++++++++++++++ ZOO TECHNOLOGY PROGRAM ANIMAL BEHAVIOR AND BREEDING ANIMAL BEHAVIOR AND BREEDING TABLE OF CONTENTS Introduction to Module I Objectives – Module I – Animal Behavior Pre-Test – Animal Behavior Pre-Test Answer Key Animal Behavior Text Material Animal Behavior Learning Activity Module I Post Test Module I Post Test Answer Key Module I Conclusion 3 4 5 7 9 32 33 37 41 Introduction to Module II Objectives – Module II – Breeding and Reproduction Pre-Test – Breeding and Reproduction Pre-Test Answer Key Breeding and Reproduction of Zoo Animals Text Material Learning Activity Breeding Strategies Text Material Module II Post Test Post Test Answer Key Module II Conclusion 42 43 44 46 Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 2 48 52 53 55 58 61 INTRODUCTION Animal behavior includes feeding behavior, maintenance behavior, responses to predators or prey, play and of course activity that leads to reproduction. Because a great part of the reproductive process involves behavior, we have included breeding with this course. The entry-level zookeeper needs to be alert to the body language and posturing of animals. Everything from a greeting, to play, to aggression and fear can often be recognized by observing ear position and body posture. Thus, interpretation of animal behavior becomes a component of keeper safety. Observation permits keepers to be aware of hostilities between exhibit mates before wounding or serious injury results and gives keepers a chance to be proactive in preventing problems related to behavior. Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 3 MODULE 1 Animal Behavior Terminal Objectives: Upon completion of this module, you should be able to: 1. Recognize the meaning of, and be able to record, various types of body language in zoo animals, 2. Be aware of instinctive and learned zoo animal behavior, 3. Be able to apply this knowledge for the safety of both keepers and animals. Enabling Objectives: 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. Be able to describe the difference between psychology and ethology. Understand the difference between instinct and learned behavior. Recognize the role of specific releasers in triggering particular behavior patterns. Look for and be able to recognize displacement activity and vacuum activity in zoo species. Be aware of how habituation of zoo species can make your work safer. Appreciate that among social species a distress call by one member may elicit attack by other members of the group. Be sensitive to the ways territories are defended. Be cognizant of and know how critical distance, breeding behavior and feeding behavior can impact your contact with zoo species. Recognize such innate forms of behavior as kinesis, taxes, instinct, reflexes and fixed action patterns and be aware of their survival value to organisms. Be aware of such learned behavior as imprinting, habituation, classical and operant conditioning, experience and insight or reasoning and know how these may influence your working with zoo animals. Recognize categories of behavior you might use in recording animal behavior. Be able to interpret animal’s body language for representative species. Be aware of the tendency of certain territorial behavior patterns and know when they develop. Understand predator-prey relationships and how this knowledge can assist you in working with predators. Understand the benefits of social rank in some species groups. Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 4 Pre-Test Animal Behavior T or F 1. An inherited behavior or pattern of response is called a reflex. T or F 2. Behavior that involves movement toward or away from a stimulus is termed taxes. T or F 3. The suppression of response to a repeated harmless stimulus is known as habituation. T or F 4. Insight or reasoning is the basis for trial and error learning. T or F 5. Operant conditioning involves a genetically programmed form of learning in which a newly hatched or newborn learns to identify with its species within a finite time by following or being exposed to stimuli it learns to identify. T or F 6. Basic written observations of zoo animal behavior can often be of benefit to zoo staff. T or F 7. Behavioral records should always include the time and date of the observation. T or F 8. Expressional behavior or body language is used in a minimal amount by zoo animals. T or F 9. Habitat could be considered analogous to an animal’s address, niche to its occupation. T or F 10. A territory is the area within the species range occupied by an animal or group of animals. T or F 11. Because animals come to feel secure within their enclosures, those that have escaped confinement will often try to return to their enclosure if given the opportunity. T or F 12. Biological rank recognizes dominance within a species. T or F 13. Social rank has to do with the position of dominance an animal holds in a species whereas social behavior modifies the way animals live together and communicate. Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 5 T or F 14. Ethology is the study of the interrelationships of organisms and their environment. T or F 15. Poikilothermic vertebrates tend to have more temperature induced activity restrictions than do homiotherms. T or F 16. All primates customarily pick up food in their hands to feed. T or F 17. Gibbons do not lap water. Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 6 Pre-Test - Animal Behavior – Answer Key F 1. An inherited behavior pattern is called an instinct. T 2. Behavior that involves movement toward or away from a stimulus is termed taxes. T 3. The suppression of response to a repeated harmless stimulus is known as habituation. F 4. Experience is the basis of trial and error learning. F 5. Imprinting involves a genetically programmed form of learning in which a newly hatched or newborn animal learns to identify with its species within a finite time by following or being exposed to stimuli it learns to identify. T 6. Basic written observations of zoo animal behavior can often be of benefit to zoo staff. T 7. Behavioral records should always include the time and date the observation was made. F 8. Expressional behavior or body language is the major way zoo animals communicate. T 9. Habitat could be considered analogous to an animal’s address, niche to its occupation. F 10. A territory is the area within a group’s or an animal’s range that is defended from other members of the same or different species. T 11. Because animals come to feel secure within their enclosures, those that have escaped confinement will often try to return to their enclosure if given the opportunity. F 12. Biological rank exists when a dominance exists between different species for feeding, watering or occupancy sites. T 13. Social rank has to do with the position of dominance an animal holds in a species whereas social behavior modifies the way animals live together and communicate. F 14. Ecology is the study of the interrelationships of organisms and their environment. Ethology is the study of animal behavior. Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 7 T 15. Poikilothermic vertebrates tend to have more temperature induced activity restrictions than do homiotherms. F 16. Some primates do not pick up food in their hands in normal feeding. T 17. Gibbons do not lap water. Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 8 ANIMAL BEHAVIOR AND BREEDING MODULE 1 BEHAVIOR The behavior of an animal is what it does, how it cares for itself, and how it reacts to other organisms and the environment in which it lives. The variety and complexity of animal behavior has impressed naturalists for years. Early naturalists were prone to attributing to animals the emotions of humans. They often made anthropomorphic judgements and suggested that many of the behaviors were “instinctive” without defining that term. The scientific study of animal behavior really began with Margaret Morse Nice who worked with some sparrows that nested in her garden as subjects. The science of ethology or animal behavior grew out of these early studies with an Austrian, Konrad Lorenz and Niko Tinbergen working in England being two early giants in the field. Ethology, the study of animal behavior, has as its basic premise: one must observe arrivals under natural conditions to understand their behavior. Psychology is the science of mental processes and behavior in humans. Psychologists study behavior in the lab. Ethologists stress the importance of observation or seeing what the animal does in its natural setting whereas psychologists stress the importance of tests and experiments with the animals under control and finally ethologists are more concerned with instinctive behavior; psychologists with the animals ability to learn. When faced with a new behavior or series of behaviors, the ethologist asks a number of basic questions. These include: the causes of the behavior - what underlying mechanisms are responsible? The function and biological significance of the behavior - what use is the behavior and how does it benefit the animal? How did the behavior evolve? What was its origin? How did a particular behavior develop in the individual? Was it instinctive or learned or a combination of both? Any behavior that an animal repeats in the same way is stereotyped; any behavior that is the same in all members of a species is species-specific. Any behavior that is performed without prior learning is called innate or instinctive. Each instinctive behavior has its own set of internal factors, hormonal and otherwise that, when they build to a certain level allow the behavior to take place. Then if given an external stimulus the animal acts. This buildup is called the specific action potential, meaning the power to perform a certain act. The specific action potential is responsible for an animal performing one behavior in preference to other behaviors. As the specific action potential builds the animal becomes restless and performs appetitive behavior, an introductory phase of the specific behavior pattern that “sets the stage” for the Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 9 instinctive behavior. Predators may be roaming, following no particular path, when a prey animal bolts. The actions of the predator become instinctive, it pursues and kills the prey. The appetitive behavior is reduced when the prey is seen and ceases with it being eaten. Sometimes when the specific action potential is high and no external stimulus releases the accumulated specific action potential the animal may act without a stimulus, at least without any stimulus we are aware of. Such behaviors are termed vacuum activities. For example a bird that hunts, catches and eats insects may perform all the actions of hunting, catching and swallowing even though no insect or other visible stimulus is present. A behavior pattern or structure, which serves to trigger a behavior pattern, is called a releaser. There seems to be a specific release for each behavior pattern. It is almost like a key opening a lock. The sounds of young can serve as releasers as can color patterns. Sometimes a releaser functions but a learned response such as fear interferes with the normal behavior pattern. If two aggressive birds meet at the edge of a territory each may have a releaser to attack but the fear of the opponent stifles the attack response and an unusual behavior called displacement activity occurs. For example two herring gulls in a conflict situation instead of attacking may preen and a young child may chew fingernails. Occasionally one of two aggressive herring gulls instead of attacking one another or fleeing instead pull up clumps of grass, a type of behavior called redirected activity. Feeding Behavior As a keeper many of your behavioral observations will relate to feeding activity, as you are present before, immediately after or during the presentation of food. It is natural to notice that many, but not all, primates pick up food with their hands as a normal means of eating and that gibbons do not lap water like a cat. You may also notice hoofed stock with long horns or antlers may have trouble reaching a feeding trough placed too close to a fence. It is not much work to solve that problem by moving the trough to a more appropriate location but sometimes it does not get done. A keeper should record such problems and make recommendations for their solution. ZOO ANIMAL BEHAVIOR Animals’ response to the environment is considered under the general heading of behavior. Instinct is the name we give inherited behavior. Learned behavior can develop new patterns or modify instinctive behavior. Some of the kinds of innate behavior include: KINESIS: This is a movement that lacks directional orientation and depends upon the intensity of the stimulation. Essentially, the stimulus produces a change of rate of random movements. Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 10 TAXES: This name is given to behavior that involves movement to or away from a stimulus. An example would be cockroaches fleeing from light. INSTINCT: This is an inherited or innate pattern of response. newborn mammals is an instinctive behavior. Suckling by REFLEXES: These are responses of part of the body to a stimulus that does not involve the higher brain centers. The blinking of an eye or knee jerk are examples of reflexes. FIXED ACTION PATTERNS: These are complex instinctive behaviors. An example might be the series of behavioral activities engaged in by birds that are not sexually dimorphic that allows mate selection. It may include bowing, mutual vocalization, mutual preening and posturing. Sometimes a series of dozens of behavioral acts, each stimulating a response, follow one another. LEARNED BEAHVIOR INCLUDES IMPRINTING: This is a genetically programmed form of learning in which a newly hatched or newborn learns to identify with its species within a finite time by following or being exposed to stimuli it learns to identify. The sensitive period for imprinting baby mallard ducks to follow a man instead of other ducks is 13 to 16 hours after hatching. This is the time when the female typically leaves the nest and, by following, the ducklings learn they are ducks. HABITUATION: This is the suppression of response to a repeated harmless stimulus. An example might be animals learning to ignore a train or the sound of a horn near the enclosure. The stimulus results in no reward or punishment. CLASSICAL AND OPERANT CONDITIONING: Involves rewarding an animal for a particular action while at the same time providing a separate and distinct stimulus. Pavlov’s work giving a dog food, which resulted in salivation while at the same time ringing a bell was repeated. Then he found just ringing a bell would elicit salivation. This operant conditioning is used to train dolphins. EXPERIENCE: This is the basis for trial and error learning. An example is the frog that snaps up a wasp and gets its tongue stung. The frog Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 11 learns to associate the color and pattern of the wasp with pain and will not again try to eat a wasp. When a young insectivorous bird leaves its nest it picks at pebbles, leaves or sticks until it finds and eats an insect. Once the bird has eaten the insect, a reward, it greatly reduces the frequency with which it picks at pebbles, sticks or other non-rewarding items. INSIGHT OR REASONING: This is the most sophisticated form of learning where the solution to an entirely different problem is used to solve a new challenge. Chimpanzees using straws to fish termites from burrows would be an example. One of the most important skills a zookeeper can acquire is the ability to observe animals and accurately record and interpret what is being seen. This ability grows with experience and as you become more familiar with the individual animals. The more you know about the animals, their normal appearance and behavior, the easier it is to determine when something is wrong. Often the sense that something is wrong is in the form of a feeling for something undefined. At other times cues may be more obvious, such as a limp, a swollen limb or lack of appetite. You must learn to make your mind receptive to the information provided by your senses. The more receptive your mind is to what you see, the easier it is for it to be registered in the conscious mind. Often your subconscious may register small changes in deportment, posture or locomotion that will trigger the feeling that something isn’t normal about an animal. Learn to trust your instinctive response to what you see. Try to really “see” what you are looking at and train yourself to observe details. Zoo keeping is a profession where observation, retention and interpretation of sensory information, plays an important role. You must not let your “book learning” blind you to subconscious interpretations or signs. Try to get to know all the animals in your care as individuals by individual markings, color or tag or band number. Scars, cuts, lost feathers or missing digits can all help you recognize individual animals. Look at things in the exhibit for clues too. Fresh secretions indicate an animal marking its territory; hair or feathers could indicate molting or a fight. The condition of the feces can be a good indicator of the animal’s general condition. Use your eyes, ears and nose to evaluate your animals. Know what a healthy specimen’s stance, or posture, coat condition, smells and eye shine are like. Look at the fences and buildings and see if there are holes, projecting nails, broken boards or other hazards and if there are repair or report them. Remember to: Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 12 1. 2. 3. 4. Observe your animals Record what you see Report what you see. Communicate Follow-up (feedback) Whenever you look at an animal you should be observing the following and asking the following questions: a) b) c) d) What is the overall feather or coat condition, eye shine, stance and locomotion, state of hooves, claws, nails, cuts or injuries, discharges and weight? Is the animal behavior normal for the species and individual or out of the ordinary? Are the animal’s feces and urine normal? Are all the members of the group present? If an animal has stopped eating you should be able to determine if it is about to shed its skin, give birth or if it is sick. If everything is not normal record it and try to ascertain why. RECORDING BEHAVIORAL OBSERVATIONS Each observation should be recorded so that other zoo staff can benefit from it. Note taking is a very important tool in observation. You may record observations with a tape recorder, videotape, movie or still photography but basic written records of observations are the most important tool. You should always record the time and date the observation was made. For behavioral studies check sheets are often created to save time writing. Keepers using abbreviations or special terms should always include a legend so that others reading the observation notes can understand what is meant. Many categories can be used to define an animal’s action when describing behavior or interaction. These include: a) Social Behavior: 1) Structure: dominance, submission, courtship, reproduction, adult/young etc. 2) Compatibility: intra-species or inter-species. 