it may influence female perceptions of sexuallytoned behaviors. Younger women are in their prime reproductive years and are more likely to be seeking mates. Women past the age of forty are less likely to bear children, and post-menopausal women are unable to reproduce. Males, on the other hand, can sire children into old age. Therefore, younger women may be more receptive than older women to sexual attention from desirable males. They may perceive mild and moderately sexually-toned behaviors as less harassing than older women perceive them. However, because severe sexually-toned behaviors imply a limitation of female choice, we expect that age would have no effect on the perception of severe behaviors. Curiously, although age is a standard demographic variable, we were unable to locate studies on sexual harassment in which age was a central variable—either theoretically or empirically. However, several studies suggest a relationship between age and sexual harassment perceptions. For example female students are less likely than (presumably older) female employees to label sexually-toned behaviors as sexual harassment and perceive sexually-toned behaviors as offensive (Bremer, Moore, & Bildersee, 1991; Brooks & Perot, 1991; Fitzgerald, Shullman, Bailey, Richards, Swecker, Gold, Ormerod & Weitzman, 1988). On the other hand, 8 because age is less related to a male’s reproductive value, we expect that it is less likely to influence males’ perceptions of sexual-toned behaviors. Method Subjects. Three hundred and fifty two (352) subjects participated in Study 1; 71 (20%) were undergraduate students and 281 were staff members from a Midwestern university. The undergraduates received extra credit points toward partial fulfillment of a class requirement. The staff received materials in campus mail, along with a cover letter asking for their assistance. The subjects ranged in age from 17 through 65 (mean = 37), and 68% were female. Materials. The stimulus materials consisted of three vignettes. They described an interaction between a male and female co-worker. The three vignettes contained the same categories of sexually-toned behavior but differed in severity. We used behaviors that were relatively ambiguous and would be considered hostile-environment behaviors; none were quid pro quo. The vignettes were based on 15 sexually-toned behaviors (see Table 1) grouped into five categories (physical contact, request for dates, comments on appearance, looking, and facial gestures) and three levels of severity (low, moderate, and high). We pilot tested the behaviors to Insert Table 1 about here determine if the levels of severity of the behaviors were perceived at the levels in which we had assigned them a priori. Subjects (n = 49) in the pilot test ranged in age from 16 through 45 (mean = 21.83). The mean severity ratings (on a scale of 1 to 7, where 1 = low and 7 = high) for behaviors in low, moderate, and high severity categories were 3.2, 5.2, and 6.8, respectively, and 9 all were significantly different from one another (all at p < .01). The vignettes are presented in full in Appendix 1. The stimulus materials were organized into packets. On the first page of each packet were instructions, which highlighted sex of the initiator and recipient and indicated that they were co-workers. Three vignettes followed the instructions. Each packet contained vignettes of mild, moderate, and high severity (see Appendix 1). We counterbalanced order of severity. Below each vignette were brief instructions and six questions. The questions related to the following perceptions of the vignettes: appropriateness, offensiveness, friendliness, severity, how complimentary the behavior appeared, and the degree to which the behavior could be considered sexually harassing. Subjects rated their perceptions on Likert scales, ranging from 1 (low) to 7 (high). The last page of the survey included demographic questions. Design. We used a 4 (observer age) x 2 (observer sex) x 3 (severity level) repeated measures, factorial design. The four categories for subject age were 18-25, 26-35, 36-45, and over 45. Severity of sexually-toned behavior was a within-subjects factor. Procedure. Participants were asked to read the first scenario and then rate the behavior of the male co-worker on the six questions described above. After making ratings on the first scenario, participants continued on to the next scenario. Students read the vignettes and completed the questionnaires in a classroom or laboratory. For the employees, we sent packets and return envelopes through the campus mailing system (36% response rate). 