Are Competence and Overconfidence related to Psychological Traits? Alexandre Zanetta (UCB) Benjamin Miranda Tabak (UCB) Resumo Este trabalho demonstra que a ilusão de competência e o excesso de confiança estão relacionados com traços psicológicos. Apresentamos novos resultados que ligam traços psicológicos individuais a ocorrência da ilusão de competência e do excesso de confiança. Focamos no grau de impulsividade, traços de personalidade, conhecimento, inteligência, sexo e idade. Ilusão de competência e excesso de confiança estão mais presentes em homens jovens, que são menos impulsivos. Além disso, os indivíduos que são mais abertos à experiência, conscientes e emocionalmente estáveis são mais propensos ao efeito competência. No caso de excesso de confiança, instabilidade emocional mostrou-se dominante. Palavras-Chaves: autoconfinaça excessiva; ilusão de competência; cinco grandes fatores; traços de personalidade; escore de impulsividade. Abstract This paper shows that competence and overconfidence are related to psychological traits. We present novel results that link individual psychological traits to the illusion of competence and overconfidence. We focus on the degree of impulsiveness, personality traits, knowledge, intelligence, gender and age. Illusion of competence and overconfidence are shown to be more present in younger men who are less impulsive. Furthermore, individuals that are more open to experience, conscientious and emotionally stable are more prone to the competence effect. In the case of overconfidence, emotional instability proved to be dominant. Keywords: overconfidence; illusion of competence; five big factors, personality traits; impulsiveness score. JEL Codes: C91; D81. Área 7 - Microeconomia, Métodos Quantitativos e Finanças 1. INTRODUCTION The primary goal of this study is to investigate whether there is some kind of relation between two theories – overconfidence and competence effect, which have been well established in the literature – and psychological traits (Heath and Tversky, 1991; Fox and Tversky, 1995; Gleser and Weber, 2007; Barber and Odean, 2001; Graham et al., 2009; Bruno et al., 2010). Our main hypothesis is that an individual’s psychological traits can be the key to the identification of the standard type of agent who experiences the two types of cognitive errors addressed here. Mapping individual traits that lead to cognitive errors may help elucidate the personal aspects that influence the decision-making process and cause agents to make biased decisions. It is important to understand the behavior of agents because it determines market behavior, which influences individual behavior. So, there exists a link between individual and collective actions. Given this hypothesis, it is of paramount importance to identify the determinants of individual behavior in order to understand how the economy works and how contagion occurs (Lea et al., 1987). In general, individuals prefer to trust their own judgment, which is subjective, rather than bet on a lottery, which has an objective probability, even when the probability of winning is the same for both bets. This behavior is known as the competence effect. An individual’s knowledge of, and competence in, a given topic is a determinant factor for defining his/her behavior towards uncertain events. The more competent in a given topic an individual believes he/she is, the greater the chance that he/she will bet on his/her own judgment. This occurs because agents inflate subjective probabilities, which become greater than an objective probability of the same value (Fox and Tversky, 1995; Heath and Tversky, 1991). Agents overvalue their predictive abilities and believe they can identify future events, even when their predictions are about uncertain events. This cognitive illusion is known as overconfidence (Tversky and Kahneman, 1974; Fischhof et al., 1977; Sustein, 1998). Overconfidence has a direct adverse effect on investors, as they tend to trade more often than do more rational agents. Men show more overconfidence and this reduces investment portfolio profitability because of the increase in trading cost and frequency, affecting more severely those investors who have a larger asset portfolio and those who are better educated (Gleser and Weber, 2007; Barber and Odean, 2001; Graham et al., 2009). There is a group of traits, with their respective intensity, which defines an individual, distinguishing him/her from others. Personality traits include emotional stability, conscientiousness, extraversion, openness to experience and level of socialization or agreeableness (Allport, 1961; McCrae and Costa, 1989; Guilford, 1975; McAdams, 1992). The big five model results are compatible with the application of other models that map personality traits, and is one of the best currently available models for the identification of personality traits (Briggs, 1992; Goldberg, 1992). 