3) Environment: territorial marking or protection; the animals use of the exhibit space, the effect of the environment on the animal and vice-versa. 4) Cyclic Behavior: seasonal or circadian changes – sleep, rest and play cycles, reproductive behavior. Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 13 b) Maintenance Behavior 1) Feeding: Social structure of the feeding group, food preferences, etc. 2) Grooming: Self and social grooming, preening, interaction with plants or the exhibit. 3) Communications: vocal, visual, olfactory and physical contact. 4) Elimination: Fecal deposition, coprophagy, and urine marking. 5) Locomotion: methods, sleep/rest position, aquatic, aerial, arboreal, terrestrial, flight or other methods. c) Agnostic Behavior 1) Flight 2) Threat: bluffing, attack 3) Thresholds: changes in critical and personal distances, etc. 4) Communications: vocal, postural 5) Cyclic behavior: aggression during feeding or rut; when with young, etc. d) Reproduction Behavior 1) Social Structure: group interaction or solitary 2) Male/Female interaction 3) Female/Male interaction 4) Copulation 5) Adult/young/parent: interaction e) Other Behavior 1) Stress: boredom, pacing other nervous behavior 2) Displacement activity: stress manifested in other behaviors. 3) Reaction to enrichment 4) Intra/Inter species: relationships with other animals 5) Spatial occupation: use of various parts and levels of the exhibit. Examples of agnostic and appeasement behaviors are shown in the next four figures. Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 14 EXPRESSIONAL BEHAVIOR IN CANINES ATTACK SUBMISSION OR DEFENSE EXPRESSIONAL BEHAVIOR IN HORSES Top row: Threat Bottom row: Greeting Note the positioning of the ears (adapted from Trumler, 1959) Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 15 Note the different positioning of the ears between a dominant and submissive animal. Also note the increased gape of the mouth towards the submissive expression. Facial expressions: top left: neutral expression top center: light threat top right: ready to attack center: intermediate expression between dominance and submission bottom: intense submission (fear)to fearful threat (Adapted from Lorenz, 1963) EXPRESSIONAL BEHAVIOR IN GULL, HERMIT THRUSH AND PHEASANT AGGRESSION APPEASEMENT POSTURES OF A GOOSE 1) at ease 6) defense 2) alarm 7)inferiority Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 3) threat 4) attack 8)approaching a mate 16 5) aggression-fear conflict BEHAVIOR LEADING TO A FIGHT NORWAY RATS Note: Attack directed to testis of opponent (Adapted from Erbe-Eibesfeldt, 1963) GRANT’S GAZELLE Appeasement and threat display. Adult exhibits “high horn threat”. Horn nodding Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 Approaching opponents. Heads turned aside. Head high throat display 17 Fighting-pushing with foreheads horns interlocked POSTURE IN A MONITOR LIZARD NORMAL POSTURE THREAT POSTURE Development of a fight from Threat Posture (adapted from Ruffenberg, 1981) Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 18 OBSERVATION TIPS Perhaps the most important item is to keep your recorded descriptions accurate. Record what you see, not your interpretation of what you think you see. Remember to listen as well as look. Knowing usual sounds and recognizing alarm calls can alert you to problems. Geese, parrots and other animals have alarm calls that you can hear even when out of sight of the animals. Learn to be observant, know what the public is doing, what animals on site (both caged and the uncaged) (magpies, squirrels, sparrows and crows) are doing and whether they pose a risk. Be aware that thunderstorms can panic animals causing them to run into fences or even be hit by lightning so consider moving them into shelter. Try to understand why your animals behave the way they do and make recordings when the observations occur. Don’t depend upon your memory to list them later. Remember the dullest pencil is better than the sharpest mind when it comes to remembering. It helps to share your observations with other zoo staff. Problem solving is often the result of discussion following several people’s observation of particular behavior. BEHAVIOR IN ANIMALS AND KEEPERS Ethology is the study of animal behavior. It involves the objective study of animals and man from a biological aspect with emphasis on species specific behavior, its function and evolution. Another definition of ethology might be the study of an organism’s reaction to its environment. Ethology as a science began in the early 1930’s. Animal behavior is of critical importance to the zookeeper, and behavioral awareness assists the keeper in doing a better job. The better you know the animals the better you can care for them. The major difference between ethology and psychology, the study of human behavior, is that ethology avoids attributing human characteristics to the animal. We should not be such egotists as to imagine that only humans have evolved certain behavioral characteristics. Play activity, curiosity, fear and aggression are common to many mammals as well as humans. To use ethology in managing and caring for zoo animals you need to know certain basic tenets. These include: 1) Typical behavior: A keeper must recognize typical behavior patterns in order to “read” the animal and detect abnormal behavior. Take some time to observe your animals at various times of the day. Different species very often have distinct typical behavior, even when taxonomically closely related. For example lions typically form family groups whereas tigers are solitary animals except during mating and rearing young. Just because closely related species have similar anatomy and physiology does Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 19 not assure common typical behavior. Be aware that although animals provide olfactory clues, auditory signals and activity patterns most, or at least much, of their communication consists of body posture and position (see figures on the preceding pages) that we call body language. 