10 Results and Discussion Because of the relatively high intercorrelations among the six items of our dependent measures, we combined them into one overall rating, which we called the “overall harassment rating.” The median correlations among the six dependent measures in the low, moderate, and high severity conditions were .71, .66, and .29, respectively. The corresponding reliability estimates (alphas) were .94, .92, and .69. These correlations suggest that subjects made finer discriminations about sexually-toned behaviors when they were severe. Main effects occurred for age, F (3, 351) = 19.35, p < .001, sex, F (1, 348) = 5.38, p < .02 and severity, F (2, 351) = 699.55, p < .001. The effect size for age was moderate (eta2 = .15), small for sex (eta2 = .02), and substantial for severity (eta2 = .77). Age interacted with severity, F (6, 348) = 6.07, p < .03, but there was no interaction between sex and age. Insert Figures 2 and 3 about here The means of the harassment ratings by severity of sexually-toned behaviors and observer sex and age are presented in Table 2. An inspection of the means supports Insert Table 2 about here our expectations that younger women perceive mild and moderate sexually-toned behavior as less harassing than older women, and that women of all ages will perceive severe behaviors as harassing. Harassment ratings increased regularly with age for women in the low and moderate 11 severity conditions. In the high severity condition, the only significant difference in female perceptions was between the youngest and oldest groups, and this difference was slight (6.44 vs. 6.73). Although there was not a significant interaction between age and sex, the pattern of means does lend support to our expectation that male perceptions will be less affected by age. In the low and moderate conditions, the only significant difference was between the youngest and oldest males. There were no differences among males by age in the high-severity condition. Male and female perceptions of harassment differed primarily in the severe condition. With the exception of the 31-40 age group, women rated the severe behaviors as more harassing than the men did, although the magnitude of the differences was small. In the low and moderate conditions male and female harassment perceptions did not differ, except among the participants in the 31-40 age group in the moderate condition. Study 2: Power, Status, and Perceptions of Sexual Harassment. Power and status are often associated with one another. Powerful people often possess high status. Perhaps because of this common association, and because studies have found that a male initiator’s power and status affect women’s perceptions of sexually-toned behavior, power and status tend to be treated synonymously in the sexual harassment literature (e.g., Gutek, Morasch, & Cohen, 1983; Tata, 1993). However, a blanket association between male power and status on the one hand and female perceptions of sexual harassment on the other hand is not yet warranted. Power and status are analytically distinct concepts. One of the more widely used conceptions of power is that of Emerson’s (1962). Emerson views power as arising from dependency relations. Power arises from a situation in which one individual is dependent on another for valued resources. Employees, for example, are typically dependent upon their supervisors for good performance evaluations, favorable assignments, and other rewards. Status, 12 on the other hand, refers to rank, typically associated with privilege and reward (Henrich & GilWhite, 2001). However, a person with high status may or may not have power over another. A closer look at the empirical evidence suggests that the association between male power and status and female perceptions of sexual harassment is equivocal. Some studies have found no association between initiator power and perceptions of sexual harassment (Hurt, et al., 1999; Jones, et al., 1987). In fact, one study (Littler-Bishop, et al., 1982) found that sexually-toned behaviors were perceived most negatively when initiated by low-status males. There are good theoretical reasons to believe that initiator power will have a different effect than status on female perceptions of harassment. An evolutionary psychological perspective suggests that women tend to regard high-status in males as desirable because of the association between status and resource acquisition (Low, 2000). Thus, females may perceive sexually-toned behaviors directed at them by high-status males more positively than sexuallytoned behaviors directed at them by low-status males.2 On the other hand, we expect that power should have the