Reading in the Secondary School

advertisement
Reading in the Secondary School
The achievement gap in secondary school
At primary and secondary school levels, international comparisons show an achievement gap
between the highest and lowest performing students, in reading, maths and science, that is
equivalent to between 2.5 and 4.5 years of school attendance (Organisation for Economic
Cooperation and Development, 1998). In New Zealand, this gap is even wider for reading
than it is for Maths and Science. In fact, New Zealand has the widest range of reading
achievement of all OECD countries that have been surveyed. This must make it very difficult
for teachers to find a "happy medium" in terms of what level to teach at.
Should students be streamed?
A possible solution to the wide range of ability is "streaming", yet this is not favoured in
many secondary schools. Instead, most classes are mixed in ability. One study found that a
problem with streaming was that once students were assigned to this level or that, there was
very little mobility across the ability groups, especially mobility upwards (Cash, 1991).
Another study found that ability grouping in English, Science and Reading classes in 7th grade
(12-year-olds) did not lead to instructional adaptations. Above average learners were more
likely to think that their courses were easy or moving at a comfortable pace than were belowaverage learners (Mecoli, 1994). Another study of mathematics classes found that streaming
(ability grouping) led to a loss of mathematical talent in that some students were tracked into
lower classes when they had potential to succeed in higher classes (Harris, 1997).
A complicating factor in the overall pattern of reading achievement in New Zealand is that
reading failure is not evenly distributed among schools. In one survey of high schools in
Auckland, students from six schools located in low socio-economic areas were assessed for
reading comprehension skills. The results showed that 90% of incoming students (13-yearolds) achieved scores that were below the national average for their age (Nicholson &
Gallienne, 1995). These were not "reluctant" readers. These were students who had difficulty
with reading.
Is reading a new problem for high schools?
In New Zealand, secondary schools used to be for the elite. Only 50 percent of children
attended school, at least until the Education Act of 1877, which made primary school
compulsory (McLaren, 1974). And school was only for a few months each year. Christ
College and Auckland Grammar were modelled on English public schools like Eton and
Harrow. Students studied Latin and the Classics. No Science was taught in those early days. It
was not taken seriously. Only English, Maths, Latin and Greek were taught. The leaving age
was not raised to 15 until 1945. Only 1 in 30 students at that time passed Matriculation, a prerequisite for University. My ex-Dean of Education, now retired, remembered that of the 35
students who started high school with him in Form 3 (Year 9), he was the only one left in
Form 6 (Year 12). The history of high schools is worth remembering. For a long time, the top
end of high school was elitist and exclusive. Even in 1970, the number of students in New
Zealand high schools at 17 years of age was small, about 13 percent (Thorndike, 1973).
In the 1990s, the situation is much different. In countries like the United States and New
Zealand, schools are open for nine months of the year, not three months. In New Zealand,
schooling is compulsory up until the age of 16, by which time most students will have sat
School Certificate, their first compulsory national examination. In the United States, 75
percent of 17-year-old youth are still at school; in New Zealand 64 percent are still at school.
In the United States, 60 percent of 17-year-olds go on to tertiary study. In New Zealand, 20
percent of 17-year-olds go to University, 20 percent to other tertiary study. That’s 40 percent
in tertiary study compared to 60 percent in the United States (Calfee & Patrick, 1995;
Ministry of Education, 1998).
What factors cause students to stay at high school?
Many New Zealand youth do not move forward to tertiary education, even though entry to
University is very open. Why is this? And why are fewer youth still at school in New Zealand
compared to the United States? There will be many factors that influence why a student will
decide to stay at school or move on. For example, it may be the size of the school, in that a
large school may seem too impersonal. Smaller schools have fewer dropouts (Barker &
Gump, 1964). It may be class size, in that students respond better to teaching if they are in
small classes (Finn & Achilles, 1990). Teachers are less stressed if they teach in small classes,
and are less likely to assign "busy work", which may make school a lot more interesting for
students (French, 1993).