2) Unconditional and Conditioned Reflexes: Unconditioned reflexes are automatic, unlearned reactions such as blinking when an object approaches the eye or pulling a limb back from a hot surface. Conditioned reflexes are indirect reactions associated with experience or knowledge. Many examples of this reflex develop in zoo animals, especially revolving around feeding routines, where animals become accustomed to expect food in a particular place at a given time. 3) Instinct: Instinctual behavior includes inherited “programmed” behavior patterns. Animals inherit these patterns and follow them without conscious knowledge of their purpose. Be aware that they may develop in different stages of growth. Territoriality for example is typically absent in the young and develops with maturity. These behavior patterns are often species specific. We believe the behavior of some groups, snakes for example, is nearly completely instinctual whereas other groups such as monkeys learn most of their behavior within a social system. 4) Stimulus and Response: Animals are programmed genetically to respond in a certain way to certain stimuli. The central nervous system has a selective mechanism, typically activated by stimuli having meaning to a particular species. The “key”, or sign stimulus, and the response it engenders fit together like a lock and key. Stimuli are species-specific; only a particular set of stimuli is of significance to each species and that species responds to those stimuli with typical reactions. “Releasers” is the name given to animal structures or responses that send or emit key stimuli that trigger a response in another individual. 5) Reproductive Behavior: Reproductive behavior is usually quite complex and may involve a chain of releasers and responses. This is an important way of controlling hybridization in nature. Where geographic barriers keep species out of contact in nature sign stimuli (for reproductive activity) may not vary enough to prevent interbreeding when animals are brought together in zoos. 6) Expressional Behavior: The primary means of communication among humans consists of vocalizations. This is supplemented by body language. In most mammals vocalizations only supplement Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 20 such body language as motion, pose, coloration and posture. As a zookeeper you must rely on observation to interpret an animal’s mental state or intention. Without knowledge of the behavior of a particular species we tend to interpret animal behavior on human terms. This often leads to inaccurate observations and can precipitate dangerous events. The sometimes-violent precopulatory behavior of rhinos or mink or the copulatory neck bites by carnivores may be mistaken for aggression. A wide variety of expressional behaviors exist between species, between sexes of the same species and even between adults, adolescents and juveniles. The preceding pages show examples of expressional behavior in mammals, birds and reptiles. HOME RANGE, TERRITORY, TIME ELEMENTS AND PREDATOR – PREY RELATIONSHIPS Animals in captivity show all the behavior of their wild conspecifics, as well as behavior stemming from their captive situation. The zookeeper must be aware that all or a part of the exhibit may be a defended territory. A good grasp of some concepts may create a safer workplace. Animals don’t live completely free in a wild state because their living space is confined by boundaries that are often invisible to the casual observer. The size of the area used is determined often by individual and species needs and environmental limitation. Distribution or range is the geographic distribution of the species. Within this range only areas of suitable habitat are occupied. In the case of migratory species distant areas connected by rather narrow migratory corridors may comprise the range. Habitat is the area within the range that contains the environmental factors and conditions needed to support the species. You could think of habitat as the animal’s address. Ecological niche describes the role of the animal within its community. It considers what the animal does in relation to the food chain, plant and animal associations and energy flow. The niche might be compared to an occupation. To put this in perspective, the geographical distribution or range of the mountain goat, Oreamnos americanus, is the Rocky Mountain cordillera from the North Slope of Alaska to Yellowstone National Park plus the West Coast Cascade Mountains. Its habitat is the steep craggy areas from near sea level to above 4000 m, generally wintering at very high altitudes. Its ecological niche is that of a large herbivore living in small herds, preyed on by golden eagles, mountain lions and grizzly bears. Home Range is the living area normally occupied and inhabited by an animal. It may belong to a single individual, a mated pair or a social unit, a cohesive group with a single dominant individual. Within the home range there is often a defended area which is identified as a territory. Typical Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 21 behavior is usually associated with establishing and defending or protecting the animal’s territory. This behavior and reproductive behavior associated with the territory is unique for each species, and allows several different animal species to live in the same space without rivalry, using different niches within the habitat. Some species maintain a permanent territory while others only establish one during the breeding season. It seems uncommon for migratory birds to establish winter territories. Certain seabirds, seals and rodents live in large densely populated colonies within which territories as small as a nest or