opposite effect. Because female choice is integral to evolved female sexuality, females will regard coercion, or even the suggestion of it, as undesirable. When a male has power over a woman and directs even mild sexually-toned behaviors toward her this creates an ambiguous situation that could be perceived as coercive. A possibility exists that the male may withhold resources unless the female responds encouragingly. On the other hand, if a male has no power over a woman, the woman can rebuff the male with little fear of potentially adverse consequences. Therefore, sexually-toned behaviors initiated by a male who has power over a female are likely to be perceived more negatively than behaviors initiated by a male who does not have power over her. Study 2 was designed to tease apart the status and power of the initiator to examine how each affects perceptions of hostile-environment harassment. 13 Method Subjects. Five hundred and forty two (542) subjects participated in Study 2; 229 (42%) were undergraduates and 313 were university staff members. The undergraduates received extra credit points toward partial fulfillment of a class requirement. The staff received materials in the campus mail, along with a cover letter asking for their assistance. The subjects ranged in age from 17 through 70 (mean = 33), and 372 were female (69%). Materials. To tease apart status and power, we created vignettes that varied levels of the male’s power and status separately. For example, in one vignette a high status male was presented who did not have any power over the woman; in another a low status male was presented who was the direct supervisor of the woman. The vignettes were similar to those used in Study 1, except that we modified them to manipulate initiator status and power over the target (see Appendix 2). The high-status initiator was a male partner in a prestigious New York City law firm, the medium-status initiator was a male manager of a gardening department in a large discount store, and the low-status initiator was a manager of a convenience store. In the law firm vignettes, we described the target as a newly hired, junior lawyer; in the discount and convenience store vignettes, we described the target as a sales clerk. Power over the target was manipulated by describing the initiator as: (1) the target’s direct supervisor (direct power), (2) a partner or manager in another department in the same firm or store and, therefore, not the target’s supervisor (indirect power), and (3) a partner or manager in a different firm or store (no power). For the direct and indirect power vignettes, the context for the interaction was a work setting. For the no-power vignette, the context for the lawyers was a meeting of a board of directors of an art museum on which the initiator and recipient served; for the discount and convenience store employees, the context was a bowling league. A manipulation check confirmed that the status 14 and power manipulations were perceived as expected. The stimulus materials were counterbalanced and organized in the same manner as those in Study 1; the six dependent measures remained the same. Design. We used a 3 (status) x 3 (power) x 3 (severity) repeated measures, factorial design. Status (low, medium, and high) and severity of sexually toned behavior (low, moderate, and high) were between-subject variables. Power over the target (direct, indirect, and no power) was a within-subjects variable. Procedure. We used the same procedure as in Study 1. The response rate from employees was 32%. Participants were asked to read one scenario carefully and then to make ratings on the same six questions as in Study 1 before moving on to the next scenario. Results and Discussion As in Study 1, we combined the six items into an overall harassment rating. Since our interest was with female perceptions, we included only female subjects in the analyses. There were significant main effects for power, F (2, 370) = 204.18, p < .01, and severity, F (2, 370) = 117.58, p < .01; both had large effects (the eta2s were .53 and .40, respectively). The main effect for status was not significant. Although we expected that high status would be associated with Insert Figure 4 About Here lower harassment ratings and visa-versa, the fact that status had no effect confirms our expectation that high status would not be associated with higher harassment ratings. However, status interacted with power, F (6, 370) = 4.20 p < .05, and power interacted with severity, F (4, 370) = 8.64, p < .01. An inspection of the means in Table 3 provides further detail regarding 15 Insert Figure 5 About Here Insert Table 3 about