Another possible explanation for the fewer numbers of our students going on to tertiary study
is to do with teachers. Students achieve more with "authoritative" teachers who have high
goals and clear expectations of them (Rutter, 1983). Students will stay longer in schools, and
achieve more, if the principal and staff have a strong belief that all their students will succeed.
Finally, there may be factors outside the school that influence retention rates in schools. An
obvious factor is family income. It is expensive to stay on at school, and to go to University.
There are many possible explanations for the success of some schools in holding students
through the upper levels. Are the basic skills of reading and writing also part of the
explanation for successful schools? Perhaps. Schools with a high success rate in national
examinations have students who are above-average in reading. In contrast, failing schools
have many below-average readers. Even successful schools will have some students who do
not do well in reading and writing. On average, 20% of students entering high school will be
reading below what we would expect students to be able to read at that age. About half of
these students (10%) will be three years below average for their age in reading (Flockton &
Crooks, 1997).
Do high school students like reading?
Here are some responses of Year 9 and 10 students in one high school to a questionnaire
survey of feelings about reading. It was clear from the survey that many students did like
reading, but some did not. Here is a selection of responses to the question, "Is reading
important for you?"
"Yes, I don’t know, but I just love to read."
"Yes, ‘cos if I couldn’t read I’d die of boredom."
"Yes it is , because I love reading. Reading is very important to me because it improves my
reading skills."
"Yes. It makes me think. It is an escape from reality."
"Yes, exercises my imagination, fun."
"Yes it’s a vital skill, very important. The more you read, the more you know."
"Yes, you can’t spell if you don’t read."
"Yes, because if you can’t read you can’t order at McDonalds or read anything else."
"If you don’t reading you can’t get you’re driving licence."
"Reading doesn’t really appeal to me as fun it’s very boring reading a big chapter book."
"No not really because I have much better things to do than read a book and while I have my
head in a book I might miss something."
"No, because I hate reading."
"No because it’s all sh**"
"No, because reading sucks."
Is low motivation a cause of poor reading?
The causes of poor academic achievement can be difficult to identify. Students who are
struggling with reading are likely to be un-motivated. Yet this lack of interest in reading may
be a consequence of some other factor, and not the cause. Let’s look at an example of this
cause-effect problem. A study of the effects of part-time work on students’ school
achievement (Bachman & Schulenberg, 1993) found that high school students who worked
more than 5 hours a week were more likely to smoke cigarettes, drink alcohol, use illegal
drugs, argue with parents, get insufficient sleep, and get lack of exercise. Their academic
achievement was also poor. At first, they thought all these things had been due to the parttime work itself, yet they eventually concluded that the part-time work was a result of poor
achievement in school, not a cause. They found that students who were doing poorly at school
sought for an escape from school. This led them into more and more part-time work. The jobs
they did also tended to have low value in terms of later careers. The students were doing
mundane jobs like cleaning offices, waiting on tables, and filling grocery bags at
supermarkets. They concluded that these students would be better off if they were given extra
instruction to lift their school achievement. Parents of these students needed to help by
limiting opportunities to avoid schoolwork. They recommended that students who were
"vulnerable to the seductions of quick earnings and premature affluence" be given help by
attacking their basic scholastic needs, providing a positive school climate of high expectations
and ensuring that they received a thorough grounding in "basic skills".
Who should teach reading?
In the Unites States, the reality for many years has been that high school subject teachers have
resisted teaching "reading" (Vacca, 1998). It is probably fair to say that this has also been the
case in New Zealand. Subject teachers are trained to teach Music or English, or History or
Social Studies, or Mathematics or Science, but not reading. Yet reading is the hidden
curriculum of high school. Reading is not only done in English. In Mathematics, for example,
there seems to be very little reading. Yet there is a strong correlation between ability to read
and mathematics achievement (Tseng, 1998). For example, Asian students achieve very well
in Mathematics, especially if they attend schools in middle class areas. Yet Asian students
would achieve even better if they were better at reading their Mathematics problems. The
correlation between reading and Mathematics is strong even for English-speaking students
(Lim, 1998). This is because there is much more reading in mathematics than is commonly
thought, especially the problem solving sections of the mathematics curriculum.
How well do students manage their textbooks?