breeding site are defended. Territory size is often related to the body size of the animal and its feeding habits, with larger forms usually having larger territories than smaller species and carnivorous forms defending larger areas than herbivores. Not every part of the home range is used evenly. Often trails lead from one activity area to another. Preferred routes exist on the ground, through treetops in water or air. Often parts of the home range may be used for loafing, feeding, sleeping, drinking, bathing or defecating. Animals are reluctant to change their patterns of movement, and familiar sites provide security. The “home” aspect is as important to many other species as it is to the human species as it provides a safety zone within the home range. Animals that have escaped confinement in the zoo will often return to their enclosure (home range) if given the opportunity. The territory is important to those animals that possess one and it is most often vigorously defended against conspecifics and to a lesser extent or sometimes not at all against other species. The more similar species are, the more likely it is for territorial disputes to occur. Territoriality is recognized as a component of “personal space”. In group species this is the distance inside of which the intrusion of a fellow member of the species creates stress where that intruder is a mate or immature. In humans this distance is about an arm’s length in western societies but is somewhat less in Mediterranean countries. Perhaps you have felt discomfort when an Italian or Greek talked to you “in your face”. Another component is what is recognized as critical distance. Encroachment of an intruder within this distance may elicit an attack. Flight distance is recognized in many prey species which, while they may defend a territory against conspecifics, flee when a perceived potential predator gets within this distance. Flight distance can be modified by habituation so zoo animals that would ordinarily flee from humans in the wild tolerate the close approach of visitors within the zoo. A territory may be delineated so other members of the species can recognize it in several ways depending upon the species. Optical, acoustic or olfactory cues or a combination of these are often used. Optical or visual demarcation may involve using the whole body or even parts of it. It has been called static-optical when the result arises from the presence or appearance of the animals’ body in Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 22 the territory as in the giraffe, and dynamic-optical when the animal uses a special signaling device in a stereotypical movement (the waving of the fiddler crabs claw). Form and color are often important. Acoustic demarcation includes the singing of birds, the calling of amphibians, the bellowing of alligators, the hooting of some primates and the buzzing of some insects. All serve to advertise the presence of a territory. Olfactory demarcation is very common among mammals with a well-developed sense of smell. Urine, feces and products of special scent glands are used alone or in conjunction to mark out the territory. Generally male scent glands are more elaborate than those of the females. Just like people other animals are creatures of habit, awakening, moving within their home range and feeding at routine times and places. These movement patterns are often clearly visible in zoos. Llamas, rhinos, muntjac and stallions routinely use special places for defecation. Even in small exhibits animals establish specific routes for travel within the exhibit. Part of the exhibit may be devoted to eating or sleeping. BEHAVIOR RELATED TO TIME Rhythms of differing lengths affect behavior. There are daily (circadian) rhythms, monthly rhythms, estrus cycles and yearly, or annual, rhythms that bring about differing responses. Circadian rhythms often govern the animal’s daily activity and rest. Often these determine the way an animal functions in its niche, that is whether it is nocturnal (active at night), diurnal (active during the day),or crepuscular ( active during twilight times). The onset of hibernation, aestivation, migration or breeding is often under the influence of the yearly cycle. Estrus and the birth of young frequently occur at set times, as does egg laying. The zookeeper needs to be familiar with the species-specific cycles in order to provide for the changing needs of the animal throughout the year. These needs may involve a change in amount or kinds of food, adjustment in light periods, heat or humidity, the provision of nest boxes and nesting materials. PREDATOR – PREY RELATIONSHIPS Free-living animals attempt to avoid contact with potential predators including man. Wild animals constantly check their surroundings for danger. In the zoo we see this in waterfowl, rodent groups, primates and meerkat sentries. Because man is regarded as a universal enemy by both predator and prey species, we can include enemy-prey relationships with predator-prey relationships. Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 23 Normally an animal that senses a predator responds with the escape reaction, aimed at avoiding contact by fleeing, remaining motionless, hiding or using camouflage. The escape reaction is specific for sex, age, the kind of predator and surroundings. For instance a marmot that would ordinarily retreat to its burrow may instead lie motionless if surprised at a distance from the burrow. An enemy, or what is perceived to be an enemy, enters into the sequence of escape reactions specific for sex, age, the kind of enemy, and surroundings. a) Flight distance: This is the distance at which an animal will begin to flee when approached by an enemy. This distance is related to the perceived significance of the intruder. Individual experiences of an animal may increase or decrease the distance involved. Man could decrease the distance (for example a keeper bringing food) or increase it (a hunter or, initially, the zoo visitors). Sometimes, as when the protective impulse to defend offspring exists, the flight distance can be greatly reduced, possibly eliminated, on the flight reaction may even be modified to an attack. b) Critical distance: This is