2 The use of personality traits in economics is not novel as they have already been utilized to understand and explain economic issues such as the participation of women in certain job markets and income determinants and differences (Rosenbloom et al., 2008; Semykina and Linz, 2007; Palifka, 2009). Maybery et al. (2005) investigate the relation between personality and the heterogeneous behavior of agents. Individuals build two types of reasoning, one called automatic and one known as reflective. Impulsiveness can be defined as a decision that is taken by an individual based on automatic reasoning, without reflection about the issue (Chaiken and Trope, 1999; Doob, 1993). Depending on the situation, automatic reasoning can produce a better response than reflective reasoning, but in some situations, the former is not easily controlled and is brought into action without interference of human wishes and can become uncontrollable in certain cases. Automatic reasoning is inherent to human beings since the beginning of times. For further details, see Lawrence and Stanford (1999) and Gerbin, Ahadi and Patton (1987). Some studies use impulsiveness as a way to try to understand and explain economic issues such as the determinants of investor behavior. Because of that this approach has been widely acknowledged and has therefore gained significant importance in the understanding of consumers’ decision-making process (Hunter and Kemp, 2004; Foxall, 2003; Lai, 2010; Billieux et al., 2008). We used the academic performance of participants as a way to measure their level of intelligence and knowledge. This variable was used as a proxy for knowledge and intelligence because, even though it is not perfect, it is widely available and contemplates important aspects observed in the analyzed characteristic. The impulsiveness and knowledge and intelligence variables are important characteristics that provide good information about how agents have been employing their rational capacity. Less impulsive individuals, who make their decisions more conscientiously and have a better academic performance, can be considered to be more rational. Finally, we also test whether gender and age differences have any influence on illusion of knowledge and overconfidence. 2. EXPERIMENT DESIGN AND PROCEDURES Three questionnaires were sequentially handed over to be answered individually. The first one included questions on personal data, personality traits and impulsiveness of participants. The second one simulated some situations and prompted participants to give their opinions about which result was correct and to what extent they found their choice to be correct. The third questionnaire requested that participants choose between betting on the ticked answer and betting on a lottery with the same winning probability. Details about the questionnaires used are shown in the Appendix. The questions about the identification of personality traits were extracted and adapted from Goldeberg (1992). The questions about impulsiveness were based on the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale. The questionnaire was extracted and adapted from Patton, Stanford and Barratt (1995). 3 To capture the aspects about participants’ individual intelligence, we used the index calculated by the Catholic University of Brasilia known as the Academic Performance Index, which is the result of the arithmetic mean of the grades obtained by students throughout their academic life. And finally, the questionnaire to measure the competence effect and overconfidence was extracted from Heathy and Tversky (1991). First part: data collection and mapping of personality traits and impulsiveness. To capture information on the personality traits of participants, we used a structured questionnaire containing five questions for each of the five traits. The questionnaire on impulsiveness contains a total of 30 questions. Second part: questionnaires on finance. The identification of cognitive failure related to overconfidence was based on a question that requested that the respondent predict his/her performance in the experiment compared to the group as a whole. Each question on finances contained two alternatives (a correct one and an incorrect one), and each participant could check only one. The participant ascribed the probability of getting the correct answer to each question. This probability should fall within the 50-100% interval. Extreme probabilities (50% and 100%) mean “guess at” and “certainty about” the chosen answer, respectively. Values smaller than 50% were not allowed as there were only two possible answers. The assumption that the probability of getting the correct answer is 50% implies that the probability that the unchosen answer is correct will be greater than 50%. Questions without a correct answer were added to this questionnaire. Getting the correct answer relied on the knowledge about the occurrence of future events, which meant a high level of uncertainty. Third part: bet on the lottery versus on his/her own answer. In this part, the participant was given the opportunity to choose, for each answered question, between betting on his/her answer or on a lottery with objective probability equal to the subjective probability attributed by him/her to the question. At the end of the experiment, we randomly chose 10% of the participants, played the game, and changed the result into a monetary prize. The individual prize corresponded to R$ 40.00 (approximately US 23.5), totaling R$ 520.00 in the whole experiment. Since this was a game, the randomly chosen participant could win or not the prize. If he/she lost it, another participant was randomly chosen until all prizes were awarded. 