The Relationship Between Reading Fluency and Reading Comprehension for ThirdGrade Students By Carla M. McConnaughhay Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Master of Education May 2008 Graduate Programs in Education Goucher College Table of Contents List of Tables i Abstract ii I. Introduction 1 Statement of the Problem 3 Statement of Research Hypothesis 3 Operational Definitions 3 II. Review of the Literature 4 Reading 4 Decoding 5 Reading Fluency 6 Reading Comprehension 8 The Relationship between Reading Fluency and Reading Comprehension 10 Reading Instruction 13 Summary 14 III. Methods 15 Design 15 Participants 15 Instruments 16 Procedure 18 IV. Results 21 V. Discussion 22 Implications 23 Threats to Validity 24 Comparison with Other Research 26 Recommendations for Future Research 27 References 29 List of Tables 1. Pearson Correlation Between DIBELS Fluency and AACPS 20 Reading Assessment 2, Comprehension Section 2. Simple Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) between DIBELS Instructional Categories and Comprehension Assessment Mean Scores and Levels i 22 Abstract The purpose of this study was to investigate the relationship between reading fluency and reading comprehension. A correlational study design was used. Participants in the study were 50 third-grade students who were enrolled in one suburban public elementary school. Of the 50 students involved in the study, 31 were females and 19 were males. Data regarding students’ performance on the Dynamic Indicators of Basic Early Literacy Skills, Sixth Edition (DIBELS) and the Anne Arundel County Public Schools Reading Assessment 2, comprehension section was collected and analyzed using the Pearson correlation. The analysis showed a significant relationship between third-grade students’ reading fluency rates and reading comprehension performance. The study also examined the DIBELS instructional categories (at risk, some risk, low risk) in relation to the comprehension assessment levels (basic, proficient, advanced) using a simple analysis of variance (ANOVA). Results from the ANOVA revealed that the instructional categories were highly related to the mean comprehension score and level of performance. Recommendations for future research include using a different comprehension measure, selecting participants from a different grade level, and conducting an experimental study using a fluency intervention. ii CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION Overview The ultimate goal of reading is comprehension and understanding. In third grade instruction begins to shift from learning to read, to reading to learn. When students reach middle school a reduced amount of time is spent on comprehension strategies and skills, and students are expected to understand what they are reading. As a result, many students struggle with reading comprehension. Research has been done to identify ways to solve this problem. Different interventions have been developed and there are numerous articles and books written on which strategies and techniques can best teach children to understand what they are reading. Comprehension is the basis for reading, and in order for students to obtain and use effective comprehension skills and strategies they must possess a variety of skills, including decoding and fluency (Pardo, 2004). In recent years fluency has become a topic of interest in education. It is often believed that fluency can be the link between decoding and comprehension. Decoding refers to a child’s ability to recognize words. Word recognition skills can be taught through phonemic awareness and phonics. For many readers problems with word recognition can lead to problems with fluency, which can lead to problems with comprehension. According to Armbruster, Lehr, and Osborn (2001), less fluent readers focus their attention on decoding words, leaving less attention for comprehension. When students begin to develop 1 decoding skills and word recognition becomes natural and automatic, gains in fluency and comprehension can be made. Fluency also allows the reader to see that meaning is not only carried through by words, but by expression, punctuation, and phrasing (Rasinski, 2003). Once a student can learn to accurately, effectively, and effortlessly decode words he or she can begin to naturally read passages and stories, and can focus on understanding. The relationship between reading fluency and reading comprehension is of considerable interest because it has significant implications for assessment (Wood, 2006). Due to the fact that high-stakes testing is timed, it is very important that students are reading the testing material quickly and accurately, and are able to comprehend what they are reading. A recent study by Wood found a strong relationship between oral reading fluency and performance on the Colorado Student Assessment Program (CSAP) for third, fourth, and fifth graders. The CSAP is designed to measure reading comprehension and to assess state standards in reading comprehension at each grade level. It was found that oral reading fluency predicted CSAP reading performance equally well for third, fourth, and fifth grade, indicating that the relationship between fluency and comprehension is consistent across the intermediate grades (Wood). Results of this study support the idea that short “curriculum-based measures of reading fluency can provide important indicators of the abilities required to perform well on standards-based reading achievement tests” (Wood, p. 100). This demonstrated relationship between fluency, comprehension and reading 2 performance suggests that fluency instruction and interventions can have an effect on reading comprehension and increase reading assessment scores. Statement of the Problem This study examined the relationship between reading fluency and reading comprehension. The study was designed to determine if students’ fluency rates are related