a component of the flight distance. A penned or cornered animal without an escape corridor will tolerate the approach of an enemy up to the point where it feels forced to defend itself; the attack/defense takes place within the critical distance range. If an animal feels the keeper is an enemy, and this can change depending upon the circumstances, an otherwise inoffensive animal may attack when you reach the critical distance. c) Personal or intimate distance is the individual distance within which conspecifics are not tolerated. In contact species, such as horses, that exhibit mutual grooming this distance may be 0. Mates, juveniles and sometimes adolescents may also be allowed within the typical personal distance but other members of the species are rebuffed or attacked when they intrude within this space. These limits must be reduced for mating to occur and males practice many strategies to penetrate the defensive zone. Personal distance is often considered a component of territoriality but often it closely resembles critical distance when an enemy is intruding. GROUP AND INDIVIDUAL BEHAVIOR The social position of an animal within a group, whether within one species group or mixed groups, allows one to discern different levels of dominance. These are often called hierarchies or peck orders after studies initially done with domestic chickens. The following identify different behavioral associations. Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 24 BIOLOGICAL RANK Biological rank exists, when a dominance exists between different species, which compete for food, water or space. It implies a situation where the competitors try to avoid each other. The species of lower rank gives way to the dominant species so flights are rare and the dominion species has preference over feeding places and occupancy sites. Examples from the wild include gorilla over chimpanzee, brown bear over black bear or Uinta Chipmunk over Colorado Chipmunk. This may result in a unique situation in zoos when various species not normally associated in the wild are exhibited together. Several years ago a few bison were fatally injured when they were penned with mountain goats that stabbed the bison in the abdomen with their stiletto like horns. A zookeeper must detect a developing biological rank system and ensure all the animals in an exhibit have access to food, water, shelter and a resting-place and are protected from injury. SOCIAL RANK Social rank is the position an animal holds in a social group of the same species. Nearly all animals that exist in packs, bands, herds, flocks, prides or other collective terms are organized into a level of dominance, with each individual’s status clearly delineated. The organization of some societies may be simple and linear as the peck order in a flock of chickens. In other societies the interactions are complex and reticulate. The premier or alpha individual that takes a lead role occupies the dominant position in the group. This individual has more privileges than any other member of the social group. Some species (elk, African elephants, and sparrows) always have a female as the alpha animal. Rhesus monkeys and Japanese macaques may have a male or a female alpha and wolves always have a male occupying the alpha position. The dominant or alpha individual does not always lead or guide the group in its movements. In some deer species the dominant males regularly follow a subdominant, perhaps as a safety precaution. In some baboon troops highranking males act as perimeter guards. The social rank of an individual may be determined by psychological characteristics not simply physical strength. Often, as in canids, the position is established while still puppies. In many societies the social position of the mother has an impact on her offspring's social status later in life. An animal that loses its alpha position usually never regains it. Often the subordinate, or beta, animal takes over the alpha spot. It is believed that each individual in the social group has an established and recognized rank, with the most submissive animal at the bottom of the hierarchy. This animal has no subordinate just as the alpha Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 25 animal has no superior. This established group structure enables the group to replace a leader with a minimum of disturbance, fights or disorder that might endanger the social group. Members constantly show their dominant and or submissive behavior appropriate to their rank to other group members. Juveniles are often under the fewest constraints until they reach adolescence. Until then they often aren’t required to adhere to rank system rules, and may take liberties denied other group members. The rank system provides benefit by reducing agnostic encounters between members of the group because each recognizes its position within the society, thus it serves to conserve energy, too. SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Social behavior modifies the way in which animals live together and communicate. Formation of flocks of birds or schools of fish demonstrate how hundreds or even thousands of individuals can fly or swim with precision. Herd mammals synchronize activities so that feeding and resting are done together. Most predators live solitary lives, whereas social insects may be formed into castes, with different individuals morphologically adapted for different tasks. CONTACT AND DISTANCE SPECIES Contact species seek out or tolerate close physical presence with conspecifics and sometimes with other species. (e.g. turtles, most primates, horses, elephants and goats). These animals have no individual distance. Hippos act like contact species when they are in the water by day but they are profoundly territorial at night on the feeding grounds. Within their enclosure they may defend the area and thus are potentially very dangerous to a keeper. Distance species neither solicit nor tolerate physical contact, aside from the period of reproduction and rearing the young. These animals maintain an individual distance (flamingos, penguins, many deer, giraffes, and tigers) PAIR FORMATION Pair formation occurs in most species, although the duration of the pair