Nicholson (1984, 1988) reported the results of extensive classroom interviews in high school
classrooms. Two reading teachers were recruited from local high schools. One teacher was
given the job of following a Year 9 class (13-year-olds) in her high school, while the other
was given the job of following a year 10 class (14-year-olds) in another school. The two
reading teachers joined each class while their regular teachers instructed them for English,
Mathematics, Science and Social Studies lessons.
The two research teachers were given audio-tape recorders, to record their conversations with
students. This was a long study. They were in the two high schools for nearly 6 months. They
attended 300 lessons, and gathered 60 hours of audio-taped conversations with pupils. During
each lesson, the research teacher would move from desk to desk, asking students about what
they were reading, and how they were attacking the reading tasks.
Let’s look at some of the results.

Vocabulary
First, there were terminology problems. Here are some examples from Mathematics:


Solve 2x = 8
Solve 2x + 6 =
What do you think are possible problems with this terminology? Well, some students weren’t
sure about mathematical symbols such as the "x" in equations. ?". One student asked, "What’s
that cross thing?" Another asked, "What’s two times plus. In mathematics, there are many
potential terminology problems. Words like "range" and "estimate" have special meanings.
Students confuse the specialist meanings of these words with their everyday common
meanings. Terminology can be a problem in other subjects as well. In social studies, words
like "consumer" and market" have special meanings; in Science, the word "bend" means
bounce. In English, words like "syllable", "adverb", and so on all have specialist meanings.
Perhaps teachers do not reinforce these meanings well enough. Students need to be quizzed
on new meanings, and learn to spell new terms correctly. The teacher can break them down,
talk about their origins, and so on. For example, in Science, discussing the water cycle
involves lots of Greek and Latin terms. Even the word "cycle" comes from the Greek word
"kuklos" meaning circle. Reading a diagram of the water cycle involves the student in reading
words like evaporation, transpiration, condensation, and precipitation.
To glue these words into students’ minds, it is important to show the structure of words. Many
content words consist of chunks of meaning. For example, the "spir" in transpiration is from
the Latin "to breathe". The chunk "spir" is the core of other words like respiration and
inspiration. The word inspire, for example, means "breathe into" the mind. In Science,
transpiration means "breathe across", which refers to the way water passes from leaves into
the air as vapour. The word evaporation means "passing off vapour". The word condensation
means "making more dense".
To give an example, here is part of an interview with one student about a water cycle diagram
given in Science class. The class were copying down a diagram of the water cycle. The
diagram shoed how water fell from the clouds, seeping through the ground to the sea, and
then being evaporated upwards again. Key words like "condensation" were used to label parts
of the diagram.:
Researcher: What bit don’t you understand?
Student: Where the rain goes down to the trees. Where does it go then?
Researcher: Is there nothing on the diagram that will show you?
Student: The lake, It must go down into the lake.
Researcher: All right. If it goes down into the lake, what happens then?
Student: Well then, it must overflow and go down into the sea.
Researcher: What is it that’s got you puzzled?
Student: The bit where it says, ‘evaporation from ocean’s surface.’
Researcher: How is this puzzling you?
Student: ‘Cause I don’t know where it goes.
The interview continued, with the student not sure where the vapour went. By the time he got
to the end of the water cycle diagram, he was stuck again:
Researcher: What are you stuck on this time?
Student: The cloud bit. What does that word say?
Researcher: Condensation.
Student: What does that mean?
The student probably needed do a lot of "word work" to get the water cycle well understood.
The teacher (with help from an etymological dictionary) can break these words into chunks.
Do students know that cycle means circle? Do they know what precipitation means? It comes
from the Latin, "praeceps" which means to throw head-long. It’s related to another word
"precipice", which students may have heard of.
In another Science class interview, the student explained that she copied down the "hard bits"
from her textbook word-for-word:
Researcher: Can you show me what you copied?
Student: ‘A heavier element, helium, is formed and, in the process some matter is converted
into energy.’
Researcher: What makes that hard for you?
Student: I dunno what helium is, more or less.
I suspect that the student may also not have understood the words matter, energy, or
converted. In just that one sentence copied from her textbook there were a lot of words to be
understood correctly.