to their reading comprehension. Hypothesis There will be a significant relationship between third-grade students’ reading fluency rates and reading comprehension performance. Operational Definitions Reading comprehension performance was defined in this study as a student’s overall score on the Anne Arundel County Public Schools Reading Assessment 2, comprehension section. The scores were calculated into percentages. Percentages were defined by the county reading office as follows: scores of 59% and below are “basic,” scores 60% to 79% are “proficient,” and scores 80% and above are “advanced.” Reading fluency was measured by students’ performance on the Dynamic Indicators of Basic Early Literacy Skills, Sixth Edition (DIBELS). DIBELS defines fluency rate as the number of correct words read per minute. Fluency rates are divided into three categories: at risk, some risk, and low risk. An oral reading fluency score of 66 or fewer correct words read per minute is considered “at risk.” A score between 67 and 91 correct words read per minute is defined as “some risk,” and an oral fluency score of 92 or higher is “low risk.” 3 CHAPTER II REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE This literature review examines the relationship between reading fluency and reading comprehension. The first section offers an overview of the components in reading. A brief synopsis about decoding is examined in section two. Section three provides an introduction to reading fluency. In section four reading comprehension as well as instructional practices and strategies are discussed. Section five explores the relationship between reading fluency and reading comprehension. Reading instruction and effective methods are investigated in section six. Reading Reading is the process of deriving meaning from written or printed text (Alvermann & Montero, 2003). It is a complex process which includes many components. According to Armbruster et al. (2001), phonemic awareness, phonics, vocabulary, fluency, and comprehension are the five major areas of reading. Alvermann and Montero believe instruction in phonemic awareness, phonics, and fluency impact children’s early reading development. It is necessary for a child to learn and understand each area in order for a child to achieve reading success. Phonemic awareness is necessary for the development of phonics; phonics is necessary for word recognition; word recognition is necessary for fluency; and fluency is necessary for reading comprehension (Eldredge, 2005). Pardo (2004) emphasized the relationship shared between all components of reading when noting that, before establishing good 4 comprehension skills, students must acquire decoding skills, fluency skills, background knowledge, vocabulary, motivation, and engagement Decoding Decoding is the process of recognizing letters and sounds in order to read words. Effective readers use decoding skills to translate printed text into the sounds of language. These skills involve instruction in phonics, phonemic awareness, and word recognition. As decoding skills become more proficient for a child, less attention can be spent on identifying what a word is and more time can be spent identifying what the word means. Fluency is seen as the link between decoding and comprehension. Problems with fluency may stem from poor decoding skills. A recent study conducted by Rasinski and Padak (1998) reviewed a large number of remedial readers and found almost all the children were well below grade level in comprehension, decoding, and fluency. Fluency was the biggest area of concern due to the lengthy manner in which the students decoded the words and read the passages. Since decoding and word recognition skills were so poor, it made it difficult for the students to comprehend any of the passages (Rasinski & Padak). Students may view reading as pronouncing words correctly and may not focus on comprehension. When students read words automatically they have good accuracy, and speed is not interrupted by frequent attempts to decode words. This automatic reading can free a student’s attention to focus on comprehension skills and strategies, and can promote a better understanding of the text. 5 Reading Fluency Reading fluency is the ability to read text accurately and quickly (Hudson, Lane, & Pullen, 2005). It is a set of skills that allows readers to rapidly decode text while maintaining high comprehension (Hudson et al.). Fluency also involves reading a text with proper expression. There are three major components of fluency: accuracy, which refers to the person’s ability to read words correctly; rate, the speed a person reads; and prosody, which is commonly referred to as reading with feeling and involves the stress, intonation, and pauses when reading (Hudson et al.; Rasinski, 2006). Fluency is often considered the bridge between word recognition and comprehension (Armbruster et al., 2001; Pikulski & Chard, 2005; Walczyk & Griffith-Ross, 2007). According to Rasinski, “readers must be able to decode words correctly and effortlessly and then put them together into meaningful phrases with appropriate expression to make sense of what they read” (p. 704). A recent study conducted by Eldredge (2005) suggested that phonemic awareness and word recognition were a precursor of fluency. Kuhn (2004) believes one important reason for the need of fluency instruction is that fluent readers no longer have to decode the majority of the words they encounter, but instead can recognize words accurately and automatically. This can allow for readers to shift their focus to comprehension and provides the main reason why fluency is so important. Instruction and Interventions Fluency instruction includes modeling oral reading rates, providing direct instruction, providing readers with text at their independent reading level, 6 providing multiple opportunities to repeatedly read familiar text independently, and providing