Smith, S., & Cormier, K. (under review). In or out? Spatial scale and enactment in narratives of native and non-native signing deaf children acquiring British Sign Language. (submitted 15 September 2012) In or out? Spatial scale and enactment in narratives of native and non-native signing deaf children acquiring British Sign Language Sandra Smitha and Kearsy Cormierb a b Centre for Deaf Studies, University of Bristol Deafness, Cognition and Language Research Centre, University College London CORRESPONDING AUTHOR Kearsy Cormier Deafness, Cognition & Language Research Centre University College London 49 Gordon Square London WC1H 0PD England k.cormier@ucl.ac.uk Phone: +44 (0)20 7679 8674 Fax: +44 (0)20 7679 8691 Co-author email address: sandra.d.smith@bristol.ac.uk SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL Funding This work was supported by the Arts and Humanities Research Council of Great Britain [119360 to K.C. and S.S.]; and the Economic and Social Research Council of Great Britain [RES-620-28-6001 and RES-620-28-6002], Deafness, Cognition and Language Research Centre (DCAL). Acknowledgements We thank Zed Sevcikova and Katherine Mumford for assistance in coding the data presented here, and to Natalie Smith for acting as model in the figures showing examples from the data. Thanks to Dr. Susan Gregory for allowing these data to be used for SS’s Postgraduate Diploma in Social Science dissertation project at the Centre for Deaf Studies, University of Bristol (1995) and to Prof. Jim Kyle for supervising that project. Thanks especially to the deaf children (and their teachers and parents) who took part in this project. Author references removed for anonymity Cormier, K., Quinto-Pozos, D., Sevcikova, Z., & Schembri, A. (in press). Lexicalisation and de-lexicalisation processes in sign languages: Comparing depicting constructions and viewpoint gestures. Language and Communication. Cormier, K., Smith, S., & Sevcikova, Z. (under review). Predicate structures and embodiment in the representation of action in British Sign Language: Evidence from deaf children and adults. Cormier, K., Smith, S., & Zwets, M. (under review). Framing constructed action in British Sign Language narratives. 2 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL In or out? Spatial scale and enactment in narratives of native and non-native signing deaf children acquiring British Sign Language Abstract Age of sign language acquisition effects have been found at various levels of linguistic structure, and also in cognitive abilities such as perspective taking. The current study investigates two aspects of perspective taking in BSL narratives by 4 deaf native signing children and 4 non-native signing children ages 8-10, via use of spatial scale – character (large) scale vs. observer (small) scale - and enactment (i.e. constructed action). Results show that all children from both groups use constructed action in character space (i.e. fully “in” the character space); this points to the primacy of enactment in all children early in development. Two deaf children from hearing families used observer scale with constructed action, but no deaf children from deaf families did. Likewise, three deaf children from deaf families used character scale without constructed action, but no deaf children from hearing families did. The differences between the two groups suggest that complex uses of perspective taking require very early exposure to a sign language. Keywords: scale, perspective, character, observer, constructed action In or out? Use of spatial scale and enactment in narratives of native and non-native signing deaf children acquiring British Sign Language 1. Introduction The vast majority of research on sign language development in deaf children has been on deaf children from deaf families – i.e. children who are acquiring a sign language as a native language (Anderson & Reilly, 2002; Emmorey & Reilly, 1998; Harris, Clibbens, Chasin, & Tibbitts, 1989; Maestas y Moores, 1980; Schick, 1987; Supalla, 1982). This is for 3 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL a good reason – we need to have an understanding of how native acquisition works before considering other developmental situations. However, the fact is that 90-95% of deaf children are born to hearing families, most of whom do not sign (Mitchell & Karchmer, 2004). Age of sign language acquisition varies enormously, with some deaf children from hearing families learning to sign when they start school, some later in childhood, some later in life, and some not at all. These highly variable and unusual language transmission patterns with sign languages provide a testing ground for notions about a critical or sensitive period for language. A range of studies have shown clear age of acquisition effects in adult signers, such that the earlier a sign language is learned, the better the linguistic performance later in life, both in production and in perception (see Mayberry, 2010 for a review). Fewer studies have examined critical period effects in deaf children, but some differences have been found between deaf children in deaf families and deaf children in hearing families, in both linguistic and cognitive abilities. For example, Galvan (1989) found differences between native and non-native signing deaf children between ages 3 and 9 in use of American Sign Language (ASL) morphology. Also, Courtin (2000) found that deaf children from deaf families between ages 5 and 8 scored significantly higher on false belief tasks than deaf children from hearing families within the same age range, suggesting that theory of mind is also affected by age of sign language acquisition. Furthermore, it appears that some differences between native and non-native signers might begin to emerge as early as infancy, partly due to interaction with deaf versus hearing parents. For instance, studies have shown that deaf parents interacting with their deaf children use a variety of strategies to establish and maintain joint attention with their infants (Harris et al., 1989; Maestas y Moores, 1980) and these patterns differ between deaf mothers and hearing mothers of deaf children (Harris & Mohay, 1997; Kyle, Ackerman, & Woll, 1987; Waxman & Spencer, 1997). 4 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL In the current study, we investigate the use of spatial scale and enactment (via constructed action) in narratives of deaf children aged 8-10 using British Sign Language (BSL). We compare the use of these devices in four deaf children from deaf families acquiring BSL natively, and four deaf children from hearing families with varying degrees of sign language exposure at home. All children attend a bilingual school where BSL and English are used. Given that a wide range of age of acquisition effects that have been found at various levels in adults, and given the various differences in acquisition of theory of mind in native vs. non-native signing children and eyegaze patterns used by deaf and hearing parents, we expect to find some differences in the combined use of spatial perspective and enactment in native vs. non-native signing children’s BSL narratives. 