Text structure
Even if vocabulary are understood, another problem for students is understanding at the
macro level. In the Nicholson (1988) study, students often commented that they had difficulty
understanding the overall content of what they read. They copied large chunks of text when
writing essays or summaries, even though they did not understand what they were copying.
One student commented about what she called "long" stories. She said that it was hard to
answer questions about a long piece of text because she would forget what was in it. She said,
"Don’t know how to answer the questions properly ‘cause you can’t remember. You read it
through and then you start to forget different parts of the story that was at the front of it. And
then you go and answer the questions and can’t even answer them properly ‘cause you can’t
remember it."
Students often can’t "see" the structure of what they are reading (Calfee & Patrick, 1995). It’s
useful to teach them to do this. Even a 1-page handout can have a structure that is hard to
"see". I think it’s useful to teach students how to visualise the design of text material, and use
this design to reconstruct the details of what they are reading. They should be able to see the
design of a text, just as an architect can visualise the design of a house. Let’s take an example.
A Year 9 student reads a novel called "Dracula". Yet when it comes to examination time,
many of the details are gone from memory. It would have been very useful if the student had
some notes which showed the structure of the text.
Students can be taught that that there are two kinds of text, narrative and expository. Or
fiction and non-fiction. Narrative texts (novels, short stories, plays) are stories. They have a
basic structure of a setting, characters, plot, and a theme. In the case of the Dracula story, the
theme is that good wins out over evil. Expository texts are not stories. They tell you things.
They have a variety of structures. For example, information may follow a sequence;
information may be about a specific topic; information about one thing may be contrasted
with another thing. And so on.
There are similarities between narratives and expository texts. For example, narratives have a
plot which follows a certain sequence. Expository text, such as a description of the water
cycle, or the life span of a butterfly, can also follow a sequence pattern. But it is better to treat
the two kinds of texts differently. For example, an article about the life cycle of a monarch
butterfly has a sequence, but it is not like the plot of a narrative story, and there are no
specific "characters". This is why it is "cleaner" to teach students that the two types of text
have similarities but are basically very different ways of writing.
Narrative text. Calfee and Patrick (1995) describe a number of ways in which narratives can
be analysed. A simple way to describe a narrative is to say that there are characters and a plot.
Characters in a novel, short story, poem, or play can be analysed by using a web (one
character) or weave structure (comparison of several characters.) A web is like a spider web.
A character web has categories which show different facets of the character (e.g., what he or
she looks like, personality, and so on.) A character weave is like a grid, where two or more
characters can be compared and contrasted. Weave diagrams are sometimes referred to as
"matrixes".
Diagrams are useful for showing the plot. A story graph can show levels of actions (high or
low) on the vertical axis, while the sequence of what happened can be written along the
horizontal axis. This can be drawn on a large sheet of paper. Events in many narratives build
slowly toward a high point. For example, in Dracula, the high point is when he is killed. After
that, the level of action sinks, while the story is concluded. Students can chart the key events,
in order, and think about their relative importance in the flow of the story. Even a simple
listing of the events in the plot can be useful. A 25 page story can be broken down to 15 or so
points. It makes the narrative content easier to remember. During discussion, pupils can be
asked to justify their decisions about high and low points in the action. They can also refer
back to the text for support when making decisions about the order of key events.
Expository text. Calfee and Patrick (1995) describe several different expository text
structures. Expository texts are not stories. There are various kinds of expository structural
designs. The three main types are sequence, description and argument. We have already
talked about sequence type structure, where information is presented in step-by-step fashion,
such as making bread, or the history of New Zealand. A sequence text (like a continuum)
shows something happening over time, like the making of pizzas. The events follow a set
order. The sequence can't be scrambled. In making pizzas, the yeast base has to be made first.
Then the topping is put on the dough. Then some seasoning, and so on. In discussion, students
can talk about why the sequence is the way it is, and they can check the text to verify their
ideas.