opportunities to practice reading (Chard, Vaughn, & Tyler, 2002; Hudson et al., 2005; Kuhn, 2004). Instruction should also provide word-study activities to build accuracy. Fluency is not a reading program itself, but “part of a comprehensive reading program that emphasizes both research-based practices and reading for meaning” (Hudson et al., p. 708). This implies that fluency should be woven into all aspects of reading instruction. Modeling is a very important aspect of fluency instruction. Students need to hear and see what fluent reading sounds like. Modeling is the basis of all good fluency instruction. Teachers can implement daily classroom practices such as read alouds, books on tape, and partner or buddy reading to provide modeling (Armbruster et al., 2001). By using guided oral instruction, fluency can also increase. Some techniques include choral, echo, phrase, and punctuation reading (Armbruster et al.). All of these methods provide practice with accuracy, rate, and prosody. Another method of fluency instruction is the use of repeated readings. With repeated readings students read a passage or story several times and are given guidance and instruction from their teacher. According to researchers, repeated reading can be a useful technique when instructing students to read fluently (Armbruster et al., 2001; Chard et al., 2002; Hudson et al., 2005; Kuhn, 2004; Rasinski, 2006). The National Reading Panel investigated two approaches to teaching fluency: repeated reading and independent silent reading. It was 7 found that repeated reading improved overall fluency and reading achievement, as well as comprehension (Armbruster et al.). Reading Comprehension Reading comprehension can be defined as the level of understanding of a passage or text (Bouchard & Trabasso, 2003). It is a “process in which readers construct meaning by interacting with text through the combination of prior knowledge and previous experience, information in the text, and the stance the reader takes in relationship to the text” (Pardo, 2004, p. 272). The ultimate goal of reading is to understand what has been read (Nation & Angell, 2006). Comprehension is the reason for reading. It involves a complex process that includes many skills and strategies (Kolić-Vehovec & Bajšanski, 2006; Nation & Angell; Pardo). To be a good reader it is critical to not only be able to identify the words, but to understand them as well. If readers can read the words, but do not understand what they are reading, they are not really reading. This process requires a numbers of skills, from recognizing individual words to “forming a coherent and cohesive mental model of a text” (Nation & Angell, p. 86). Effective reading comprehension is the culmination of mastering vocabulary, phonics, fluency, and reading comprehension skills (Dougherty-Stahl, 2004). Instruction and Strategies Effective instruction includes direct explanation, modeling, guided practice, and application (Armbruster et al., 2001; Bukowiecki, 2007; Kolić-Vehovec & Bajšanski, 2006). Comprehension skills should be taught and applied before, during, and after reading takes place (Bukowiecki). Instruction in comprehension 8 can help students understand what they have read, remember what they have read, and communicate to others what they have read (Armbruster et al.). A primary method of teaching reading comprehension is modeling reading comprehension skills, a technique that accelerates the improvement of reading comprehension. Teachers must model effective comprehension strategies. According to Armbruster et al., “text comprehension can be improved by instruction that helps readers use specific comprehension strategies” (p. 49). Bukowiecki further asserts, “classroom instructors must model and directly teach students specific strategies that will enable them not only to understand the meaning of individual words, but also to comprehend the meaning of the entire text” (p. 61). Comprehension strategies must be explicitly taught, and scaffolding should be used to ensure appropriate utilization of the strategies (DoughertyStahl, 2004). Effective strategies include making predictions, drawing conclusions, making inferences, monitoring and clarifying, asking questions, connecting events to prior knowledge, visualizing, and summarizing (Nation & Angell, 2006). Dougherty-Stahl reported that good readers apply numerous comprehension strategies such as predicting, visualizing, making inferences, monitoring, synthesizing, and summarizing. These strategies “have the potential to provide access to knowledge that is removed from personal experience” and allows readers to understand and recall more of what they read (DoughertyStahl, p. 598). In a recent study conducted by Kolić-Vehovec and Bajšanski (2006), upper elementary school children’s use of comprehension monitoring, a 9 strategy used by readers to monitor their understanding as they read, revealed a significant improvement in text-level comprehension. The correlations showed that comprehension monitoring is considerably and consistently associated to reading comprehension for upper elementary school-aged children (KolićVehovec & Bajšanski). Furthermore, reading comprehension can be developed by teaching comprehension strategies and by helping readers use those strategies, flexibly and in combination (Armbruster et al., 2001; Bukowiecki, 2007). By providing direct explanation, modeling, guided practice, and application teachers can ensure the comprehension success of their students. The Relationship between Reading Fluency and Reading Comprehension Poor Fluency Can Have an Effect on Reading Comprehension Comprehension is not guaranteed with fluency, but it is difficult without fluency. If a reader has to frequently stop to figure out unknown words, most likely the reader will not remember or understand much of what is read (Perfetti, 1985, 1999; Pikulski & Chard, 2005; Samuels & Flor, 1997). Often students skilled in comprehension read faster than students with poor reading comprehension (Jenkins, Fuchs, van de Broek, Espin, & Deno, 2003). Fluent readers recognize words and comprehend at the same time, whereas less fluent readers must focus their attention on figuring out the words, leaving them little attention for understanding the text (Armbruster et al., 2001; Perfetti; Samuels & Flor). When gains are made in fluency, readers can focus their attention on comprehension and understand more of what is read (Pikulski & Chard). 