2. Background 2.1. Spatial scale Signers use the space in front of them in a variety of communicative ways. The most commonly discussed split is between a large scale use of space (where the signer uses the space as if he/she were interacting with people/objects on a real-world scale), and a small scale use of space (where the signer uses his/her hand to represent all or part of an entity on a small scale in front of the body). Some researchers also distinguish other types of space such as the space used for pointing, including pronominal signs and locative points (Liddell, 2003; Poizner, Klima, & Bellugi, 1987), and the space that is used for phonological purposes but not necessarily in any meaningful way, particularly with lexical signs (Emmorey, 2002; Poizner et al., 1987). Other terminology for these various uses of space (and related perspectives) are shown in Table 1. For the purposes of the current study, we are interested in children’s use of large-scale space on the one hand (what we refer to as character scale) 5 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL versus the use of small-scale space, pointing space and non-locative space on the other. Therefore we combine the latter three uses of space into observer scale. TABLE 1 ABOUT HERE Large-scale and small-scale uses of space are often described in terms of the prototypical depicting constructions that signers tend to use with each. Depicting constructions are predicates of location, motion, and/or handling and are considered to be part of the productive lexicon which consists of constructions which are highly variable and weakly lexicalised (Brennan, 1992, 1994; Author, in press). Small-scale space is typically associated with entity constructions which depict location and/motion of all or part of an entity, as shown in Figure 1. Large-scale space is typically associated with handling constructions which depict handling or manipulation of an object, as shown in Figure 2. FIGURE 1 ABOUT HERE FIGURE 2 ABOUT HERE Although these patterns are prototypical, other patterns may occur as well. Entity constructions are often used with large-scale space (e.g., Aarons & Morgan, 2003; Dudis, 2004; Metzger, 1995; Perniss & Ozyurek, 2008; Perniss et al., 2007; Quinto-Pozos, 2007; Sallandre, 2007; Sallandre & Cuxac, 2002; Slobin et al., 2003) which lends to a mixing of perspectives, with the signer’s body representing a referent on a large scale, and the signer’s hand representing the same (or a different) referent on a small scale, as shown below in Figure 4. The opposite pattern – i.e. use of handling constructions in small-scale space – has 6 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL been documented for only two sign languages (German Sign Language and Turkish Sign Language) and appears to be relatively infrequent (Perniss & Ozyurek, 2008). Perniss and Ozyurek (2008) also describe another even less typical pattern with scale and perspective which involves a fusion of character perspective and observer perspective – such that the signing space in front of the signer is mapped onto two different perspectives at once. Figure 3a shows a Turkish Sign Language signer describing a cartoon mouse handling a pan and flipping a pancake within the pan repeatedly (which the action occurring directly up and down at the signer’s saggital axis). On an up swing, the direction of the movement then changes laterally so that the motion and location of the falling pancake is depicted as falling toward the signer’s left side, and the signer looks down at her left side. From the perspective of the mouse in the cartoon, the pancake falls directly in front of him. As a viewer watching the cartoon, the mouse is on the right side of the screen, and the pancake on the left, as shown in Figure 3b. Thus this signer is simultaneously depicting the character’s perspective (i.e. the signer’s head and torso are mapped onto the mouse), and an observer’s perspective (i.e. her own perspective as she is watching the cartoon). Perniss and Ozyurek note that this type of fused construction only occurred in their Turkish Sign Language data (and rarely), and did not occur at all in their German Sign Language data. FIGURE 3A ABOUT HERE FIGURE 3B ABOUT HERE 2.2. Constructed action Another feature that has been noted to be particularly common with the use of largescale space is constructed action (Dudis, 2004; Metzger, 1995) – i.e. the use of the signer’s head, face, torso, arms and/or hands to directly represent the same articulator(s) of a referent. 7 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL Constructed action is certainly not limited to large-scale space, however, and may be used concurrently with any other type of space, including small-scale space - e.g., where an entity construction (small-scale space) is used simultaneously with constructed action (large-scale space). Constructed action may also be used simultaneously with pointing or non-locative space (de Beuzeville, Johnston, & Schembri, 2009; Dudis, 2004; Emmorey & Reilly, 1998; Metzger, 1995; Reilly, 2000). However, few studies have explicitly investigated the use of spatial scale with the use of constructed action. Although large-scale space is typically associated with handling constructions, it is also possible for signers to represent the arms and/or hands of a referent without any representation of handling or manipulation of objects. For example, Figure 4 shows an example of constructed action where the signer is enacting a bear about to attack someone. This is an overt example of constructed action, where all visible articulators (head, face, arms, hands, torso) are meant to map onto the equivalent articulators of the bear. FIGURE 4 ABOUT HERE Many researchers seem to agree that a change or a break in eyegaze is the main and obligatory marker of CA in sign languages (Kegl, 1985; Loew, 1984; Padden, 1986). Although shift in eyegaze is indeed seen by many as the most prominent marker of constructed action, some have claimed that such shift in eyegaze is optional, and that other markers such as facial expression and/or head/body position may mark constructed action without break in eyegaze (Metzger, 1995; Pyers & Senghas, 2007). According to Padden (Padden, 1986), both eyegaze and facial expression determine constructed action (which she refers to as role shifting) with a body position change as optional.i 8 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL 2.3. Space rotation and perspective shift Although Padden (1986) notes that a change in body position to mark role shift is optional, this body shift has traditionally been considered to be a primary (and noticeable) marker of constructed action (or role shift) within sign languages, so much so that this is often taught to hearing students learning sign language (Lentz, 1986). This change in body position involves rotating the torso one direction to represent one role, and then rotating the torso the other direction to represent another role. Padden (1986) notes that this is particularly common when representing dialogue between two people. However, Janzen (2004) argues that such shifts in body position need not occur, and that a shift in perspective can be achieved (with or without constructed action) by using the signing space in a way such that the space is used (and understood) relative to the signer’s body. Although Janzen (2004) notes that eyegaze and other non-manual features are used commonly (and in complex ways) throughout adult signers’ narratives for the purposes of indicating a particular role, it is the use of space in a particular way, rather than the use of these non-manual features, that mark that the signer is taking on a given role: “…given… the fact that eye gaze is frequently directed toward the addressee during the articulation of these clauses, it is clear that eye gaze directed toward various loci around the space is not the sole determinant of perspective and nor therefore perspective shifts” (pg. 163). 2.4. Spatial scale, constructed action and narrative skills in deaf children Deaf children start using large-scale space for the purposes of enactment as early as 23 years of age (Loew, 1984; Schick, 2006).ii Slobin et al. (2003) report that the earliest uses of depicting constructions, including handling constructions, are used before age 3 and are heavily gestural with overt use of facial expressions and body movements. The use of entity constructions within small-scale space starts around the same age (2-3 years) but progresses slowly. Furthermore, Slobin et al. (2003) suggest that after an early phase of fairly successful 9 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL mastery, there is a prolonged phase of learning to use these constructions as a flexible discourse tool. Some studies have shown that by age 8, depicting constructions have not yet been fully mastered (de Beuzeville, 2006; Schick, 1987, 1990). Even by age 12, Slobin et al. (2003) claim, children still struggle with various discourse and pragmatic functions of depicting constructions in small-scale and large-scale space. While children are developing a mastery of spatial scale, they are also developing narrative skills. Morgan (2002, 2006) studied BSL narratives of deaf children (native signers and early learners who all used BSL at home and in school) between ages 4;3 and 13;4 telling the ‘frog story’ (Frog Where Are You?, Mercer Mayer). Morgan observed that at the earliest stage (ages 4-6), the children had difficulty sequencing co-occurring events. They were able to use different types of constructions and different types of space, but not cohesively throughout the narrative. At the latest stage (ages 11-13), the children’s narratives were not yet as complex and cohesive as adults, but they were able to combine the use of large scale and small scale space simultaneously, and they were able to “flashback” in the narrative to refer to earlier episodes and then jump back ahead, as adult signers do. At the middle stage (ages 7-10), children were able to include relevant information about both characters and to switch reference between them, but they still used primarily sequential strategies for telling the narrative. In terms of development of sequential and simultaneous organisation, and also gradual development of both linguistic and gestural devices, these stages are comparable to narrative development in hearing children (Colletta, 2009). 