3. RESULTS 3.1. Competence effect For the competence effect to occur, it is expected that the higher the subjective probability attributed individually, the greater the tendency of individuals to bet on their own answer. The hypothesis can be written as: H0: Ha: Correlation > 0, implying presence of competence effect. 4 When we compare quantitative variables, we use Pearson’s correlation coefficient, which, according to Malhotra and Birks (2007), should be used to measure the intensity and direction of the linear association between two variables of this type. For qualitative variables, we adopted Cramer’s V correlation coefficient, which is more appropriate to deal with indicator variables. We calculated the correlation coefficient for all participants and we set those with a positive correlation apart from those with a negative or zero correlation. At a 10% significance level, we found that 53% of participants had a positive correlation. For indicative signs of the presence of the competence effect to exist, it is necessary that the higher the average probability the higher the percentage of choosing one’s own answer. The hypothesis can be written as: H0: Ha: Correlation > 0, implying presence of competence effect. We found a directly proportional correlation of 35.6% (p=0.000) between the average probability and the percentage related to choosing one’s own answer. Thus, the higher the probability of getting the correct answer to a given question, the greater the tendency of an individual to bet on his/her own answer. From this part onwards, we split participants into different groups, categorizing them according to several characteristics. Whenever we split participants into two groups, in order for the competence effect to occur, we expected that the higher the subjective probability individually attributed in a given group, the greater the tendency of individuals to bet on their own answer. In each group, we calculated the correlation coefficient for all participants and we separated those with a positive correlation from those with a negative or zero correlation. First, we categorized participants according to gender to verify whether the competence effect was stronger in the group comprised of men only or in that made up of women only. At a 5% significance level, we could reject that 68% of male participants with a positive correlation was equal to 50% (p=0.038). We did not find significant results for the group comprised of women only. We also measured the mean linear association of both groups to check whether there was some relation between the attributed probability and the choice of one’s own answer. For the group of men, we found a statistically significant positive correlation coefficient of 55.2% (p=0.0001), whereas for the group of women, the correlation coefficient was not significant (p=0.111). This way, men were more vulnerable to the competence effect than were women. We categorized participants according to age to check whether the presence of the competence effect was stronger in the group comprised of younger individuals only or in that made up of older individuals. From this part on, we used the median to split the group into two. In each group, we calculated the correlation coefficient between the subjective probability individually attributed and the tendency of individuals to bet on their own answer. We separated those with a positive correlation from those with a negative or zero correlation. 5 At a 10% significance level, we found that 53% of younger participants had a positive correlation, whereas older ones had a positive correlation in only 49% of the cases. For the younger group, we obtained a statistically significant correlation coefficient of 43.7% (p=0.006). For the older group, the correlation coefficient was not significant (p=0.252). Younger individuals were more vulnerable to the competence effect than older ones. In this part, we categorized participants according to their academic performance to verify whether the competence effect was stronger among those with a better performance or in the group comprised only of individuals with a poorer performance. At a 10% significance level, we found that 58% of participants with a better performance had a positive correlation and that 60% of those with a poorer performance had a negative correlation. For those with a better performance, we obtained a statistically significant positive correlation coefficient of 34.7% (p=0.030). For those with a poorer performance, we did not find a significant correlation coefficient (p=0.147). Individuals with a better performance were more vulnerable to the competence effect than those with a poorer performance. Again, we split participants according to their impulsiveness score to determine whether the presence of the competence effect was stronger in the group with more impulsive individuals or in that made up only of less impulsive individuals. At a 10% significance level, 54% of less impulsive participants