10 If children are too focused on word reading, then little remains for higherlevel comprehension (Pikulski & Chard, 2005). Two theories, the automaticity theory (Samuels & Flor, 1997) and the verbal efficiency theory (Perfetti, 1985, 1999), highlight the harmful effects of inefficient fluency skills on comprehension. According to both theories, beginning readers first concentrate on word reading and gradually shift their attention to what they read and understand (Perfetti; Samuels & Flor). Perfetti suggested that when readers focus attention heavily on decoding accurately, less attention is available for comprehension. However, when decoding becomes automatic, requiring little attention, more attention may be allocated for comprehending a text (Perfetti). Thus, a direct relationship can be assumed between fluency and reading comprehension. According to Hudson et al. (2005), each aspect of fluency has a clear connection to reading comprehension. For example, inaccurate word reading can lead to misinterpretations of the story, poor automaticity can strain the reader’s ability to construct ongoing interpretation of the story, and poor prosody can lead to confusion through inappropriate groupings of words or the inappropriate use of expression (Hudson et al.). Fluency Instruction Can Have an Effect on Reading Comprehension The National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP) found a close relationship between fluency and reading comprehension (Armbruster et al., 2001). A representative sample of the nation’s fourth-grade students who scored low on fluency measures also scored low on comprehension measures (Armbruster et al.). This suggests that fluency is often neglected in many 11 classrooms across the country and may be affecting many students’ reading comprehension. All three fluency areas -- accuracy, rate, and prosody -- need to be developed for effective comprehensive reading instruction for students (Hudson et al., 2005). Although some readers may recognize words automatically in isolation or on a list, they might not read the same words fluently when they appear in context. It is important to provide students with instruction and practice in fluency as they read (Pikulski & Chard, 2005). A study conducted by Jenkins et al. (2003) revealed that context fluency, which is accurately reading words in context, was a stronger predictor of comprehension than list fluency. The study suggested that “context fluency captures significant comprehension processes beyond those measured by pure word-list fluency” (Jenkins et al., p. 725). These findings can allow teachers to use a measure of context fluency to estimate overall reading comprehension. According to Reutzel and Hollingsworth (1993), fluency development showed a positive effect on second graders’ reading comprehension. The study assessed the effects of developing second-grade students’ oral reading fluency using the oral recitation lesson (ORL) and the effects that fluency training had on reading comprehension (Reuzel & Hollingsworth). Results of this study found that the performance of students who participated in the ORL group was “superior to that of the control group” (Reutzel & Hollingsworth, p. 329), suggesting fluency development had a strong effect on reading comprehension (Reutzel & Hollingsworth). 12 Repeated reading is considered the most commonly recommended procedure for improving reading rate (Armbruster et al., 2001). Repeated reading of text aimed at developing fluency also may be related to improvement in students’ reading comprehension (Reutzel & Hollingsworth, 1993). When accuracy and reading rate are considered together, reading rate accounts for a significant difference in reading comprehension, suggesting that rate is more related to comprehension than accuracy (Jenkins et al., 2003). A study conducted by O’Conner, White, and Swanson (2007) found that repeated reading not only improved reading rate, but also word identification and reading comprehension for below-level readers in grades two through four. This suggested that repeated and monitored oral reading improved reading fluency and overall reading achievement. Reading Instruction Effective reading instruction involves numerous components, including phonemic awareness, phonics, vocabulary, fluency, and comprehension. Teachers are responsible for modeling appropriate reading skills. According to Bukowiecki (2007), no matter the “age, grade level, and reading proficiency of the student, the teacher is a valuable component in the reading act” (p. 59). Teaching decoding skills, helping students to build fluency, building and activating prior knowledge, teaching vocabulary words, motivating students, and engaging students in personal responses to the text are all considered effective methods of instruction (Pardo, 2004). It is the educator’s responsibility to ensure that effective methods are employed and strategically taught. 13 Summary The ability to read is crucial for a student’s success both in and out of school. Effective reading instruction is necessary for success in reading. In the classroom, it needs to be recognized that knowing how to read is much more than being able to identify the words on a page; it is being able to understand what is being communicated as well. Thoughtful attention to fluency can have a positive impact on reading comprehension. The ability to read fluently can increase reading comprehension, and by focusing on fluency instruction, educators can impact reading achievement. 