3. Research questions The general research question for the current study is: How do deaf children at ages 810 with different signing backgrounds combine spatial scale and constructed action in BSL narratives? In particular, we compare the use of character scale (i.e. large-scale space, where 10 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL the signer can be considered ‘in’ the story space) and observer scale (small-scale space, where the signer can be considered ‘out’ of the story space), as described in §2.1, and the use of constructed action in deaf children from deaf families (native signers) and deaf children from hearing families (early learners). Given previous research, we expect that native and non-native signing children will use character scale with constructed action. Additionally we expect that native and non-native signing children will use observer scale without constructed action. As noted above, these patterns have been documented in both native and non-native signing children around ages 23 years (Schick, 2006; Slobin et al., 2003). We leave open the question of whether we may find the use of small-scale space with constructed action, or large-scale space without constructed action. The former has been noted to be rare in adult signers of German Sign Language and infrequent in adult signers of Turkish Sign Language (Perniss & Ozyurek, 2008). The latter has not been explicitly documented or discussed in any detail in the sign language literature, though as noted above, Janzen (2004) does suggest that the use of character scale without constructed action is possible. 4. Method 4.1. Participants Participants in this study include a total of 8 profoundly deaf children: 4 deaf children from deaf families and 4 deaf children from hearing families, as shown in Table 2. Four of the children (DD) are from deaf families with deaf parents. The generations of deafness in each family varied from one generation (deaf parents but hearing grandparents) to four generations (deaf great grandparents). All four children attended a school for the deaf with a bilingual policy – i.e. where BSL was used in the classroom alongside written English. Four of the children (DH) are from hearing families with no other deaf members of the family. Two of 11 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL the children had some (minimal) signing input from their families but attended a deaf unit within a hearing school with a bilingual policy with BSL used in the classroom so they did have daily signing input at school starting from around age 5. The other two children are from single parent households but both mothers have good signing skills (one with a qualification as a BSL/English interpreter). No other members of their families could sign. One of the DH children attended a deaf school with a bilingual policy and the other three attended a deaf unit within a hearing school with a bilingual policy. TABLE 2 ABOUT HERE 4.2. Stimulus Narratives were elicited from the children using a Pink Panther cartoon clip called “Keep our Forests Pink”. This cartoon has no spoken dialogue. The full length of the clip that was shown is 2.53 minutes. The current study focuses on the part of the children’s narratives that covered one particular 45-second excerpt from the cartoon. In this 45-second excerpt, a man comes out of a tent holding an axe and sees a signpost that says “NO OPEN FIRES”. The man walks up to the signpost and chops it down. While the man is busy, a dog comes out from behind the trees. As the dog approaches, he sees that the man has started a fire using the wood from the signpost. The dog happily curls up in front of the fire, and the man leaves. The Pink Panther then arrives, wearing a park ranger hat. Alarmed by the fire, he runs off to get some water, comes back on the scene with a bucket of water and pours it on the fire, putting it out. This wakes up the sleeping dog. The Pink Panther, satisfied that he has done his duty, walks off the scene calmly. The man then comes back out of the tent, smiling and carrying a frying pan, and walks toward the fire (not realising that it had been put out). The dog smiles, pleased to see the man with the frying pan. The man stops when he sees the 12 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL dampened fire and picks up a dripping stick from the remains of the fire. The dog is still grinning and wagging his tail. The man, dropping the stick, then points accusingly at the dog, as if blaming him for putting out the fire. The dog looks back with an innocent expression and shakes his head. Importantly, from the perspective of the viewer, all of the primary movements of the characters within the cartoon (i.e. entering and leaving the scene) are from right to left or from left to right of the screen. 4.3. Task The children met one at a time with the researcher, Author, a Deaf native signer of BSL with many years of experience working with deaf children. The children were told they would be watching a cartoon on video and that the researcher had never seen the cartoon before. They were to watch the video and remember it so they could explain it to the researcher. They were told they would have the chance to watch the clip again. The child would watch the cartoon with either the mother or the father (if at the child’s home) or with a colleague (if at the child’s school) whilst the researcher left the room or read a book in another part of the room. After the second viewing, each child then described the cartoon to the researcher. The researcher showed appropriate interest in the story and if anything was unclear the child was questioned. 4.4. Coding All narratives were annotated by the first author (XX) using the ELAN multimedia annotation tool (Wittenburg, Brugman, Russel, Klassmann, & Sloetjes, 2006). Each child’s narrative was first glossed in English using two gloss tiers, one for the dominant hand and one for the non-dominant hand. Additionally, narratives were coded for spatial scale and constructed action on another tier. Figure 5 shows an example screenshot of the tiers used for 13 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL coding. The narratives also coded by two research assistants who were given the coding criteria described below and asked to comment on any coding that they thought did not follow the coding guidelines: one research assistant commented on all 8 child narratives, and a second research assistant independently commented on 4 of those 8 narratives. Some annotations were changed based on these comments; for others, disagreements were resolved. FIGURE 5 ABOUT HERE 4.4.1. Scale/CA tier. On the scale/CA tier, there were four different options: character scale with constructed action, character scale without constructed action, observer scale with constructed action, and observer scale without constructed action. We define constructed action as the use of one or more manual or non-manual articulators to represent the actions, thoughts, feelings or attitudes of a referent. This was operationalised via a dependency between articulators used for constructed action and the role(s) represented by those articulators (Author, under review(a); Author, under review(b)). That is, if it was suspected that one or more articulators (e.g. eyegaze, face, head, torso, arms, hands) were being used for constructed action (via native signer intuition), one or more character roles needed to be attributed to those articulators. Likewise, if it was suspected that one or more character roles was being represented (via native signer intuition), then one or more articulators needed to be identified as the marker(s) of that constructed action. In all cases, we define entity constructions as any construction where the hand represents an entity in order to describe location and/or motion of that referent. 