and only 47% of more impulsive ones had a positive correlation. For the group with a higher score, we obtained a statistically significant correlation coefficient of 22.5% (p=0.100). For the group with a lower score, we found a more significant positive correlation coefficient of 30.7% (p=0.036). Individuals with a lower impulsiveness score were more vulnerable to the competence effect than those with a higher score. We also assessed academic performance and the impulsiveness score and we used this association as strong characteristics that are observed in more rational individuals. We separated individuals with a lower impulsiveness score and with a higher academic performance from the others. This group showed greater positive intensity regarding those aspects required for individuals to base their decisions on rationality: higher level of knowledge and larger use of their reflective capacity, making more conscientious choices. At a 10% significance level, we found that 53% of participants with more rational traits had a positive correlation. At this same significance level, we found that 64% of participants with less rational characteristics had a negative correlation. For the group with more rational characteristics, the positive correlation coefficient corresponded to 32.3% (p=0.103). In the group with less rational individuals, the correlation coefficient was not significant either (p=0.212). The results obtained were insufficient to determine that individuals with more rational characteristics are more vulnerable to the competence effect than others. We categorized participants according to the intensity displayed by each personality trait to verify whether the competence effect was stronger in the group comprised only of those with greater intensity or in that made up of those with a smaller intensity . 6 At a 10% significance level, we found that 53% of participants who were more open to experience had a positive correlation. At this same significance level, we observed that only 49% of participants who were more closed to experience had a positive correlation. For the group of those more open to experience, there was a statistically significant positive correlation coefficient of 41.9% (p=0.008). For those more closed to experience, the correlation coefficient was not significant (p=0.200). Individuals more open to experience were more vulnerable to the competence effect than those who were less open to it. We separated participants according to the conscientiousness trait to verify whether the competence effect was stronger than in the group made up only of more conscientious individuals or in that made up only of less conscientious individuals. At a 10% significance level, we found that 53% and 56% of more conscientious and less conscientious participants, respectively, had a positive correlation. For the group of more conscientious individuals, we obtained a statistically significant positive correlation coefficient of 46.2% (p=0.002). For the group of less conscientious individuals, the correlation coefficient could not be considered to be different from zero (p=0.378). More conscientious individuals were more vulnerable to the competence effect than less conscientious ones. For the emotional stability trait, we verified whether the presence of the competence effect was stronger in the group made up only of more emotionally stable individuals or in that comprised only of less emotionally stable ones. At a 10% significance level, we found that only 47% of more emotionally stable participants had a positive correlation whereas 54% of less emotionally stable participants showed the same type of correlation. For the group of more emotionally stable individuals, we obtained a statistically significant positive correlation coefficient of 51.4% (p=0.001). For the group of less emotionally stable individuals, the results were not significant (p=0.471). More emotionally stable participants were more vulnerable to the competence effect than those who were less emotionally stable. There was no statistical significance for any other personality trait. In what follows, we check the intensity at which this deviation affects both groups. First, we verified whether the average subjective probability can be considered to be different across the groups. After that, we checked whether the percentage related to the choice of one’s own answer is different across these groups. We used Student’s t test in all cases described in the present study in order to compare two population means from two samples. This test is used to check whether the means of two populations are significantly different from each other. We did not consider the normality of the sample because according to Myers and Well (2003) the parametric Student’s t test is robust to type I error, even when the distributions are flat or biased. 