14 CHAPTER III METHODS The purpose of this research study was to determine whether or not a relationship exists between reading fluency and reading comprehension, and, if so, to what degree. Design The study used a correlational design in order to gain insight into the relationship between two variables: reading fluency and reading comprehension. Participants in this study completed two measures that assessed their level of reading fluency and reading comprehension. The results of both assessments were then correlated to determine the relationship between the variables. Both assessments were completed over a three-week period. Participants The participants used for this research were 50 third-grade students ranging in age from eight to nine years old from Manor View Elementary School. The sample consisted of 31 females and 19 males. The participants were primarily Caucasian (60%) and African-American (30%). Other ethnic groups represented included Latin Americans (4%), Asians (2%), and Pacific Islanders (2%). The participants were selected randomly from a group of 92 students using a table of random numbers. This ensured a random sample was used to conduct the study. 15 Manor View Elementary School is a public school located in Anne Arundel County on Fort George G. Meade. The population is diverse and consists of 99% military families. Due to the military lifestyle, Manor View has a very transient population. The students represent a wide range of socio-economic status levels, from lower to upper middle class. Instruments This study used two instruments: the Dynamic Indicators of Basic Early Literacy Skills, Sixth Edition (DIBELS) and the Anne Arundel County Public Schools Reading Assessment 2, comprehension section, for third grade. DIBELS is designed to be given individually to students in grades kindergarten through third. It is intended to identify and monitor those students who are unlikely to meet state reading standards in third grade. DIBELS consists of seven different assessments: Letter Naming Fluency, Initial Sound Fluency, Phoneme Segmentation Fluency, Nonsense Word Fluency, Oral Reading Fluency, Oral Retelling Fluency, and Word Use Fluency. Normative data was collected between 1997 and 2001 by the Early Childhood Research Institute at the University of Oregon. Participants were from kindergarten, first, second, and third grade classrooms in two elementary schools (University of Oregon, 2003). Entering scores into the online system also allows for a comparison with 300 school districts, 600 schools, and 32,000 children (Shanahan, 2004). The researcher used DIBELS Oral Reading Fluency (DORF) to conduct an assessment of students’ reading fluency. The DORF is a standardized test of accuracy and fluency. Passages used in the DIBELS Oral Reading Fluency 16 measures were gathered from the Test of Oral Reading Fluency. Readability was determined using the Micro Power & Light readability software, and Spache readability was used to revise and refine passages to keep the readability in a target range for each grade (Good & Kaminski, 2002). Passages included both fiction and nonfiction stories. The official DIBELS web site (University of Oregon, 2003) displays reliability and validity information for the DORF; alternate form reliability ranges from .89 to .96 and concurrent validity ranges from .91 to .96. These results are consistent with reviews found in Mental Measures Yearbook (Shanahan, 2004). According to Shanahan, DIBELS, particularly DORF, seemed to have fairly high levels of test-retest (.92-.97) and alternative form (.92) reliability, as well as high predictive and concurrent validity when compared to the Woodcock-Johnson Reading Tests and other measures. The predictive validity coefficients were .66 and the average concurrent validity coefficients were .80 for the DIBELS Oral Reading Fluency (Shanahan). Shanahan found DIBELS to be useful in the classroom for its intended purpose; however, more information about the discriminant validity with regard to the instructional categories used (at risk, some risk, low risk) would be favored. The Anne Arundel County Public Schools Reading Assessment 2 for third grade is a group administered, timed assessment. It is designed to measure student performance in reading. Standards, indicators, and objectives are within the Maryland Voluntary State Curriculum for Reading. The assessment is divided into three sections: word study, vocabulary, and comprehension. The researcher 17 used the comprehension section to assess students’ comprehension level. The comprehension section included five short passages, fiction and nonfiction, with 17 selected-response and four brief-constructed-response items. Test items for the Reading Assessment were purchased from a standardized item bank published by Harcourt. The items were selected by pvalues. The p-value refers to the test item’s difficulty level. It is calculated as the proportion of a specific group that answers a test item correctly. p-values range in value from 0.0 to 1.0, with lower values corresponding to more difficult items and higher values corresponding to easier items. During test construction, the Anne Arundel County Reading Office attempted to average out the p-values so that the test was close to a 0.6 p-value. This information only applies to the selected response items. The Reading Office constructed the brief-constructedresponse items, so they are less reliable statistically. The Anne Arundel County Testing and Accountability Office has run studies that indicate the benchmark assessments are very good predictors of MSA success, believed to be a 0.8 correlation (K. Callison, personal communication, February 17, 2008). Procedure Student performance on DIBELS was measured by having students individually