here the nature of the interactions. As power increased and as status decreased, harassment ratings increased. This suggests that status mitigated the effect of power on harassment perceptions. Within each category of sexually-toned behavior, the highest harassment ratings occurred with the low status, high power initiator. Power had a somewhat greater (albeit not a significantly different) effect on harassment ratings in the low severity condition than in the high severity condition. This is consistent with our expectation that initiator power is more likely to influence perceptions of sexually-toned behaviors when behaviors are more mild and hence ambiguous. It should also be noted that in 25 of the 27 cells in Table 3, women’s harassment ratings reflected significant differences based on initiator power. In other words, women made consistent and fine-grained distinctions among levels of power held by the male initiator. General Discussion We argued that evolutionary psychology provides a useful framework for theory and research on perceptions of sexual harassment. In particular, observer age and initiator power and status were expected to influence sexual harassment perceptions in the directions predicted by an evolutionary perspective. Age was expected to affect perceptions because of its relation to fertility. Younger women are in their prime reproductive years, and, therefore, they were expected to be more receptive than older women to sexual attention from desirable males. Status 16 and power were expected to have opposite effects on sexual harassment perceptions. Women were expected to regard high status in males as desirable because of the association between status and resource acquisition, and therefore perceive sexually-toned behaviors directed at them by higher status males more positively than sexually-toned behaviors directed at them by lower status males. Power, on the other hand, was expected to have the opposite effect because female choice is fundamental to female sexuality. Females were expected to regard explicit or implicit sexual coercion as extremely undesirable. Therefore, sexually-toned behaviors initiated by a male who has power over a female were expected to be perceived more negatively than behaviors by a male who does not have power over a female. Our results were consistent with an evolutionary psychological perspective of sexual harassment. In Study 1, older women perceived mild and moderate sexually-toned behaviors as more harassing than younger women. However, also consistent with an evolutionary psychological perspective, female age had little influence on harassment perceptions of severe behaviors—women of all ages regarded severe behaviors as highly harassing. In Study 2, status and power had differential effects on women’s perceptions of sexually-toned behaviors. Status did not have a main effect on harassment perceptions, while a large main effect occurred for initiator power. However, power and status interacted, so that harassment ratings increased as power increased and status decreased. Females made consistent and fine-grained distinctions in harassment ratings according to the male initiator’s power—with higher ratings being associated with higher initiator power. Although not of primary interest in this study, in a separate analysis, we examined male observers’ harassment ratings with respect to initiator status and power. The results were generally similar, with the following exception. Unlike females, male harassment ratings did not significantly differ in the direct and indirect power in the high and medium status 17 conditions. In line with an evolutionary perspective, females were more sensitive than males to the male initiator’s power over the recipient. By and large, males and females were similar in their perceptions of which sexually-toned behaviors were harassing. This supports Gutek’s (1985) argument that the effects of sex on perceptions of sexually-toned behaviors has been overstated. Men and women did not differ in their harassment ratings in the low and moderate severity conditions; women gave slightly higher ratings than men in the severe condition. That people also seemed to process information in a more vigilant and systematic fashion in the high severity condition than the low and moderate conditions suggests that strongly toned behaviors were viewed as threatening or inappropriate. People perceive the more severe hostile-environment behaviors (e.g., inappropriate touching, leering) as harassing, and, thus, sexually-toned behaviors need not be as extreme as quid pro quo to be reliably regarded as harassing. Limitations