7 Men attributed a mean probability of correct answers of 76.02% compared to 68.79% by women (p=0.003). Therefore, we can say that men tend to attribute more subjective probability to getting their answers correct than do women. In 64.83% of the cases, men preferred to bet on their own choices whereas women had such preference in only 54.06% of the cases (p=0.030). This way, it is possible to say that men tend to prefer to bet on their own choice than do women. The group made up of more sociable participants attributed an average probability of 67.87% to getting the correct answer compared to 72.98% by the least sociable ones. The percentage values can be considered statistically different from each other (p=0.074). In 64.29% of the cases, the most sociable individuals preferred to bet on their own choices whereas the least sociable had such preference in 53.98% of the cases (p=0.067). Thus, we can state that the most sociable ones prefer to bet on their own answers than do the least sociable ones. For the remaining groups, the differences were not statistically significant. In what follows, we show a table that summarizes the significant results obtained for the competence effect. 8 Table 1 – Summary of competence effect results Whole group Academic Performance Age Openness to Experience Conscientiousness Emotional Stability Corr(i)(SP,OA)>0 in 53% of cases (p=0.100) Corr(T)(SP,OA)=37% (p=0.000) Group (+) Group (-) Corr(i)(SP,OA)>0 in 58% of cases (p=0.100) Corr(T)(SP,OA)=35% (p=0.030) Corr(i)(SP,OA)>0 in 53% of cases (p=0.100) Corr(T)(SP,OA)=44% (p=0.006) Corr(i)(SP,OA)>0 in 53% of cases (p=0.100) Corr(T)(SP,OA)=42% (p=0.008) Corr(i)(SP,OA)>0 in 53% of cases (p=0.100) Corr(T)(SP,OA)=46% (p=0.002) Corr(i)(SP,OA)>0 in 47% of cases (p=0.100) Corr(T)(SP,OA)=51% (p=0.001) Corr(i)(SP,OA)>0 in 40% of cases (p=0.100) Corr(T)(SP,OA)=20% (p=0.147) Corr(i)(SP,OA)>0 in 49% of cases (p=0.100) Corr(T)(SP,OA)=11% (p=0.252) Corr(i)(SP,OA)>0 in 49% of cases (p=0.100) Corr(T)(SP,OA)=14% (p=0.200) Corr(i)(SP,OA)>0 in 56% of cases (p=0.100) Corr(T)(SP,OA)=6% (p=0.378) Corr(i)(SP,OA)>0 in 54% of cases (p=0.100) Corr(T)(SP,OA)=-1% (p=0.471) SP = 67.87% SP = 72.98% (p=0.074) Level of Socialization OA = 64.29% OA = 53.98% (p=0.067) Gender Male Female Corr(i)(SP,OA)>0 in 68% of cases (p=0.100) Corr(T)(SP,OA)=55% (p=0.000) Corr(i)(SP,OA)>0 in 41% of cases (p=0.100) Corr(T)(SP,OA)=18% (p=0.111) SP = 76.02% SP = 68.79% (p=0.003) OA = 64.83% OA = 54.06% (p=0.033) Where: SP = Subjective probability (probability of correct answers attributed by the participant) OA = Bet on one’s own answer (percentage of questions in which the participant preferred to bet on his/her own answers) Corr(i) = Individual correlation Corr(T) = Total correlation 9 3.2. Overconfidence To estimate self-confidence, we built an index that shows the difference between the probability ascribed to correct answers subtracted from the percentage of actual correct answers. For the overconfidence effect to exist, we expect the mean self-confidence index to be greater than zero. The larger the value obtained for the self-confidence index, the larger the presence of overconfidence. We can write the hypothesis as: H0: Mean self-confidence index Ha: Mean self-confidence index > 0, implying presence of overconfidence. By using a 5% significance level, we cannot reject that the mean value of 4.25% for the self-confidence index shown by participants is smaller than or equal to zero. When the data were submitted to normality tests, they passed the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test with Lilliefors significance correction (K-S:p=0.200) and the Shapiro-Wilk test (SW:p=0.455). Therefore, we found evidence of the overconfidence effect in the made up of all participants in this experiment. We compared the self-confidence index with the stated self-confidence reported by participants in order to check whether there was some kind of association between the declared and observed indices. In order for an association to be found between the two, those individuals who believed they would outperform their peers are expected to have a higher self-confidence index. We can write the hypothesis as: H0: Correlation = 0, implying absence of association between the stated and observed self-confidence. Ha: Correlation observed self-confidence. The presence of directly proportional correlation between the self-confidence index and the stated self-confidence was equal to 21.4% (p=0.048). In this case, those who considered themselves to be self-confident had a greater probability to show cognitive error. We categorized participants according to gender to verify whether overconfidence was stronger in the group made up only of men or in that comprised only of women. For the group of men, at a 5% significance level, we cannot reject that a mean value equal to 7.95% shown by participants is smaller than or equal to zero (K-S: p=0.200; S-W: p=0.912). For the group of women, even at a 10% significance level, we cannot reject that the mean value of the self-confidence index is different from zero. By comparing the values obtained for the two groups, we can reject that the mean self-confidence indices between men and women are equal to each other (p=0.024). Thus, the group comprised only of men had a positive and higher overconfidence index than that of the group of women. We stratified participants by age to verify whether overconfidence was stronger in the group of younger individuals or in that made up only of older individuals. 