read three different passages aloud for one minute. The researcher informed the participants they would be reading three different stories aloud and would be timed for one minute on each story. The researcher pointed to the first word of the first passage, asked the student to begin, and started the stopwatch when the student said the first word. Words omitted, substituted, and hesitations 18 of more than three seconds were scored as errors. When the minute was up, a bracket was placed after the last word provided by the student. The number of correct words per minute was the oral reading fluency total for that passage and was recorded. The procedure was repeated for the next two passages. The median score of the three passages was recorded as the oral reading fluency rate. The rate was divided into three categories: at risk, some risk, and low risk. An oral reading fluency score of 66 or less was considered “at risk.” A score between 67 and 91 correct words read per minute was defined as “some risk,” and an oral fluency score of 92 or higher was “low risk.” Participants were given 65 minutes to independently complete the reading comprehension assessment. Students were instructed to read five short passages and had to answer 17 selected-response and four brief-constructedresponse items. The participants recorded their answers for the selectedresponse items on a scantron answer sheet, and brief-constructed-response items were answered on a separate response sheet. The selected-response items were scored using a scantron machine. The brief-constructed-response items were scored by four third-grade teachers using a rubric system of 3, 2, 1, or 0, three being the highest score possible. A scoring tool and sample responses were provide by the test maker and used during scoring. Results of the comprehension assessment were scanned for each participant and student profile sheets were created. The scores were calculated into percentages and the county reading office defined each percentage as follows: 59% and below is 19 a “basic” score, 60%-79% is a “proficient” score, and 80% and above is an “advanced” score. 20 CHAPTER IV RESULTS The purpose of this research study was to determine whether or not a relationship exists between reading fluency and reading comprehension, and, if so, to what degree. The Dynamic Indicators of Basic Early Literacy Skills, Sixth Edition (DIBELS) was used to measure reading fluency and the Anne Arundel County Public Schools Reading Assessment 2, comprehension section, was used to measure reading comprehension. Fifty third-grade students were randomly selected and results of both assessments were analyzed using a Pearson correlation. The results of the analysis are presented in Table 1 below. Table 1 Pearson Correlation Between DIBELS Fluency and AACPS Reading Assessment 2, Comprehension Section Measures Pearson Correlation DIBELS Fluency AACPS Reading Assessment 2, Comprehension Section 0.783* *p < 0.001 The data supports the hypothesis that there will be a significant relationship between third-grade students’ reading fluency rates and reading comprehension performance. 21 CHAPTER V DISCUSSION The purpose of this research study was to determine whether or not a relationship exists between reading fluency and reading comprehension, and, if so, to what degree. The results suggested a significant relationship between third-grade students’ reading fluency rates and reading comprehension performance. Based on the analysis of the data using the Pearson correlation, the relationship between the scores on the Dynamic Indicators of Basic Early Literacy Skills, Sixth Edition (DIBELS), and the Anne Arundel County Public Schools Reading Assessment 2, comprehension section, was statistically significant (r = .783, p < 0.001). This indicated that the strength of association between the variables (fluency and comprehension) was very high and the correlation coefficient was significantly different from zero (p < 0.001). “p < 0.001” means that the probability was less than 0.1 percent that the observed relationship was due to chance alone. In summary, a higher score on the DIBELS was associated with a higher score on the comprehension assessment, and a lower score on DIBELS was related to a lower score on the comprehension assessment. A further analysis was used to investigate the DIBELS instructional categories (at risk, some risk, low risk) in relation to the comprehension assessment levels (basic, proficient, advanced). A simple analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to determine if there was a significant difference among the 22 DIBELS instructional categories and the means of the comprehension scores. The results of the analysis are presented in Table 2 below. Table 2 Simple Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) between DIBELS Instructional Categories and Comprehension Assessment Mean Scores and Levels DIBELS Instructional Comprehension Mean (%) Categories Comprehension Assessment Level Low Risk 87% Advanced Some Risk 76% Proficient At Risk 38% Basic Results from the ANOVA revealed that the instructional categories were highly related to the mean comprehension score and level of performance. Students in the “at-risk” category for DIBELS scored significantly lower on the comprehension assessment than those students in the “some-risk” and “low-risk” category. Implications This study is very valuable from an educator’s perspective and provides helpful data for reading instruction. The results indicate how important fluency is for a reader and how it can be related to achievement in reading comprehension. Comprehension is a complex process and by focusing some attention on fluency skills during reading instruction, a teacher can help ease this process. 