A description text can have a list structure, where there is no set order. It may be a list of
different kinds of drill centres for a lesson in Workshop Technology, or a list of different
kinds of shortages that occurred in war-time, for a Social Studies lesson. It doesn't matter
what goes first in the list. Students can be challenged as to whether or not the text fits a list
structure. Why isn’t this a sequence? And so on.
A description text can also have a web structure. This would apply to a text about one thing,
for example, a text about small birds known as "white eyes". The web structure has
categories, such as where white eyes originally came from (they came from Australia), where
they live, what they eat, what they look like, who their enemies are, and so on.
Another description structure is the weave (or matrix) This is a grid structure. It is often called
a weave structure in that it threads different ideas across one another. An easy example of a
weave is a comparison of two countries, such as Japan and New Zealand. In Social Studies,
several countries can be compared on a number of dimensions, such as population, area,
language, culture, manufacturing, exports, and so on. In Music, different kinds of music, such
as jazz, classical, rock, and rap can be compared in terms of origins, purposes, examples of
each kind, and kinds of notes used to create each type of music.
The argument structure is persuasive text. A series of arguments are presented, with
supporting details. For example, the text might be on the topic "Should the driving age be
raised?" An argument text has a point and counterpoint structure For example, one factor in
deciding age of driving is ability. The argument for raising the age is that "older are wiser".
But the argument against is that "older can be dumber". Another factor is income. An
argument for raising the age might be that when you are older, you are better able to pay for
maintenance of your car, insurance, etc. A counterpoint is that many young people use their
parents’ cars, so this is not a problem. The point-counterpoint structure can be diagrammed as
just two columns, with the headings For and Against.
Does text structure teaching improve reading comprehension? The quick answer is that it is
very useful for comprehension of specific material, such as topics that have to be read in
textbooks. In terms of improving comprehension in general, as measured by standardised
tests, the evidence is not crystal clear. In a study of one primary school in Los Angeles, the
reading progress of students was charted over a 10-year period (Calfee & Patrick, 1995). The
students had been taught text structure analysis. The chart showed steady improvements each
year, after the text structure (and phonics) programme had been introduced. The reading
growth curve of the school was much steeper than that of the average for the whole of the Los
Angeles area school district. In other words the school was quickly catching up with the
average for Los Angeles schools in general. This result is positive, but it was not a controlled
study, so it’s hard to rule out other factors (e.g., more resources, a new principal) that might
have caused the improvements.
Text structure training seems to improve specific text comprehension, such as textbook
chapters and assigned novels and plays. A number of studies have shown this. In one study
(Lipson, 1995), first-year University students were shown how to break psychology textbook
passages into categories, with supporting details listed under each category heading. This was
a "web" structure. The textbook passages were on topics like "structure of memory" and
"kinds of emotions". Students had to produce a web each session. A control group of students
were given study guide training . The control group had a study guide that related to the
passage. Their job was to check statements in the guide in terms of whether the statements
were right or wrong. Students also wrote some study guide questions of their own each
session. The results showed that students who received the structure training were
significantly better than the other group in answering questions about a totally new passage in
the psychology textbook. Why did the structural training work? First, students used 4 or 5
categories to "chunk" different pieces of information. This was easier to remember. And a
visual diagram is more memorable. Second, they were spending extra time on the material.
This extra work and extra time on task made students read the text more carefully.
Text structure training helps students to be more disciplined in their reading. Some students
lack good strategies for reading. For example, in Nicholson (1988), a Social Studies class was
studying social change. The teacher gave the class a newspaper article called "Why go
North?" The article started by saying that although there is a popular idea that people move to
the North Island form the South Island because of the climate, that is actually not the reason.
It gave various reasons why people move from the South Island to the North Island. But all
the reasons related to work opportunities. People came North for new jobs, for more advanced
job training, and sometimes for better salaries. The article mentioned that moving North was
expensive, that people had to give up hobbies like mountaineering. Various people were
mentioned in the article, such as a dentist who was employed by Auckland Hospital. The
dentist felt that Dunedin was "too dead". He was referring to lack of job opportunities.
Another person was working in Auckland as a stock-broker. Another person was a customs
officer, who was earning more money in Auckland than he did in Dunedin.