10 For the group of younger participants, at a 5% significance level, we cannot reject that the mean value of 5.33% for the self-confidence index shown by participants is smaller than or equal to zero (K-S: p=0.200; S-W: p=0.246). For the group of older individuals, even at a 10% significance level, we cannot reject that the mean value of 1.61% for the self-confidence index shown by participants is equal to zero (K-S: p=0.200; S-W: p=0.774). We found evidence of the overconfidence effect only in the group constituted of younger participants. The values presented by the two groups can be considered statistically different from each other (p=0.031). The group made up of younger individuals had a positive and higher self-confidence index than that of the group of older individuals. We obtained a negative correlation coefficient of 20.6% between age and selfconfidence, which is statistically different from zero (p=0.089). The older the participant, the smaller the self-confidence index. Taking academic performance into consideration, we checked whether the presence of overconfidence was stronger in the group made up only of individuals with a better academic performance or in that which included only those individuals with a poorer performance. We found a positive correlation coefficient of 28.6% between academic performance and the self-confidence index, which was statistically different from zero (p=0.028). The better the academic performance, the higher the self-confidence index. By using a 5% significance level for the group with better academic performance, we could not reject that the mean value of 9.55% for the self-confidence index is smaller than or equal to zero (K-S: p=0.200; S-W: p=0.455). For the group with poorer academic performance, even at a 10% significance level, we could not reject that the mean value of -2.49% for the self-confidence index was equal to zero (K-S: p=0.200; S-W: p=0.977). We observed indicative signs of the overconfidence effect only in the group comprised of individuals with a better academic performance. The values obtained for both groups can be regarded as statistically different from each other (p=0.001). The group of individuals with better academic performance had a positive overconfidence index that is larger than that of the group with a poorer performance. We categorized participants according to the impulsiveness score to verify whether overconfidence was stronger in the group of more impulsive individuals or in that of less impulsive participants. No significant correlation coefficient was found between the impulsiveness score and the self-confidence index (p=0.668). For less impulsive individuals, even at a 10% significance level, we cannot reject that the mean value of 3.73% for the self-confidence index is smaller than or equal to zero (K-S: p=0.200; S-W: p=0.522). For more impulsive individuals, even at a 10% significance level, we cannot reject that the mean value of 2.93% for the self-confidence index is equal to zero (K-S: p=0.200; S-W: p=0.335). Therefore, both more and less impulsive individuals did not show the overconfidence effect. 11 Afterwards, we categorized participants according to rational characteristics to check whether overconfidence was stronger in the group of individuals with a better academic performance associated with a low impulsiveness score or in the group made up of those with a poorer performance associated with a higher level of impulsiveness. We found a positive correlation coefficient of 24.2% between rational characteristics and the self-confidence index that was statistically different from zero (p=0.045). The more rational the participant, the higher the self-confidence index. For those with more rational characteristics, at a 5% significance level, we could reject that the mean value of 12.32% for the self-confidence index was equal to zero (K-S: p=0.200; S-W: p=0.093). As to the group of less rational individuals, even at a 10% significance level, we cannot reject that the mean value of 2.19% for the self-confidence index is equal to zero (K-S: p=0.200; S-W: p=0.126). We found evidence of the overconfidence effect only in the group of more rational individuals. The values obtained for both groups can be considered statistically different from each other (p=0.020). The group of more rational individuals had a positive overconfidence index that was greater than that of the group of less rational individuals. The participants were categorized according to the “openness to experience” trait to verify whether overconfidence was stronger in the group of individuals that were more open to experience or in that in which they were closed to experience. We obtained a correlation coefficient of 24% between the “openness to experience” trait and the self-confidence index, which was statistically different from zero (p=0.047). We calculated the mean self-confidence index for both groups and compared their results. By adopting a 5% significance level for the group of individuals who were more open to experience, we could reject that the mean value of 5.77% for the selfconfidence index was equal to zero (K-S: p=0.188; S-W: p=0.018). For the group of individuals who were more