One limitation of this study involves the inextricable relationship between age and experience. Older participants were also working adults while most of the younger participants were students. Although, it is common for students to hold part-time jobs, it is possible that different amounts of work experience, rather than participants’ age, may have influenced their reactions to the harassment scenarios. If this were the case, sexual harassment may well have different meanings to the older participants due to factors other than age. However, that harassment ratings did not differ as strongly by age for male participants suggests that factors other than experience may be responsible for difference in ratings among women of different age groups. Moreover, that these differences are considerably smaller in the more severe conditions indicates that older and younger participants agreed on the meaning of harassment and therefore 18 work experience may not lead to completely different definitions of harassment by age group. Nonetheless the effect of work experience probably does influence the ratings to some extent and should be considered when interpreting the results found here. Future research that controls for work experiences when examining age differences would be useful. A second limitation is that because the return rate among the working adult sample was less than 100%, the people who responded may have different views – perhaps stronger views – about sexual harassment than those who did not respond. Again, one should look to the sample of males. There were minor differences among males in different age groups. Males in the 18-22 groups did not significantly differ from males in the 23-30 or 31-40 groups on any of the ratings across any of the severity conditions. This suggests that, a response bias among the working adults was unlikely to have had a strong influence on the results. However, as is almost always the case with field research where it difficult to get 100% participation, the possibility of a response bias should nonetheless be considered when interpreting the data. Implications The results of these studies are relevant for both structural interventions and training programs. One difficulty in dealing with accusations of sexual harassment is that people often perceive the same sexually-toned behavior differently. Yet little research has focused on what individual differences systematically affect harassment perceptions. This study shows that a person’s age appears to affect harassment perceptions, particularly a woman’s age. Therefore, investigations of sexual harassment should take into account the effects of age and involve decision-makers from different age groups. Just as age-related demographic analyses are useful in predicting crime rates (crime goes up as the percentage of young males in a population increases), demographic analyses of organizations may be helpful in predicting (and 19 understanding) sexual harassment accusations. For example, organizations in which age is bimodally distributed are more likely to experience problems. On the other hand, sex by itself had a weak effect on perceptions. By and large, males and females gave similar harassment ratings to sexually-toned behaviors. Neither males nor females regarded mild-flirtation as particularly harassing. However, there was a consensus that severely-toned behaviors were harassing. The agreement between males and females on the meaning of more severe behaviors suggests that organizations can proceed more confidently in clearly defining and sanctioning hostile-environment behaviors. Given the strong effects of initiator power, training programs for supervisors and managers need to emphasize that, even though their intent may be benign, sexually-toned behaviors initiated by males with power over females are likely to be perceived as harassing. Moreover, based on the results of this study, males seem to be less aware than females of the effects of power on female perceptions of harassment. This lends support to Bargh and Raymond’s (1995) suggestion that males may unconsciously and automatically associate power over females with sex. That males may be unaware of this association is all the more reason to emphasize sexual harassment training for individuals in supervisory positions. The workplace is increasingly a primary location where reproductive-age people meet and form mating partnerships. Therefore, by knowing more about how and under what circumstances people are likely to perceive sexually-toned behaviors as inappropriate, organizations can better control sexual harassment without placing unrealistic constraints on sexuality. Evolutionary psychology and sexual selection theory provide powerful heuristics for understanding sexuality in the workplace and for devising programs to deal effectively with the problem of sexual harassment. 