The teacher asked the class to read the article and write reasons to explain why people move
from the South Island to the North Island. The student we interviewed had written 14 reasons,
many of which were not the reasons stated in the article. He had sifted through the article,
found key words, and made them fit into possible reasons for moving North. He had copied
these key words out of the text. He mentioned "more alive" as one reason (the text words
were "too dead", referring to lack of job opportunities). When asked why he wrote "more
alive" he said, "Well, it said Dunedin and all those sort of places, it’s pretty dead, but over in
the North Island, you got skiing, you got roller-skating, you got more discos and that sort of
stuff. It’s more alive." Another reason he gave was "stock". When asked what he meant, he
said: "And more stock handling. You know, getting more experience with stock." He looked
in the text for reasons to support this reason. He said, "Where is it. Somewhere down here,
that a guy became a stock agent. He reckoned you learned more about stock than you did in
the South Island. Here you are, ‘stock-broker’". Notice that the student had confused the
meaning of the word "stock", thinking it referred to animals, instead of shares. The 14 reasons
were all plausible in his mind but this was partly because he lacked a disciplined way of
attacking the structure of the text.
Researcher: Have you read the whole article?
Student: No, just picking out the bits, you know, that have to do with the question.
A text structure approach to this lesson, where this student worked with others to analyse the
article, may have been more beneficial for him. A possible structure for the article is a matrix.
In the vertical columns, you could put the names of the people interviewed for the article. In
the horizontal columns you could list different categories such as person’s occupation, reason
for moving, advantages of moving, disadvantages of moving. This seems a more productive
way of summarising the material. And would be less confusing. Students need a way of
taking all the details and organising them into a smaller number of information chunks.
Writing
Text structure strategies will be useful for essay writing. For example, in the 1996 School
Certificate national examination for Mäori (Ministry of Education, 1996), one question was to
write an essay on the following topic: "Imagine you are a seagull. You meet a kiwi. Before
long you are having a conversation about your homes. Obviously, the student must be able to
write in Maori. But what does the student write about? By using a weave structure, the student
could draw a grid pattern, with seagull and kiwi in vertical columns and categories for
comparison along the horizontal columns. For example the home of the birds could be
described in terms of "location" (beach versus bush), features (straw and materials found on
beach versus hole in ground), purpose of home (to be near fish, to be near worms), and so on.
A weave structure would give the student a framework for writing an interesting essay – in
Maori.
Poor readers in Years 9 and 10
In Nicholson (1988) below-level readers were also interviewed. Often the text material was
too hard for them. They needed help at all levels of the reading process: decoding,
vocabulary, sentences, paragraphs, and the text structure as a whole. Some students really
struggled with decoding. Here are some examples: potassium permanganate misread as
"permanent gannet", urban misread as "unbran", rural misread as "ruin", Hamlet misread as
Harlet", the mathematics sign misread as "tee cee". One student, when asked why she
wasn’t reading the class play along with her classmates said, "You get lost when you gotta
blimmin watch the damn words."
Surviving in high school is difficult for poor readers. The transition into high school for a
poor reader is huge in terms of reading demands and kinds of reading tasks. There is a flood
of new subjects of study, of terminology, and of textbooks to study. On top of reading and
writing difficulties, a poor reader may be required to take a range of content classes, such as
Maori, English, Maths, Science and Social Studies. There are strong social reasons for doing
this. Nevertheless, it must be an uphill battle for a poor reader.
Poor readers and spellers are entitled to reader-writers in some classes, and for examinations.
But poor decoding means that much of their textbook material will go unread. Even class
notes will go un-read. Poor reading and spelling skills also make it difficult to revise for
examinations. This is why it is important to give them lots of help in preparation for class
tests and examinations. A clear list of things to study is important. Often, students do not
know what they have to study. It is sometimes difficult for a student to work out exactly what
the curriculum of study is for each subject. The student has notebooks full of exercises, but it
is hard to "see" the structure of the curriculum. The teacher knows the curriculum, but do the
students? A page which sets out the curriculum of study could be placed at the front of their
workbooks, and teachers could ensure that students check off each topic as it is completed.
This will indicate clearly to students what they have covered and what they need to study.