1. Character scale with CA (CSCA) was coded when the child was using constructed action and the space surrounding the child was used on a real-world scale as if the child was taking on the role of that referent. Character scale with CA could occur 14 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL alone without any lexical signs or depicting constructions (as in Figure 6), or it could include simultaneous use of lexical signs, pointing signs, entity constructions (as in Figure 4 or 5) or handling constructions (as in Figure 2). 2. Observer scale with CA (OSCA) was coded where the child was using constructed action and the signing space in front of the child was not used on a real-world scale – i.e. if the space were used on a small scale using entity constructions (as in Figure 1, except with constructed action), or use of space for articulation of lexical signs. 3. Observer scale without CA (OSnoCA) was coded when the child was taking on the perspective of an observer, using the signing space for lexical signs and/or partly lexical constructions such as entity constructions to describe events, with no evidence of co-occurring constructed action. 4. Character scale without CA (CSnoCA) was coded when the child was taking on the perspective of an observer using entity classifier constructions to represent referent(s) in the signing space relative to his/her own body as another referent, with no evidence of co-occuring constructed action. 4.4.2. Segmentation of scale/CA annotations. Segmentation of annotations for scale/CA proceeded as follows. From the beginning of the narrative, an annotation for scale/CA was begun, with one of the four values above. The end of this annotation was considered to be when there was a change in scale/CA (e.g. from CSCA to OSnoCA or from CSnoCA to CSCA), or when there was a change in role (e.g. from CSCA representing the Pink Panther to CSCA representing the dog), or when there was a change in subject (e.g. from OSnoCA with the man as the subject to OSnoCA with the Pink Panther as the subject).iii 3. Results Here we report the frequencies of each of the 4 possible combinations of spatial scale and constructed action, within each group of children (DD and DH), as shown in Table 3. Table 4 15 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL summarises which combinations of spatial scale and constructed action occurred (or did not occur) in each group. Below we go through each combination of spatial scale and constructions action in turn. TABLE 3 ABOUT HERE TABLE 4 ABOUT HERE 5.1. Character scale with constructed action (CSCA) Character scale with constructed action was used by all children from both groups. Figure 6 replicates DH3 representing the man looking down, putting the wood into a pile before lighting it up with matches.iv FIGURE 6 ABOUT HERE All of the children produced tokens of CSCA similar to these, for these events and for others within the cartoon. This is hardly surprising, given that these enacting constructions have been documented in deaf children as early as 2-3 years of age, and that hearing children produce similar enacting gestures along with their speech by a similar age, as noted above in §2.4. The examples given here show some fairly overt tokens of constructed action within character scale, such that the head, face, torso, arms and hands all appear active as markers of constructed action. More subtle uses of constructed action within character scale such as use of only eyegaze or facial expression were also identified. For example, Figure 7 replicates DD4 producing the lexical sign WALK, with a slight break in eyegaze with the addressee 16 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL (who was just next to the camera), and with the head and face slightly enacting the Pink Panther within the cartoon. FIGURE 7 ABOUT HERE 5.2. Observer scale without constructed action (OSnoCA) Observer scale without constructed action was also used by all children in all groups. This was used for a variety of purposes, including establishment of subject and object arguments via lexical noun signs as well as entity constructions without CA (as shown above in Figure 1 and below in Figure 8). In Figure 8, the child represents two entity constructions, one on each arm/hand, to represent the dog in the story and a tree. This child, a deaf child from a hearing family (DH3), had some difficulty negotiating his left hand around his upright right arm when showing that the dog comes around from behind the tree. FIGURE 8 ABOUT HERE 5.3. Observer scale with constructed action (OSCA) The use of observer scale with constructed action occurred much less frequently than CSCA or OSnoCA. In fact, we only found 4 tokens of OSCA, from two deaf children from hearing families (DH2 and DH4). Three tokens of OSCA occurred in DH2’s narrative after he has described the Pink Panther throwing water on the fire and leaving the scene. In the cartoon, the Pink Panther simply leaves the scene and does not return (presumably he has left the scene entirely). In DH4’s narrative, he uses CA to “become” the Pink Panther hiding behind a tree (an event which not actually occur in the cartoon), with simultaneous use of his right arm representing a tree (as an entity construction, in observer scale), and use of his head 17 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL and shoulders hunched down behind his upright forearm (i.e. constructed action representing someone, presumably the Pink Panther, hiding behind the tree). Simultaneously, with his left hand, with his arm outstretched, he produces an entity construction representing someone coming on to the scene, from left to right, as shown in Figure 9. During his narrative, DH4 produces the construction shown in Figure 9, then signs MAN with his left hand, then repeats Figure 9. Then still with his arm outstretched, in the same location where he shows the man walking on the scene with an entity construction, he also signs with his left hand FIRE STOP (i.e. ‘the fire went out’). Thus he is fusing two different scales here: character scale via CA representing someone behind a tree hiding, and also observer scale, via the entity construction and the placement of his lexical signs in the signing space well in front of him, from the child’s own perspective as viewer of the cartoon. FIGURE 9 ABOUT HERE During DH2’s narrative he does not mention the Pink Panther or the fire being extinguished. When asked what happened to the fire, DH2 says that he doesn’t know, that maybe someone poured water on it. (See §5.5 for more discussion of this.) When asked who put out the fire, he says he doesn’t know and then ME SEE MAN followed by the directional verb LOOK with constructed action. Specifically, DH2 signs LOOK which he moves from left to right (as shown in Figure 10), indicating that someone is watching the man walk from left to right as he comes out of the tent. This is accompanied by simultaneous constructed action with his head and eyegaze (both of which follow the path of LOOK from left to right), a representation of the child himself as viewer of the cartoon casually watching the man walk past on the screen. FIGURE 10 ABOUT HERE 18 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL Importantly, the direction of DH4’s entity construction representing the man walking out of the tent was from the left to the right of DH4’s signing space, and likewise DH2’s directional verb LOOK indicating someone watching the man walking from the tent also moved from his left to his right. These cues indicate that both children were representing the spatial layout of the scene as an observer, since the movement of the man as viewed by the child as an observer is from the left to right, while the movement of the man from the perspective of anyone hiding behind the tree would have been right to left, and from the perspective of the dog the man would have been approaching from in front. 