23 The instructional categories provided by DIBELS can be a useful tool for any teacher. Teachers could give DIBELS in the early part of the school year and determine who their “at-risk” students are. Once a teacher has indentified that subgroup, efforts could be made to work on fluency skills as well as comprehension strategies. These categories could also be used to predict the outcome for the comprehension assessment. Preventive measures could be taken to aid these students in both fluency and comprehension. Additionally, specific interventions in fluency might be used with the “at-risk” students. It is also important to note that if a student is a fluent reader, less attention could be focused on fluency skills and more to other comprehension skills and strategies, such as vocabulary development. Threats to Validity There are several threats to validity in this study. Both measures used lack technical evidence for validity and reliability. Most of the validity and reliability for DIBELS was reported by the maker of the test and can be found on their website. According to Brunsman (2004), the documentation provided by DIBELS for the reliability of the scores and the evidence of validity for the described purposes is inadequate. Furthermore, Brunsman argues that additional information on the reliability and validity is insufficient to support the use of the DIBELS instructional categories and that the developers did not describe any studies investigating the predictive relationship of DIBELS scores to state assessments of reading standards. 24 The Anne Arundel County Public Schools Reading Assessment 2 for third grade was developed by reading teachers in the county and has little evidence of reliability and validity. Although test items for the Reading Assessment were purchased from a standardized item bank published by Harcourt, this was done based on the p-value of the items and only for the selected response items. The Reading Office developed the brief-constructed-response items, so they are less reliable statistically. All of the information about the reliability and validity of this test was reported by the test maker and therefore could be considered inadequate. Another important threat is that the assessment was created in 2001, therefore causing the norms to be outdated. An additional threat to validity is related to the scoring of the briefconstructed-response items. These items were scored by four third-grade teachers using a rubric system of 3, 2, 1, or 0, three being the highest score possible. A scoring tool and sample responses were provided by the test maker and used during scoring; however, the reliability of the scoring can come into question due to the subjectivity of these items. The items were written responses and not all students’ responses coincided with the sample responses and therefore were left to the interpretation of the scorer. One final threat is related to the teachers and the amount of test preparation before the comprehension assessment. The participants in this research were taught by four different teachers, each with their own teaching style and level of expertise. Of the 50 students in the sample, 12 were taught by teacher one, 11 by teacher two, 8 by teacher three, and 19 by teacher four. 25 While some of the teachers found it necessary to take time to review possible material on the test, others did not. In some of the classes, sample briefconstructed-response items that were found on the assessment were given to students prior to the test. In addition, the assessment was administered by different teachers, and different motivational or encouragement strategies may have influenced a student’s performance. Comparison with Other Research Results of this study help to support research signifying the relationship between reading fluency and reading comprehension. In a recent study conducted by Wood (2006), a strong relationship was found between oral reading fluency and performance on the Colorado Student Assessment Program (CSAP) for third, fourth, and fifth graders. It was found that oral reading fluency predicted CSAP reading performance equally well for third, fourth, and fifth grades (Wood). The research reported by Wood supports the current study by emphasizing the relationship between reading fluency and reading comprehension and the use of the instructional categories provided by DIBELS. An additional study by the National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP) found a close relationship between fluency and reading comprehension (Armbruster et al., 2001). A representative sample of the nation’s fourth-grade students who scored low on fluency measures also scored low on comprehension measures (Armbruster et al.). This study is similar to the current research findings that students who scored “at risk” on the DIBELS assessment 26 also performed poorly on the comprehension assessment, achieving at the “basic” level. Furthermore, Rasinski (2003) supports that there is a relationship between a student’s lack of fluency and comprehension problems. Rasinski stated, “students struggle so much with fluency, and in putting so much cognitive effort into the task, that little is left over for understanding the text” (p. 35). Rasinski’s statement directly supports the results of the current study by suggesting that those students who scored below average on the comprehension assessment did so based on their level of fluency, according to their DIBELS score. Recommendations for Future Research Suggestions for future research include using a different comprehension measure, selecting participants from different grade levels, and conducting an experimental study using a fluency intervention. DIBELS is a widely-used and popular tool for assessing fluency. The same cannot be said for the Anne Arundel County Public Schools Reading Assessment. This assessment is used only in Anne Arundel County; therefore, these findings would be difficult to generalize to other school districts. However, if a study were conducted using the Maryland School Assessment (MSA), the results could be considered more comprehensive and be used in the state of Maryland. In addition a study investigating the predictive relationship of DIBELS scores to state assessments of reading standards would be significant and useful for many school systems. 