Research on teaching poor readers in high school
What does research say about effective teaching for poor readers in high school? Let’s look at
some recent work that has been done. There is evidence in favour of after-school reading
tuition. In one study (Leslie, 1998) Year 9 students attended an after-school programme for
two days each week, 3:30 p.m. to 5:00 p.m. Tuition was given in reading and mathematics,
and help was given with homework. Results showed that reading improved, and failure rates
decreased compared to a control group that did not receive the programme. But there was no
improvement in mathematics, and no effect on discipline referral rates.
In another study (Newton, 1990), 48 remedial readers in Year 9 were given either a pull-out
programme (17 students) of basal reading instruction, or were given in-class instruction (31
students) which combined instruction in geography with instruction about reading strategies.
The results showed that the pull-out programme was more effective for reading
comprehension (though not vocabulary) than the in-class programme, especially for boys.
In another study of the effects of cross-age tutoring, high school remedial readers acted as
tutors for primary school children who needed help with reading. Results showed that the
high school remedial tutors made only very slight gains in reading as compared to a control
group of high school remedial readers who did not do tutoring. The study found that only the
primary school tutees benefited (Charnofsky, 1983).
In order to make cross-age tutoring effective, older remedial tutors have to be well prepared
and instructed in how to tutor younger children, and in what they are trying to achieve. A
study (Juel, 1996) which found positive effects for cross-age tutoring was one in which the
tutors were given weekly 2-hour training sessions in how to tutor. The tutors were asked to do
4 hours of reading each week on their own, and to keep a reading log. The tutors used text
material that had lots of repetitive, high-frequency vocabulary. The tutors also engaged in lots
of letter-sound instruction.
In another study, sustained silent reading was used with Year 9 remedial readers. Results
showed it did not make any extra difference when compared with their normal remedial
instruction (Lund, 1983). On a positive note, it was no worse than the regular tuition they
received. This result is consistent with a number of studies on sustained silent reading.
Widdowson, Moore and Dixon (1998) found that when the teacher modelled sustained silent
reading in front of students, by sitting at his or her desk reading a book, student on-task
reading behaviour increased, though only for average and below-average readers. This study
was unable to show whether the extra on-task behaviour led to gains in reading skill, but it
does suggest a possible reason to explain why there are often no improvements in reading
comprehension.
There is some evidence to suggest that strategies to increase decoding skills can improve the
reading comprehension of high school poor readers. Taka (1997) taught adult beginner
readers to improve their word reading accuracy and speed, using a bingo game. The improved
word reading skills also led to improvements in comprehension. Practice in reading breadand-butter subject area terms that are hard to decode can be helpful (e.g., in English, words
like "preposition" "theme", and "character"; in Science, words like "energy" and
"transpiration").
It is important to teach poor readers how to read long words. Calfee & Patrick, 1995) call this
"word work". Many long words are compounds of two smaller words (e.g., "rain-drops").
Many long words have a simple structure of core meaning, plus a prefix and/or suffix,
especially Latin and Greek borrowed words. Many words can be analysed as a set of
syllables. To identify syllables, a simple rule is to underline each vowel in the word. This tells
the number of syllables. Then draw a line after each vowel. A useful rule is that if two
consonants come after the vowel, draw a line between the consonants. Then decode the word
syllable by syllable.
Conclusion
Reading is the hidden curriculum of the high school classroom. It is not a specific subject as it
is in the primary school. Yet many reading skills can be developed as part of subject area
instruction. Students can learn to decode technical terms, study the meanings of new
vocabulary, and learn about the structures of content area texts.
References
McLaren, I (1974). Education in a small democracy: New Zealand. London: Routledge and
Kegan Paul.
Organisation for Economic Cooperation and Development (1998). Overcoming failure at
school. Paris: Author.
Cash, K. (1991). Perceptions of selected upper elementary public school teachers toward
grouping and evaluation of pupil learning progress in reading. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation, University of Massachusetts.
Mecoli, K.B. (1994). A comparative study of student perceptions of learning environments in
ability and non-ability grouped classrooms. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Miami
University.