5.4. Character scale with no constructed action (CSnoCA) The use of character scale with no constructed action was also not very frequent. This combination of scale and CA was found only in the deaf children from deaf families, and was identifiable by the use of entity constructions depicting the movement of the characters in the cartoon forwards and backwards. Because the movement of the characters as viewed by the child on screen was mainly left to right or right to left, the fact that these children were using entity constructions forwards and backwards suggests that they were taking on a character perspective – i.e. that the position and the movement of the entity constructions was meant to be with respect to their own (imagined) position within the story, rather than representations of the spatial layout as an external observer of the scene (cf. OSCA above). These examples (as shown in Figure 11) all lacked constructed action, showing that these children were able to position themselves in the story without needing to embody the characters at all. This may be due to the importance of location and motion in these narratives – i.e. showing how and where the different characters came onto and left the scene. For example, when DD4 has described how the Pink Panther put the fire out and then left the scene, she then signs MAN 19 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL and then an entity construction moving forward, representing the man coming out of the tent and back on the scene, as shown in Figure 11. In these cases it appears that the children focused on showing these arrivals and departures of characters by assuming the position of a character already on the scene. Importantly, because they are not using constructed action, the only cue to their assuming this position is the use of space of the entity constructions around them, relative to their own position. Three of the 4 DD children used this type of construction; none of the DH children did. FIGURE 11 ABOUT HERE 5.5 Individual patterns There was some individual variation in the use of scale and CA across all children. Each of the DD children used both CSCA and OSnoCA in their narratives. DD2 used only CSCA and OSnoCA – i.e. fully “in” as character or fully “out” as narrator. The remaining DD children (i.e. DD1, DD3, and DD4) additionally used CSnoCA, with clear transitions between CSnoCA and CSCA, or between CSnoCA and OSnoCA. For example, immediately after the token of CSnoCA shown in Figure 15, DD4, then goes fully “into” character as the man (CSCA) holding the saucepan. Likewise DD1 uses CSnoCA to show the man leaving the scene then clearly transitions into OSnoCA to explain as narrator that the man is going into the tent to fetch his frying pan. Within the DH children, all of them used CSCA. DH1 used CSCA only, and thus was fully “in” character during his whole narrative (including walking around the room even though he began seated). The remaining DH children used CSCA and OSnoCA, and two of them also used OSCA (which none of the DD children did). 20 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL DH2’s narrative was notable in that he did not mention the Pink Panther at all. He described the man coming on the scene, cutting down the signpost, making the fire, and the dog being happy. But rather than saying that the man left and the Pink Panther arrived and extinguished the fire, he then skipped ahead to the man scolding the dog. When asked how the fire was extinguished, DH2 said that he didn’t know and that perhaps someone poured water on it. When asked who might have done that, he said that he didn’t know and then explained how he had seen the man walk across the screen (using the directional verb LOOK as described in §5.3). The man in the cartoon does not know who put the fire out, so it is interesting that DH2 also claims to not know how the fire was put out. When describing the dog waking up after the fire has been put out, DH3 uses constructed action to represent the dog waking up surprised. While keeping one hand held as the dog’s paw, he then goes on to sign WHAT-FOR BAD BEHAVE (“why this bad behaviour?”), with his head and eyegaze directed downward. Both the scolding content of the lexical signs WHAT-FOR BAD BEHAVE as well as the downward direction of his head and eyegaze should represent the Pink Panther, and yet his non-dominant hand is kept as the dog throughout this stretch of CA. It may be possible that the use of OSCA in DH2 and DH4 is linked to the fact that they had little if any signing exposure at home, compared to DH1 and DH3 whose mothers both signed with them at home (cf. Table 2). However, this does not mean that DH1 and DH3 were more native-like in their use of scale and CA, given that DH1’s narrative was entirely in CSCA, and DH3’s narrative included some conflicting uses of constructed action. 6. Discussion As we initially hypothesised, the use of CSCA and OSnoCA was similar for both groups of children – i.e. deaf children from deaf families (DD) and deaf children from 21 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL hearing families (DH). This is consistent with the early use of enactment and entity constructions reported in deaf children as early as 2-3 years of age (Loew, 1984; Schick, 1987; Slobin et al., 2003; Supalla, 1982). It seems that by ages 8-10 deaf children have mastered these combinations of CA and perspective well enough that there are no differences in frequency in narratives between native and non-native signing children. This is true even though two of the non-native signing children had little or no exposure to BSL until they started school at age 5. This is not surprising if we consider that hearing children have also been found use visible forms of enactment very early in their gestures (McNeill, 1992). In our initial hypotheses, we left open the question of whether we may find small-scale space with constructed action (OSCA) or large-scale space without constructed action (CSnoCA). Interestingly we found opposing patterns with these two uses of spatial scale and constructed action. That is, only deaf children from deaf families used CSnoCA (no deaf children from hearing families did), while only deaf children from hearing families used OSCA (no deaf children from deaf families did). The tokens of OSCA identified in the current study were identified only in the DH children, not in the DD children. Furthermore, these tokens seem odd for an adult signer of BSL. These OSCA tokens are very similar to the “fused” perspective constructions identified by Perniss and Ozyurek (2008). These “fused” constructions were found to be rare in the Turkish Sign Language (TID) data and did not occur at all in their German Sign Language (DGS) data. Perniss and Ozyurek argue that this difference may be due to language-specific patterns, such that different sign languages might impose different constraints on the use of space and perspective. Given these few tokens of OSCA that have occurred in the current study, there may be a few other explanations for Perniss and Ozyurek’s findings. One may be the age of acquisition of their Turkish signers. They note that all of their participants were native or early learners of TID – i.e. all had learned TID at age 6 or earlier. If all of their 22 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL participants who produced “fused” productions were non-native TID signers, then this would be consistent with the findings of the current study, where only the deaf children from hearing families produced OSCA. However, this does not appear to be the case; three of the four TID signers from the Perniss and Ozyurek study were native signers, including the signer shown in Figure 3a who had deaf parents and grandparents (Perniss, personal communication). Therefore another explanation is needed. One possibility may be the age of TID as a language. Perniss and Ozyurek note that the first deaf school in Turkey was established in 1902. BSL and DGS are both older sign languages, with the first deaf schools in Britain (Jackson, 2001) and Germany (Vogel, 1999, cited in Perniss & Ozyurek, 2008) being established more than 100 years earlier than in Turkey. Sign languages can change rapidly within that amount of time, as we have seen with the evolution of Nicaraguan Sign Language in the past 40 years (Senghas, 2000, 2003; Senghas & Coppola, 2001; Senghas, Coppola, Newport, & Supalla, 1997). Of particular relevance here is the speed with which has Nicaraguan Sign Language developed systematic spatial modulations. For example, Senghas (2003) reports that in a task which requires spatial rotation for the purposes of verb directionality, first cohort adult signers (i.e. those who attended the first school for the deaf between 1974 and 1983) were largely unrestricted in their spatial rotation for verb agreement purposes. However, second cohort adult signers (i.e. who attended the school between 1985 and 1990) were much more consistent in their rotation patterns, i.e. more consistently rotating their signing space to take on the perspective of the referent. Thus, it may be that the relative youth of TID compared to BSL and DGS accounts for why “fused” constructions (i.e. a mixture of two scales) may occur in TID but may not occur in BSL or DGS. The fact that a few of the DH children in the current study used this type of construction in BSL (OSCA) may reflect early stages in both language diachrony and child development. v 23 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL The tokens of CSnoCA in this study were identified only in the DD children, not in the DH children. With CSCA, signers position themselves “in” the story space in terms of how they use the space around them, but also use elements of constructed action. With CSnoCA, the use of space is the only cue that the signer is positioning themselves “in” the story space; there is no CA to additionally give this indication. The use of CSnoCA is consistent with Janzen’s (2004) claim that a shift in signer’s perspective can be achieved by using the signing space in a way such that the space is used relative to the signer’s body, and the implication that this can occur with or without constructed action. Janzen’s study was based on personal experience narratives by adult native signers of ASL. The fact that we see in the current study the use of CSnoCA in BSL narratives in native signing children (but not non-native signing children) at 8-10 years of age suggests that native exposure to a sign language is required for this skill to be acquired by children by this age. This may be linked to differences in eyegaze as used by deaf versus hearing mothers when deaf children are infants (Harris & Mohay, 1997; Kyle et al., 1987; Waxman & Spencer, 1997). It could also be linked to age of acquisition effects that have been found for theory of mind in deaf children via false belief tasks (Courtin, 2000). Whether there is CA or not, the use of character scale does require taking on someone else’s perspective. However, CSCA involves full embodiment where various articulators on the signer’s body are mapped onto the other referent and also the space around the signer is used as if the signer is the referent. CSnoCA is similar but requires the ability of the signer to “switch off” CA via his/her bodily articulators and only use the space as if he/she is the referent. Thus CSnoCA is a quite complex use of spatial scale compared to CSCA. It may be that the early use of sign language appropriate eyegaze patterns that deaf native signing children are exposed to early on as infants, and the increased abilities of theory of mind in deaf native signing children, both help 24 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL explain why this complex CSnoCA is found in native signing children but not non-native signing children in these data. In addition to the presence of OSCA and lack of CSnoCA in the DH children, all four of the DH children exhibited some problems with their narrative skills, This included embellishment of narratives with events that did not occur in the cartoon and likewise omission of important events in the cartoon. For example, DH4 narrated that the Pink Panther hid behind a tree (as described in §5.3 above), and DH3 described the Pink Panther scolding the dog (the Pink Panther does not even acknowledge the dog in the cartoon). Also, DH2 omitted the Pink Panther from his narrative entirely, and when asked about who put the fire out, he claimed that he did not know, even though this is an important event that the addressee would need to know in order to understand the story. Additionally, as noted above, DH1 mimed the entire narrative so in a sense there was no real narration at all. These patterns contrast with Morgan’s (2002, 2006) studies on BSL narratives in native signing / early learning deaf children who use BSL both at school and at home. As noted in §2.4, these children at ages 7-10 were able to include relevant information about characters. The fact that the DH children in the current study lacked important (or included inaccurate) information, or lacked narration altogether, suggests that early exposure at both home and school (not just at school) is important for acquisition of narrative skills. These findings have important implications for our understanding of age of acquisition effects in sign languages. While age of acquisition effects (i.e. differences between native and non-native signers) have been found in deaf adults at various levels of sign language grammar, there have been fewer studies examining age of acquisition effects in deaf children. The current study suggests that cognitive abilities such as perspective taking and use of spatial scale should be considered along with linguistic abilities when looking at age of acquisition effects. 25 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL There are also important implications for the sign linguistics literature which has largely focussed on the use of constructed action with or without use of depicting constructions (Aarons & Morgan, 2003; Dudis, 2004; Metzger, 1995; Quinto-Pozos, 2007) or spatial scale with depicting constructions (Perniss, 2007; Perniss & Ozyurek, 2008). Very rarely are all three considered together – i.e. spatial scale, constructed action, and depicting constructions (Janzen, 2004 may be one exception). The current study has shown subtle but important differences between native and non-native signing children in the combined use of spatial perspective and constructed action, which would not have been found if we had only compared one or the other. To our knowledge, the use of CSnoCA in signers has not been described explicitly in the literature before. More attention to CSnoCA (and also OSCA), and further evidence from larger datasets, may help us further untangle what may often be subtle differences in age of acquisition related to perspective, space and enactment within sign languages. 7. Conclusion The current study has addressed the question of how deaf children from deaf and hearing families use scale in their signing space and how this interacts with use of constructed action. We have found that deaf signing children at ages 8-10 – whether from deaf families or hearing families - use enactment in their narratives within large-scale space, as if they become fully “in” the space, and also that these children use the signing space on a small scale for topographic reference and/or for articulation of lexical material without enactment. These similarities reflect the primacy of enactment and straight narration (e.g. via lexical material or topographic constructions) in sign language development. Furthermore, we have found differences between native and non-native signing deaf children in (a) the use of observer scale with constructed action, which may reflect early stages in child language 26 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL development and language diachrony, and (b) the use of character scale without constructed action, which may require native sign language exposure to arise in narratives by ages 8-10. These groups also exhibited differences in narrative skills. Together, these differences reflect age of acquisition effects which may not be at a purely grammatical level reinforcing the notion that cognitive abilities are also subject to such effects. References Aarons, D., & Morgan, R. (2003). Classifier predicates and the creation of multiple perspectives in South African Sign Language. Sign Language Studies, 3(2), 125-156. doi: 10.1353/sls.2003.0001 Alibasic Ciciliani, T., & Wilbur, R. B. (2006). Pronominal system in Croatian Sign Language. Sign Language and Linguistics, 9(1-2), 95-132. doi: 10.1075/sll.9.1.07ali Anderson, D., & Reilly, J. (2002). The MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory: Normative data for American Sign Language. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 7(2), 83-106. doi: 10.1093/deafed/7.2.83 Bahan, B. (1996). Non-manual realization of agreement in American Sign Language. (PhD dissertation), Boston University, Boston. Brennan, M. (1992). An introduction to the visual world of BSL. In D. Brien (Ed.), Dictionary of British Sign Language/English (pp. 1-133). London: Faber & Faber. Brennan, M. (1994). Pragmatics and productivity. In I. Ahlgren, B. Bergman & M. Brennan (Eds.), Perspectives on sign language usage: Papers from the Fifth International Symposium on Sign Language Research (Vol. 1, pp. 371-390). Durham: ISLA. Colletta, J.-M. (2009). Comparative analysis of children’s narratives at different ages. Gesture, 9(1), 61-96. doi: 10.1075/gest.9.1.03col 27 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL Courtin, C. (2000). The impact of sign language on the development of deaf children: The case of theories of mind. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 5(3), 266-276. doi: 10.1093/deafed/5.3.266 de Beuzeville, L. (2006). Visual and Linguistic Representation in the Acquisition of Depicting Verbs: a study of native signing deaf children of Auslan (Australian Sign Language). (PhD dissertation), University of Newcastle, Callaghan, New South Wales, Australia. de Beuzeville, L., Johnston, T., & Schembri, A. (2009). The use of space with indicating verbs in Australian Sign Language: A corpus-based investigation. Sign Language and Linguistics, 12(1), 52-83. doi: 10.1075/sl&l.12.1.03deb Dudis, P. G. (2004). Body partitioning and real-space blends. Cognitive Linguistics, 15(2), 223-238. doi: 10.1515/cogl.2004.009 Emmorey, K. (2002). Language, Cognition, and the Brain: Insights from Sign Language Research. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Emmorey, K., & Reilly, J. (1998). The Development of quotation and reported action: Conveying perspective in ASL. In E. Clark (Ed.), Proceedings of the Twenty-ninth Annual Child Language Research Forum (pp. 81-90). Stanford, CA: CSLI Press. Galvan, D. (1989). A Sensitive Period for the Acquisition of Complex Morphology: Evidence from American Sign Language. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, 28, 107-114. Harris, M., Clibbens, J., Chasin, J., & Tibbitts, R. (1989). The social context of early sign language development. First Language, 9(25), 81-97. doi: 10.1177/014272378900902507 28 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL Harris, M., & Mohay, H. (1997). Learning to look in the right place: A comparison of attentional behavior in deaf children with deaf and hearing mothers. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2(2), 95-103. Jackson, P. W. (2001). A Pictorial History of Deaf Britain. Winsford, UK: Deafprint. Janzen, T. (2004). Space rotation, perspective shift, and verb morphology in ASL. Cognitive Linguistics, 15(2), 149-174. doi: 10.1515/cogl.2004.006 Kegl, J. (1985). Causative marking and the construal of agency in ASL. In W. H. Eilfort, P. D. Kroeber & K. L. Peterson (Eds.), Proceedings from the Twenty-First Regional of the Chicago Linguistic Society (CLS 21) (pp. 120-137). Chicago, IL: Chicago Linguistics Society. Kyle, J., Ackerman, J., & Woll, B. (1987). Early mother-infant interactions: Language and pre-language in deaf families. In P. Griffiths, A. Mills & J. Local (Eds.), Proceedings of the Child Language Seminar: University of York. Lentz, E. M. (1986). Teaching Role Shifting. In C. A. Padden (Ed.), Proceedings of the Fourth National Symposium on Sign Language Research and Teaching (pp. 58-69). Silver Spring, MD: NAD. Liddell, S. K. (2003). Grammar, gesture and meaning in American Sign Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Loew, R. C. (1984). Roles and Reference in American Sign Language: A Developmental Perspective. (PhD dissertation), University of Minnesota, Minnesota. Maestas y Moores, J. (1980). Early linguistic environment: Interactions of deaf parents with their infants. Sign Language Studies, 26, 1-13. Mayberry, R. I. (2010). Early Language Acquisition and Adult Language Ability: What Sign Language reveals about the Critical Period for Language. In M. Marschark & P. 29 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL Spencer (Eds.), Oxford Handbook of Deaf Studies, Language, and Education: Volume 2. Oxford: Oxford University Press. McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind: What gestures reveal about thought. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Metzger, M. (1995). Constructed dialogue and constructed action in American Sign Language. In C. Lucas (Ed.), Sociolinguistics in Deaf Communities (pp. 255-271). Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press. Meurant, L. (2008). The Speaker’s Eye Gaze: Creating deictic, anaphoric and pseudo-deictic spaces of reference. In R. M. d. Quadros (Ed.), Sign Languages: Spinning and Unraveling the Past, Present and Future. TISLR 9, Forty-five Papers and Three Posters from the 9th Theoretical Issues in Sign Language Research Conference (pp. 403-414). Petrópolis/RJ. Brazil: Editora Arara Azul. Mitchell, R. E., & Karchmer, M. (2004). Chasing the mythical ten percent: Parental hearing status of deaf and hard of hearing students in the United States. Sign Language Studies, 4(2), 138-163. doi: 10.1353/sls.2004.0005 Morgan, G. (2002). Children's Encoding of Simultaneity in British Sign Language Narratives. Sign Language & Linguistics, 5(2), 131-165. doi: 10.1075/sll.5.2.04mor Morgan, G. (2006). The development of narrative skills in British Sign Language. In B. S. Schick, M. Marschark & P. Spencer (Eds.), Advances in the Development of Sign language by Deaf Children (pp. 314-343). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Neidle, C., Kegl, J., MacLaughlin, D., Bahan, B., & Lee, R. (2000). The Syntax of American Sign Language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Padden, C. A. (1986). Verbs and role shifting in American Sign Language. In C. Padden (Ed.), Proceedings of the Fourth National Symposium on Sign Language Research and Teaching (pp. 44-57). Silver Spring, MD: NAD. 30 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL Perniss, P. (2007). Space and iconicity in German Sign Language (DGS). Nijmegen, Netherlands: Radboud University. Perniss, P., & Ozyurek, A. (2008). Constructing action and locating referents: A comparison of German and Turkish Sign Language narratives. In J. Quer (Ed.), Signs of the time. Selected papers from TISLR 8. Hamburg: Signum Press. Perniss, P., Pfau, R., & Steinbach, M. (2007). Can't you see the difference? Sources of variation in sign language structure. In P. Perniss, R. Pfau & M. Steinbach (Eds.), Visible Variation: Comparative Studies on Sign Language Structure (pp. 1-34). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Poizner, H., Klima, E. S., & Bellugi, U. (1987). What the hands reveal about the brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Pyers, J. E., & Senghas, A. (2007). Reported action in Nicaraguan and American Sign Languages: Emerging versus established systems. In P. M. Perniss, R. Pfau & M. Steinbach (Eds.), Visible Variation: Comparative Studies on Sign Language Structure (pp. 279-302). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Quinto-Pozos, D. (2007). Can constructed action be considered obligatory? Lingua, 117, 1285-1314. doi: 10.1016/j.lingua.2005.12.003 Reilly, J. (2000). Bringing affective expression into the service of language: Acquiring perspective marking in narratives. In K. Emmorey & H. Lane (Eds.), The Signs of Language Revisited: An Anthology to Honor Ursula Bellugi and Edward Klima (pp. 415-433). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Sallandre, M.-A. (2007). Simultaneity in French Sign Language discourse. In M. Vermeerbergen, L. Leeson & O. Crasborn (Eds.), Simultaneity in signed languages: Form and function (pp. 103-126). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 31 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL Sallandre, M.-A., & Cuxac, C. (2002). Iconicity in Sign Language: A Theoretical and Methodological Point of View. In I. Wachsmuth & T. Sowa (Eds.), Gesture and Sign Language in Human-Computer Interaction (Vol. 171-180). Berlin: Springer. Schick, B. S. (1987). The Acquisition of Classifier Predicates in American Sign Language. (PhD dissertation), Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN. Schick, B. S. (1990). Classifier Predicates in American Sign Language. International Journal of Sign Linguistics, 1(1), 15-40. Schick, B. S. (2006). Acquiring a visually motivated language: Evidence from diverse learners. In B. S. Schick, M. Marschark & P. Spencer (Eds.), Advances in the Development of Sign language by Deaf Children (pp. 102-134). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Senghas, A. (2000). The Development of Early Spatial Morphology in Nicaraguan Sign Language. Proceedings of the Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, 24(2), 696-707. Senghas, A. (2003). Intergenerational influence and ontogenetic development in the emergence of spatial grammar in Nicaraguan Sign Language. Cognitive Development, 18, 511-531. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2003.09.006 Senghas, A., & Coppola, M. (2001). Children creating language: How Nicaraguan Sign Language acquired a spatial grammar. Psychological Science, 12(4). Senghas, A., Coppola, M., Newport, E. L., & Supalla, T. (1997). Argument Structure in Nicaraguan Sign Language: The Emergence of Grammatical Devices. Proceedings of the Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, 21(2), 550-561. Slobin, D., Hoiting, N., Kuntze, M., Lindert, R., Weinberg, A., Pyers, J., . . . Thurmann, H. (2003). A cognitive/functional perspective on the acquisition of "classifiers". In K. 32 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL Emmorey (Ed.), Perspectives on classifier constructions in sign languages (271-296 ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Supalla, T. (1982). Structure and Acquisition of Verbs of Motion and Location in American Sign Language. (PhD dissertation), University of California at San Diego, San Diego, CA. Thompson, R., Emmorey, K., & Kluender, R. (2006). The relationship between eye gaze and verb agreement in American Sign Language: An eye-tracking study. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 24(2), 571-604. doi: 10.1007/s11049-005-1829-y Van Valin, R., & LaPolla, R. (1997). Syntax, Structure, Meaning and Function. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Vogel, H. (1999). Geschichte der Gehörlosenbildung. In A. Beecken, J. Keller, S. Prillwitz & H. Zienert (Eds.), Grundkurs Deutsche Gebärdensprache, Stufe I, Arbeitsbuch (pp. 46-49). Hamburg: Signum Press. Waxman, R., & Spencer, P. (1997). What mothers do to support infant visual attention: Sensitivities to age and hearing status. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2, 104-114. Wittenburg, P., Brugman, H., Russel, A., Klassmann, A., & Sloetjes, H. (2006). ELAN: a Professional Framework for Multimodality Research Proceedings of LREC 2006, Fifth International Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation. Woll, B., Sutton-Spence, R., & Waters, D. (Producer). (2004). ECHO data set for British Sign Language (BSL). Retrieved from http://www.let.ru.nl/sign-lang/echo 33 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL i It has also been argued that eyegaze grammatically marks person in pronouns and in agreement verbs (Alibasic Ciciliani & Wilbur, 2006; Bahan, 1996; Meurant, 2008; Neidle, Kegl, MacLaughlin, Bahan, & Lee, 2000; Thompson, Emmorey, & Kluender, 2006), although there have been no clear attempts to distinguish eyegaze as person marker from eyegaze as a marker of constructed action. ii This is not limited to deaf children; hearing children also make use of large-scale space in enacting/iconic gestures, and they start doing so at a similar age (McNeill, 1992). iii Our notion of ‘subject’ here corresponds to Van Valin & LaPolla ‘s (1997) semantic macrorole ‘actor’ which in the case of these narratives is the animate character executing the action. iv Data collection for this study took place in 1993, before it became widely standard within sign language research to obtain indefinite consent for specific uses of video clips in publications. Therefore, examples are modeled here by a deaf adult BSL signer, replicated as closely as possible to the actual productions of the child participants in this study. v It would be useful to know if OSCA constructions occur in non-native adult signers of older sign languages like BSL, as evidence of whether OSCA as identified in non-native child signers of BSL might persist into adulthood. It would also be useful to know at what age this type of construction emerges in native signing children, or if this stage is bypassed by native signing children completely. We leave these issues for future research. 34 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL Table 1 Overview of Types of Signing Space as Described by Different Researchers Character scale Observer scale Large-scale space Small-scale space Pointing Emmorey Viewer space (2002) Diagrammatic Non-locative space space -- Articulatory space -- space Schick (1987) Real-world space Model space -- Liddell (2003) Surrogate space Token space -- Depicting space Morgan (2002, Shifted referential Fixed referential -- 2006) space (SRS) Poizner et al. Shifted referential Topographic space Fixed (1987) space -- space (FRS) Syntactic space referential space Perniss (2007) Character Observer perspective perspective Slobin et al. Protagonist Narrator (2003) perspective perspective -- -- -- -- 35 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL Table 2 Participant Details Participant Age of Family background Communication at Type of Communication code child home school method of school DH1 8 Mother signs at Deaf unit Bilingual Bilingual Hearing family home DH2 9 Hearing family Parents sign a little Deaf unit DH3 9 Hearing family Mother signs at Deaf school Bilingual home DH4 9 Hearing family Parents sign a little Deaf unit Bilingual DD1 9 3-4 generation deaf BSL Deaf school Bilingual BSL Deaf school Bilingual Deaf parents, hearing BSL Deaf school Bilingual family, deaf siblings DD2 10 3 generation deaf family, hearing sibling DD3 9 grandparents, deaf siblings DD4 10 2 generation deaf BSL Deaf school Bilingual family, deaf and hearing siblings 36 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL Table 3 Frequency of Spatial Scale and Constructed Action Use: Number of Tokens by Participant Participant code CSCA OSnoCA OSCA CSnoCA DH1 9 0 0 0 DH2 6 5 1 0 DH3 10 7 0 0 DH4 9 7 3 0 Total DH 34 19 4 0 DD1 10 10 0 3 DD2 10 11 0 0 DD3 13 19 0 3 DD4 9 11 0 2 Total DD 42 51 0 8 37 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL Table 4 Use of Spatial Scale and Constructed Action in DD and DH Group Narratives DD DH CSCA OSnoCA OSCA CSnoCA 38 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL Figure 1. Depicting (entity) construction representing an upright entity moving from right to left in small-scale space 39 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL Figure 2. Depicting (handling) construction representing handling a flat object in large-scale space 40 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL <IMAGE PERMISSIONS TBC> Figure 3a. Fused character and observer perspective production in Turkish Sign Language, describing scenes shown in Figure 3b (reprinted with permission from Perniss & Ozyurek, 2008). 41 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL Figure 3b. Stills from stimulus for Figure 3a (from Perniss & Ozyurek, 2008) Reprinted with permission: © I. Schmitt-Menzel / Friedrich Streich, WDR mediagroup GmbH, Die Sendung mit der Maus ® WDR 42 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL Figure 4. Constructed action in large-scale space with no handling/manipulation (Woll et al., 2004) 43 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL Figure 5. ELAN tiers and annotations 44 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL Figure 6. Example from DH3 using CSCA representing the man making a fire 45 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL Figure 7. Example from DD4 using CSCA with subtle use of CA enacting the Pink Panther 46 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL Figure 8. Example from DH3 in observer scale with entity constructions representing a tree on the right hand and the dog on the left 47 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL Figure 9. Example from DH4 in observer scale representing location and motion of man with simultaneous CA as Pink Panther hiding behind tree 48 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL Figure 10. Example from DH2 in observer scale signing LOOK with simultaneous CA as himself as viewer watching man walk across screen 49 SPATIAL SCALE AND ENACTMENT IN BSL Figure 11. Example from DD4 signing MAN then using character scale with an entity construction showing the man moving forward, with no constructed action. 50