27 The researcher also recommends using participants from different grade levels. The participants used for this study were all third-grade students and results only pertain to that grade level. By using students in fourth and fifth grades and middle school students as well, the results could be generalized to a larger population. Also, results would indicate whether or not fluency still remains a factor in students’ comprehension levels as they get older. Furthermore, conducting an experimental study using a fluency intervention could lead to establishing a relationship between reading fluency interventions and reading comprehension. The researcher suggests preassessing students in reading comprehension and fluency and then providing a six-week fluency intervention. Upon completion of the fluency intervention, the researcher would reassess those students to indicate if growth in reading comprehension and fluency had been made, therefore determining if gains in fluency caused achievement in comprehension. The use of a control group would help to determine if the gains in fluency influenced the comprehension success or if time and maturity accounted for the achievement in comprehension. 28 REFERENCES Alvermann, D. E. & Montero, M. K. (2003). Literacy and Reading. In Encyclopedia of Education (Vol. 4, pp. 1513-1518). New York: Macmillan. Armbruster, B. B., Lehr, F., & Osborn, J. (2001). Put Reading First: The research building blocks for teaching children to read: Kindergarten through grade 3. Washington, DC: CIERA. Bouchard, E., & Trabasso, T. (2003). Comprehension. In Encyclopedia of Education (Vol. 6, pp. 1977-1985.). New York: Macmillan. Brunsman, B. A. (2004). Review of DIBELS: Dynamic Indicators of Basic Early Literacy Skills (6th ed). Mental Measurement Yearbook, 16. Retrieved March 2, 2008, from http://www.unl.edu/buros/. Bukowiecki, E. M. (2007). Teaching children how to read. Kappa Delta Pi Record, 43, 58-65. Chard, D. J., Vaughn, S., & Tyler, B. (2002). A synthesis of research on effective interventions for building reading fluency with elementary students with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35, 386-406. Dougherty-Stahl, K. A. (2004). Proof, practice, and promise: Comprehension strategy instruction in the primary grades. Reading Teacher, 57, 598-609. Eldredge, J. L. (2005). Foundations of fluency: An exploration. Reading Psychology, 26, 161-181. Good, R. H., & Kaminski, R. A. (2002). DIBELS Oral Reading Fluency passages for first through third grades (Technical Report No. 10). Eugene, OR: University of Oregon. 29 Hudson, R. F., Lane, H. B., & Pullen, P. C. (2005). Reading fluency assessment and instruction: What, why, and how? Reading Teacher, 58, 702-714. Jenkins, J. R., Fuchs, L. S., van de Broek, P., Espin, C., & Deno, S. L. (2003). Sources of individual differences in reading comprehension and reading fluency. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 719-729. Kolić-Vehovec, S., & Bajšanski, I. (2006). Metacognitive strategies and reading comprehension in elementary-school students. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 21, 439-451. Kuhn, M. (2004). Helping students become accurate, expressive readers: Fluency instruction for small groups. Reading Teacher, 58, 338-344. Nation, K., & Angell, P. (2006). Learning to read and learning to comprehend. London Review of Education, 4, 77-87. O'Connor, R. E., White, A., & Swanson, H. L. (2007). Repeated reading versus continuous reading: Influences on reading fluency and comprehension. Exceptional Children, 74, 31-46. Pardo, L. S. (2004). What every teacher needs to know about comprehension. Reading Teacher, 58, 272-280. Perfetti, C. A. (1985). Reading Ability. New York: Oxford University Press. Perfetti, C. A. (1999). Cognitive research and the misconceptions of reading education. In J. Oakhill & R. Beard (Eds.), Reading Development and the Teaching of Reading: A Psychological Perspective (pp. 42-58). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishers. 30 Pikulski, J. J., & Chard, D. J. (2005). Fluency: Bridge between decoding and reading comprehension. Reading Teacher, 58, 510-519. Rasinski, T. V., & Padak, N.D. (1998). How elementary students referred for compensatory reading instruction perform an school-based measures of word recognition, fluency, and comprehension. Reading Psychology, 19, 185-216. Rasinski, T. V. (2003). The Fluent Reader: Oral reading strategies for building word recognition, fluency, and comprehension. New York: Scholastic Professional Books. Rasinski, T. V. (2006). Reading fluency instruction: Moving beyond accuracy, automaticity, and prosody. The Reading Teacher, 59, 704-706. Reutzel, D. R., & Hollingsworth, P. M. (1993). Effects of fluency training on second graders' reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Research, 86, 325-331. Samuels, S.J., & Flor, R.F. (1997). The importance of automaticity for developing expertise in reading. Reading and Writing Quarterly: Overcoming Learning Difficulties, 13, 107–121. Shanahan, T. (2004). Review of DIBELS: Dynamic Indicators of Basic Early Literacy Skills (6th ed). Mental Measurement Yearbook, 16. Retrieved March 2, 2008, from http://www.unl.edu/buros/. University of Oregon. (2003). Dynamic Indicators of Basic Early Literacy Skills (DIBELS). Retrieved March 2, 2008, from http://dibels.uoregon.edu. 31 Walczyk, J. J., & Griffith-Ross, D. A. (2007). How important is reading skill fluency for comprehension? Reading Teacher, 60, 560-569. Wood, D. (2006). Modeling the relationship between oral reading fluency and performance on a statewide reading test. Educational Assessment, 11, 85104. 32