Harris, L.J. (1997). An analysis of mathematical achievement, differential aptitude, academic
attitudes, and demographic characteristics of ability grouped ninth-grade mathematics
students. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Northern Illinois University.
Nicholson, T., & Gallienne, G. (1995). Struggletown meets Middletown: A survey of reading
achievement levels among 13-year-old pupils in two contrasting socioeconomic areas. New
Zealand Journal of Educational Studies, 30, 15-24.
Thorndike, R.L. (1973). Reading comprehension education in fifteen countries. New York:
John Wiley.
Ministry of Education (1998, November). Education Statistics News Sheet, 8, No. 11.
Calfee, R.C., & Patrick, C.L. (1995). Teach our children well. Stanford, CA: Stanford
Alumni.
Rutter, (1983). School effects on pupil progress: Research findings and policy implications.
Child Development, 54, 1-29.
Finn, J. D., & Achilles, C. M. (1990). Answers and questions about class size: A statewide
experiment. Educational research Journal, 27, 557-577.
Finn, J. D. et al. (1990). Three years in a small class. Teaching and Teacher Education, 6,
127-136.
Barker, R. G., & Gump, P. V. (1964). Big school, small school. Stanford, CA: Stanford
University Press.
French, N. K. (1993). Elementary teacher stress and class size. Journal of Research and
Development in Education, 26, 66-73.
Flockton, L., & Crooks, T. (1997). Reading and speaking assessment results 1996.
Wellington: Ministry of Education.
Bachman, J. G., & Schulenberg, J. (1993). How part-time work intensity relates to drug use,
problem behavior, time use, and satisfaction among high school seniors: Are these
consequences or merely correlates? Developmental Psychology, 29. 220-235.
Vacca, R. T. (1998). Let’s not marginalize adolescent literacy. Journal of Adolescent and
Adult Literacy, 4, 604-609.
Newton, C.R. (1990). The impact of compensatoryeducation models upon reading
achievement of saeventh-grade students in a Pensylvania school distriuct. Unpublished
doctoral thesis, LeHigh University.
Nicholson, T. (1988). Reading and learning in the junior secondary school. Wellington:
Department of Education.
Nicholson, T. (1984). Experts and novices: A study of reading in the high school classroom.
Reading Research Quarterly, 19, 436-451.
Tseng, S.M. (1998). Reading comprehension and motivation: A comparison of Taiwanese and
English-speaking secondary school students. Unpublished master’s thesis, The University of
Auckland
Lim, B.S. (1998). Factors associated with Korean-American students’ mathematics
achievement. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Washington.
Calfee, R.C., & Patrick, C.L. (1995). Teach our children well. Stanford, CA: Stanford
Alumni.
Nicholson, T. (1997). Solving reading problems across the curriculum. Wellington and
Melbourne: Australian and New Zealand Councils for Educational Research
Thompson, G.B., & Nicholson, T. (1999). Learning to read: Beyond phonics and whole
language. New York: Teachers College Press.
Dymock, S.J. (1999). Learning about text structure. In G.B. Thompson & T. Nicholson
(Eds.), Learning to read: Beyond phonics and whole language. New York: Teachers College
Press.
Lipson, M. (1995). The effect of semantic mapping instruction on prose comprehension of
below-level College readers. Reading Research and Instruction, 34, 367-378.
Leslie, A.V. (1998). The effects of an after-school tutorial program on the reading and
mathematics achievement, failure rate, and discipline referral rate of students in a rural middle
school. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Georgia.
Charnofsky, N.M. (1983). The effects of a cross-age tutoring program for teaching remedial
reading to high school students. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Brigham Young
University.
Juel, C. (1996). What makes literacy tutoring effective? Reading research Quarterly, 31, 268289.
Lund, J.M. (1983). Sustained silent reading with junior high school remedial readers.
Unpublished doctoral study, Yeshiva University.
Widdowson, D.A., Moore, D.W., & Dixon, R. (1999). Engaging in recreational reading. In
G.B. Thompson & T. Nicholson (Eds.), Learning to read: Beyond phonics and whole
language. New York: Teachers College Press.
Download