20 References Abbey, A. (1982). Sex differences in attributions for friendly behavior: Do males misperceive females’ friendliness? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 830-838. Baker, D. D., Terpstra, D. E., & Cutler, B. D. (1991). Perceptions of sexual harassment: A re-examination of gender differences. Journal of Psychology, 124, 409-416. Bargh, J. A., & Raymond, P. (1995). The naive misuse of power: Nonconscious sources of sexual harassment. Journal of Social Issues, 51, 86-95. Bremer, B. A., Moore, C. T., & Bildersee, E. F. (1991). Do you have to call it “sexual harassment” to feel harassed? College Student Journal, 25, 258-268. Brooks, L., & Perot, A. R. (1991). Reporting sexual harassment: Exploring a predictive model. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 15, 31-47. Browne, K. R. (1997). An evolutionary perspective on sexual harassment: Seeking roots in biology rather than ideology. Journal of Contemporary Legal Issues, 8, 5-78. Browne, K. R. (2002). Biology at work: Rethinking sexual equality. New Brunswick: Rutgers University Press. Buss, D. (1994). The evolution of desire. New York: Basic Books. Cacioppo, J. T., & Berntson, G. T. (1999). The affect system: Architecture and operating characteristics. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8, 133-136. Clore, G. L., Schwarz, N., & Conway, M. (1994). Cognitive causes and consequences of emotion. In R. S. Wyer and T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (2nd ed., pp. 323417). Hillsdale, NJ: Earlbaum. Cronin, H. (1991). The ant and the peacock. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. 21 Ellis, S., Barak, A., & Pinto, A. (1991). Moderating effects of personal cognitions on experienced and perceived sexual harassment of women at the workplace. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 21, 1320-1337. Emerson, R. M. (1962). Power-dependence relations. American Sociological Review, 27, 31-40. Equal Employment Opportunity Commission (1980). Guidelines on discrimination because of sex. Federal Register, 45, 74676-74677. Fitzgerald, L. F., & Shullman, S. L. (1993). Sexual harassment: A research analysis and agenda for the 1990s. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 42, 5-27. Fitzgerald, L. F., Shullman, S. L. Bailey, N., Richards, M., Swecker, J., Gold, Y., Ormerod, N., & Weitzman, L. (1988). The incidence and dimensions of sexual harassment in academic and the workplace. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 32, 152-175. Gutek, B. A. (1985). Sex and the workplace. San Francisco, CA: Jossey Bass. Gutek, B. A., Morasch, B., & Cohen, A. (1983). Interpreting social-sexual behavior in the work setting. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 22, 30-48. Henrich, J., & Gil-White, F. J. (2001). The evolution of prestige: Freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22, 165-196. Hrdy, S.B. (1999, rev. ed.) The woman that never evolved, Cambridge, MA. Harvard University Press. Hurt, J., Maver, J. A., & Hoffman, D. (1999). Situational and individual influences on judgments of hostile-environment sexual harassment. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 29, 1395-1415. 22 Jones, T. S., Remland, M. S., & Brunner, C. C. (1987). Effects of employment relationship, response of recipient and sex of rater on perceptions of sexual harassment. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 65, 55-63. Lengnick-Hall, M. L. (1995). Sexual harassment research: A methodological critique. Personnel Psychology, 48, 841-864. Littler-Bishop, S., Seidler-Feller, D., & Opaluch, R. E. (1982). Sexual harassment in the workplace as a function of initiator’s status: The case of airline personnel. Journal of Social Issues, 38, 137-148. Low, B.S. (2000) Why sex matters. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Petralia, S. M., & Gallup, Jr., G. G. (2002). Effects of a sexual assault scenario on handgrip strength across the menstrual cycle. Evolution and Human Behavior, 23, 3-10. Small, M. F. (1992). The evolution of female sexuality and mate selection in humans. Human Nature, 3, 133-156. Studd, M. V., & Gattiker, U. E. (1991). The evolutionary psychology of sexual harassment in organizations. Ethology and Sociobiology, 12, 249-290. Tata, J. (1993). The structure and phenomenon of sexual harassment: Impact of category of sexually harassing behavior, gender, and hierarchical level. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 23, 199-211. Taylor, S. E. (1991). Asymmetrical effects of positive and negative events: The mobilization-minimization hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 67-85. Trivers, R. L. (1972). Parental investment and sexual selection. In B. Campbell (Ed.), Sexual selection and the descent of man 1871-1971 (pp. 136-179). Chicago: Aldine. 23 Author Note Stephen M. Colarelli, Department of Psychology, Central Michigan University, Mt. Pleasant, MI 48859 USA, colar1sm@cmich.edu; Stephanie Haaland is now at Denison Consulting, Ann Arbor, MI, USA, shaaland@denisonculture.com. We would like to thank Kingsley Browne for his comments on an earlier version of this article, Maxine Tubbs for her help with the data collection, and Felix Famoye and Charlie Yang for their help with the data analysis. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Stephen M. Colarelli, Department of Psychology, Central Michigan University, Mt. Pleasant, MI 48859. Electronic mail can be sent to colar1sm@cmich.edu. 24 Footnotes 1. For example, different legal standards have emerged over the years--a "reasonable woman," "a reasonable person," and a "reasonable victim" (Bennet-Alexander & Pincus, 1995). Different courts, however, have held differently, and some do not make clear what standard they are applying (K. R. Browne, personal communication, September 8, 1999). 2. This is most likely why Littler-Bishop, et al. (1982) found that status was negatively related to harassment perceptions. In their study, status referred to occupational and socio-economic status, rather than formal authority. Table 1 Sexually-toned Behaviors Used In Scenarios Tone of Sexual Behavior Behavior Physical contact Low Shoulder squeeze Moderate Severe Arm around Touch shoulder Request for dates Ask to lunch Ask to dinner buttocks Repeated requests for dates Comments about appearance Looking “You look nice today” Eye-contact “Brings out your eyes” Double take “Accentuates your body” Look up and down 25 Facial gestures Smile Wink Licks lips 26 Table 2 Study 1 Cell Means: Overall Harassment Ratings by Severity of Sexually-toned Behaviors over Participant Age and Sex Severity of Sexually-toned Behaviors Low Moderate High Age 18-22 Male Female Male Female Male Female M 2.451 2.662,3 3.815 3.746,7,8 6.1110 6.4410,12 SD 1.00 1.07 1.21 1.16 1.02 .591 M 3.39 3.054 4.77 4.846 6.3311 6.7611 SD 1.07 1.28 1.25 1.67 .92 .435 M 2.77 3.693 4.459 5.187,9 6.39 6.55 SD 1.23 1.54 .96 1.38 .74 .780 M 3.731 3.982,4 4.975 5.418 6.4813 6.7312,13 SD 1.63 1.59 1.47 1.25 .72 .49 23-30 31-40 41+ Note. Means with the same subscripts are significantly different from one another. For ease of presentation, subscripts reflect differences across sex and observer age categories within severity condition. 28 Table 3 Study 2 Cell Means: Female Observers’ Overall Harassment Rating by Male Status and Power and Sexual Tone of Behavior Male Initiator’s Status High Medium Low Severity of Sexually-toned Behavior Power Low Moderate High Low Moderate High Low Moderate High M 3.841, a 4.761, b 6.521, c, d 4.052, e 5.032, f 6.612, g 4.403, i 5.343,22 6.763,23 SD 1.447 1.515 .684 1.437 1.505 .599 1.719 1.276 .485 M 3.344, a 4.264, b 6.234, d 3.465, e 4.215, f 6.385,g 3.966, h 4.337,22 6.376,7,23 SD 1.383 1.677 .912 1.362 1.761 .859 1.631 1.566 .814 M 3.008, a 3.838, b 6.148, c 3.009, e 3.6210, f 6.079, 10, g 3.0711, h, i 3.6312,22 6.1311,12,23 SD 1.370 1.816 1.006 1.335 1.876 1.089 1.364 1.713 .993 Direct Indirect No Power Note. Means on a given variable with the same subscripts are significantly different from one another. For ease of presentation, subscripts 1 through 12 reflect differences within status and across severity conditions; subscripts a through i reflect differences within status and severity across power conditions. 29 Figure Caption Figure 1. A Model of Perception and Labeling of Sexually-toned Behaviors. Figure 2. Effects of Age, Sex, and Severity. Figure 3. Interaction of Age x Severity. Figure 4. Effects of Power, Status, and Severity. Figure 5. Interactions of Status x Power and Status x Severity 30 Behavior Interpretation as sexual or non-sexual Universal and cultural sexual signals If sexual, is behavior welcome or unwelcome? Reactions to, judgements about, and labeling of behavior Context: actor attributes, target attributes, and nature of behaviors and setting 31 Overall Harassment Rating 7 6 5 4 Female Male 3 2 1 0 17-22 years 23-30 years Age 31-40 years 41+ years Low Medium High Severity 32 7 6 5 4 3 Age 17-22 2 23-30 31-40 1 41+ 0 Low Medium Severity High 33 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 