closed to experience, even by adopting a 10% significance level, we could not reject that the mean value of 1.23% for the selfconfidence index is equal to zero (K-S: p=0.200; S-W: p=0.873). We found evidence of the overconfidence effect only in the group of individuals who were more open to experience. The values obtained for both groups cannot be regarded as statistically different (p=0.176). The “openness to experience” trait was not a good criterion to determine the presence of the overconfidence effect. We categorized the participants according to the “conscientiousness” trait to check whether overconfidence was stronger in the group of more conscientious individuals than in the group of less conscientious ones. The “conscientiousness” trait was not strong enough to determine the presence of the overconfidence effect. To analyze the “emotional stability” trait, we categorized the participants in order to verify whether overconfidence was stronger in the group of more emotionally stable individuals or in the group of less emotionally stable ones. We observed no evidence of the overconfidence effect in the group of more emotionally stable individuals. 12 We found evidence of the overconfidence effect only in the group of less emotionally stable individuals. The value obtained for both groups could be regarded as statistically different from each other (p=0.024). The group of less emotionally stable individuals had a positive overconfidence index that was greater than that of more emotionally stable individuals. We categorized the participants according to the “extraversion” trait to check whether overconfidence was stronger in the group of more extroverted individuals or in the group of more introverted ones. We obtained a correlation coefficient of 25.0% between the “extraversion” trait and the self-confidence index, which was statistically different from zero (p=0.038). We calculated the mean self-confidence index for both groups and compared their results. By adopting a 10% significance level for the group of extroverted individuals, we could reject that the mean value of 4.55% for the self-confidence index is equal to zero (K-S: p=0.200; S-W: p=0.128). For the group of introverted individuals, even after adopting a 10% significance level, we could not reject that the mean value of 2.35% of the selfconfidence index is equal to zero (K-S: p=0.200; S-W: p=0.983). We found evidence of the overconfidence effect only in the group of extroverted individuals. The values obtained for both groups cannot be considered to be statistically different from each other (p=0.515). The “extraversion” trait does not seem to be an important criterion for determining the presence of the overconfidence effect. Finally, we categorized the participants according to the level of socialization in order to verify whether overconfidence was stronger in the group of more sociable individuals than in the group of less sociable ones. No correlation coefficient was found between the level of socialization and the self-confidence index (p=0.930). There was evidence of the overconfidence effect only in the group of less sociable individuals. The values obtained for both groups cannot be considered to be statistically different (p=0.414). The level of socialization trait was not strong enough to determine the presence of the overconfidence effect. Table 2 summarizes the significant results observed for the overconfidence effect. 13 Table 2 – Summary of overconfidence effect results Description Whole group Academic Performance (AP) Age (A) Openness to Experience (OE) Extraversion (E) Academic Performance and Impulsiveness (APxI) Gender (G) Self-Confidence Index (SCI) SCI = 4.25% (p=0.005) Corr(SCI,SO)=21.4% (p=0.048) Group (+) Group (-) SCI = 9.55% SCI = 0% (p=0.050) (p=0.100) Different means (p=0.001) Corr(SCI,AP)=28.6% (p=0,028) SCI = 0% SCI = 5.33% (p=0.100) (p=0.050) Different means (p=0.031) Corr(SCI,A)=-20.6% (p=0.089) SCI = 5.77% SCI = 0% (p=0.050) (p=0.100) Different means (p=0.176) Corr(SCI,OE)=24.0% (p=0.047) SCI = 4.55% SCI = 0% (p=0.100) (p=0.100) Equal means (p=0.515) Corr(SCI,E)=25.0% (p=0.038) AP (+) e I(-) AP (-) e I(+) SCI = 12.32% SCI = 0% (p=0.050) (p=0.100) Different means (p=0.020) Corr(SCI,APxI)=24.2% (p=0.045) Male Female SCI = 7.95% SCI = 0% (p=0.050) (p=0.100) Different means (p=0.024) Where: SO = Stated Overconfidence SCI = Mean probability of getting a correct answer – Percentage of questions answered correctly (Self-Confidence Index) 4. FINAL REMARKS We found evidence that people with important rational characteristics also have illusions of overconfidence and competence, at least in the same intensity as those with less rational characteristics. Male and younger individuals are more vulnerable to the influence of these two types of cognitive biases. The information gathered by our research group helped us outline a profile of individuals who are influenced by these two types of biases. Even though the study of the characteristics at issue in our paper is far from exhaustive, these characteristics are strong enough to determine the presence of biases and to identify personal aspects that may help distinguish individuals in a larger group. 14 For the competence effect, we found that young men are affected more often, showing low impulsiveness and personal characteristics such as adventurous, focused and resilient. More sensible individuals preferred to bet on their own answers. However, they ascribed a lower probability to getting the correct answers than did challenging individuals. In terms of overconfidence, younger men are more prone to make such a mistake, showing characteristics that are inherent to rationality, which was addressed herein because of the low impulsiveness score, and also showing more emotional reactions. The reactive characteristic of the emotional stability trait indicates people who easily experience negative emotions, such as anger, depression, stress, and frustrations. The highest self-confidence index was observed in individuals who show important rational characteristics. These individuals had approximately three times as high an index than the mean value found for all participants. 5. REFERENCES Allport, G.W. (1961) Pattern and growth in personality. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston. Barber, B.M.; Odean, T. (2001). Boys Will be Boys: Gender, Overconfidence, and Common Stock Investment. Quarterly Journal of Economics. 116:261-292 Billieux, J.; Rochat, L.; Rebetez, M.M.L.; Van Der Linden, M. (2008) Are all facets of impulsivity related to self-reported compulsive buying behavior? Personality and Individual Differences. 44(6): 1432-1442. Briggs, S. R. (1992) Assessing the Five-Factor Model of personality description. Journal of Personality. 60: 253-293. Bruno, M.; Matsushita, R.; Da Silva, S. (2010) Risk Seeking Behavior of Preschool Children in a Gambling Task. Journal of Economic Psychology. 32:In press. Chaiken, S.; Trope, Y. (1999) Dual Process Theories in Social Psychology. Nova York: Guilford. Doob, L. (1993) Hesitation: impulsivity and reflection. In McCown, W.; Johnson, J.L.; Shure M.B. The Impulsive Client – Theory, Research and Treatment. Washington: American Psychological Association. Fischhof, B. et al. (1977) Knowing with uncertainty: The appropriateness of extreme confidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 3: 522-564. Fox, C.; Tversky, A. (1995) Ambiguity Aversion and Comparative Ignorance. Quarterly Journal of Economics. 110: 585-603. Foxall, G.R. (2003) The behavior analysis of consumer choice: An introduction to the special issue. Journal of Economic Psychology. 24(5) 581-588. Gerbing, D.W., Ahadi, S.A. Patton, J.H. (1987) Toward a conceptualization of impulsivity: a comparison across the behavioral and self-report domains. Multivariate Behavioral Research. 22:357-379. Gleser, M.; Weber, M. (2007) Overconfidence and trading volume. The GENEVA Risk and Insurance Review. 32:1-36. 15 Goldberg, L. R. (1992) The development of markers for the Big-Five factor structure. Psychological Assessment, 4: 26-42. Graham, J.R.; Harvey, C.R.; Huang, H. (2009) Investor competence, trading frequency. Management Science. 55(7): 1094-1106. Guilford, J.P. (1975) Factors and factors of personality. Psychological Bulletin, 82: 802-814. Hunter, K.; Kemp S.(2004) The personality of e-commerce investors. Journal of Economic Psychology. 25(4): 529-537. Heath, C.; Tversky, A. (1991) Preference and Belief: Ambiguity and competence in choice under uncertainty. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty. 4: 5-28. Lai, C.W. (2010) How financial attitudes and practices influence the impulsive buying behavior of college and university students. Social Behavior and Personality: an international journal. 38(3):373-380. Lawrence, J.B.; Stanford, M.S. (1999) Impulsivity and time of day: effects on performance and cognitive tempo. Personality and Individual Differences. 26:199207. Lea, S. E.G.; Tarpy, R. M.; Webley, P. (1987) The individual in the economy. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Malhotra, N.K.; Birks, D.F.(2007) Marketing Research: An Applied Approach. 3rd Ed. Prentice-Hall, Harlow. Maybery, D.; Crase, L.; Gullifer, C.(2005). Categorising farming values as economic, conservation and lifestyle. Journal of Economic Psychology. 26(1): 59-72 McAdams, D.P. (1992) The Five-factor Model in personality: A critical appraisal. Journal of Personality. 60: 329-361. McCrae, R.R.; Costa, P.T. (1989) More reasons to adopt the Five-Factor Model. American Psychologist. 44: 451-452. Myers e Well (2003) Palifka, B.J. (2009) Personality and income in Mexico: Supervisor assessments vs. selfassessments. Journal of Economic Psychology. 30(1):92-106. Patton, J.H.; Stanford, M.S, Barratt, E.S. (1995) Factor structure of Barratt impulsiveness scale. J Clin Psychol. 51:768–74. Rosenbloom, J.L.; Ash, R.A., Dupont, B.; Coder, L. (2008) Why are there so few women in information technology? Assessing the role of personality in career choices. Journal of Economic Psychology. 29(4): 543-554. Semykina, A.; Linz, S.J. (2007) Gender differences in personality and earnings: Evidence from Russia. Journal of Economic Psychology. 28(3): 387-410. Sunstein, C.R. (1998), "Selective Fatalism". Journal of Legal Studies. 27: 799-823. Tversky, A.; Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science. 185: 1124-1131. 16