0 Low Medium High Status None Indirect Direct Power Low Moderate High Severity 34 6 5.5 5 4.5 Status 4 Low 3.5 Medium High 3 None Indirect Direct Power 7 6.5 6 5.5 5 Power 4.5 Direct Indirect None 4 3.5 3 Low Moderate Severity High 35 Appendix 1: Severity Scenarios Low Severity As Jennifer walks by, Gary glances up, makes eye contact and says, “You look nice today.” He then squeezes her shoulder says, “You’re doing a good job,” and smiles. As Gary turns to put on his coat he asks, “would you like to go to lunch?” Moderate Severity As Jennifer walks by, Gary does a double take and says “That blouse really brings out the color of your eyes.” He then puts his arm around her shoulders, says, “You’re doing a good job,” and winks. As Gary turns to grab his coat he asks, “would you like to go to dinner?” High Severity As Jennifer walks by, Gary slowly looks her up and down and says “That outfit really accentuates your body.” He then pats her buttocks says, “You’re doing a good job,” and slowly licks his lips. As he turns to grab his coat he says “are you sure I can’t talk you into going to dinner?” Note: The introduction to each scenario in Study 1 was: Please read the following interaction between Jennifer, a 29 year old , and Gary, a 34 year old, who have been co-workers in the same department of company XYZ for the past year. 36 Appendix 2 Status and Power Scenarios Power Status Low l High Jennifer, a 29-year old female, is a clerk who works the day shift at a Stop & Go convenience store. Gary, a 34-year old male, is the day manager of the Stop & Go store. Gary is Jennifer's direct supervisor. (Insert one of three Severity Scenarios from Study One) Medium Catherine, a 29-year old female, is a clerk who works the weekday shift in a Stop & Go convenience store. Michael, a 34-year old male, is the weekend manager at the same Stop & Go store. Because Michael works a different shift, he is NOT Catherine's direct supervisor. (Insert one of three Severity Scenarios from Study One) Low Barbara, a 29-year old female, is a clerk who works the day shift in a Stop & Go convenience store. Thomas, a 34-year old male, is a manager at a Stop & Go store in another city. Barbara and Thomas are members of the same bowling league. After a league competition one night…(Insert one of three Severity Scenarios from Study One) Medium High Jennifer, a 29-year old female, is a clerk in the sporting goods department of a large Shop-4-Less discount store. Gary, a 34-year old male, is the manager of the sporting goods department in the same Shop-4-Less discount store. Gary is Jennifer's direct supervisor. (Insert one of three Severity Scenarios from Study One) Medium Catherine, a 29-year old female, is a clerk in the sporting goods department of a large Shop-4-Less discount store. Michael, a 34-year old male, is the manager of the gardening department in the same Shop-4-Less discount store. Because Michael works in a different department he is NOT Catherine's direct supervisor. (Insert one of three Severity Scenarios from Study One) 37 Low Barbara, a 29-year old female, is a clerk in the sporting goods department of a large Shop-4-Less discount store. Thomas, a 34-year old male, is a department manager at Save Mart, a different large discount store in another city. Barbara and Thomas are members of the same bowling league. (Insert one of three Severity Scenarios from Study One) High High Jennifer, a 29-year old female, is a junior lawyer in the corporate defense division of a prestigious New York City law firm. Gary, a 34-year old male, is a lawyer and partner (owns a share of the firm) in the same law firm. Gary is the manager of the corporate defense division, and is Jennifer's direct supervisor. (Insert one of three Severity Scenarios from Study One) Medium Catherine, a 29-year old female, is a junior lawyer in the corporate defense division of a prestigious New York City law firm. Michael, a 34-year old male, is a lawyer and partner (owns a share of the firm) in the same law firm. Michael is the manager of the criminal defense division. Because he is in a different department than Catherine, he is NOT her direct supervisor. Low Barbara, a 29-year old female, is a junior lawyer in a prestigious New York City law firm. Thomas, a 34-year old male, is a lawyer and partner (owns a share of the firm) in a different law firm, in Manhattan. Both Thomas and Barbara are on the board of trustees of the New York Museum of Modern Art. Their affiliation with the museum is voluntary, based on their interest in art. After a museum board meeting one evening…(Insert one of three Severity Scenarios from Study One)