bond is extremely variable. Species such as many birds of prey, large parrots, geese, swans, geese and swift foxes mate for life. Others maintain the pair bond only so long as necessary to copulate or to raise the young (weasels, most small migratory songbirds). Most amphibians, reptiles, and fish do not form established pairs at all but only come together for copulation or fertilization of the eggs. Harems are common strategies for many mammals as is transient pair formation for mating only (bears, most cats, bats, lagomorphs, mink). Some species have pairs or harems that last only the season (elk, fur seals, and elephant seals) others hold permanent harems (sperm whales, horses, and pronghorns). During Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 26 the nonbreeding season many species form herds that are all male or all female with young (bighorn sheep, walrus, and elk). FAMILY RELATIONSHIPS Formation of a family bond is important to be successful for raising young for many species. The family bond ensures a food supply and sometimes training for the developing young and some protection from enemies or predators. “Releaser” behavior initiated by the young animal stimulates the adult to feed its offspring. Puppies biting the lips of adult canids cause the adults to regurgitate food. Young gulls pecking the adult’s beak or baby penguins pecking at the gape of the bill elicit a like reaction. Greeting and warning behaviors protect the young. An adult crane seeing a coyote gives a call that causes the young sand hill crane to drop flat and remain motionless until the adult sounds the “all clear”. Without key releasers specific to each species, parents might well mistake their young for food or threats and eat them. Where the male parent does not normally participate in raising the young these protective mechanisms are not developed. Such males often destroy young when they encounter them. Bears are good examples but it also applies to some solitary cat species. Many amphibians reptiles and fish have no parental/offspring ties at all and the young may be eaten by the parents when they are encountered. BREEDING BEHAVIOR Breeding behavior is extremely variable and takes many forms. This behavior can include elaborate courtship displays, combat, or even methods such as those used by some salamanders, where the male deposits a spermatophore that the female must pick up with the muscular lips of her cloaca in order for the eggs to be fertilized. This is a method that most of us would probably consider slipshod and unlikely to succeed. The zookeeper must be aware of the behavioral changes associated with the breeding season. Some male animals, such as elephants, may become very aggressive and extremely dangerous. Female behavior usually changes after impregnation and close to parturition time. Normally approachable animals can react quite differently. Keepers must know when animal births are imminent so that proper precautions can be taken to ensure the best possible chances of safe delivery. In the past, many zoo animals or newborns have died during the birth process so it is important to be prepared for the event by anticipating what might go wrong and developing a mitigation plan. Mating strategies range from a single mated pair to polygamous situations where a female may mate with many males or males may mate with several females. In the peculiar mating strategy of several birds and some mammals, males defend an area among several other male’s territories. In this so-called lek the females come and mate with a male that holds one of the small territories. Usually, this is near the center of the group of defended areas. Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 27 PLAY BEHAVIOR Play behavior is more highly developed in mammals than in other classes. Mammals have distinct, separate behavior for play but most of the actions can be modified into prey capture, escape or fighting later in life. Young animals play more than older ones and their motivation appears to be entertainment. Play is most important in species that take a long time to mature. It can sometimes be observed in group behavior in juveniles, adolescent and adult peer group relationships. Playing youngsters may involve members from every level of the hierarchy as well as their own siblings and cohort members. Play encompasses mock situations that serve to train the young, develop survival skills and provide practice in different behaviors. A single behavior may be repeated or there may be a switch from - attack to defense, from fighting to prey killing, or from attack to fleeing. Often a characteristic exuberance is evident and play behavior often shares species-specific characteristics. General physical fitness is promoted by play behavior. The young acquire species-specific action patterns and improve actions requiring accurate coordination. A young animal that does not learn to use these effectively may be unable to escape predators or catch food. Play enlarges the individual’s experience. As a communal activity it provides a reason for young animals to seek the company of others while their mother is absent. For example puppies deprived of contact with other dogs when they are 3-10 weeks old will not develop normal social responses. Play is a true activity in its own right and the internal motivating factors, which affect it, change with age. The internal stimulus to play diminishes in intensity with age, although adults do play. (The black-backed jackal uses play to determine strength and social status without fighting). Adults will play with babies and predators playing with prey learn by practice. FEEDING BEHAVIOR Species feed on a particular food in a particular way. Animals are designed to effectively gather and use certain foods and evolution has functioned to make them experts. The zookeeper needs to know what the animal has been evolved to feed on and where and how it is gathered. Some animals eat high up in trees, others on or even below ground. Some feed early in the morning, the evening, at night, or all day. Many birds require food throughout the day as their bodies are designed for regular food input. Some animals eat quickly, other animals are slow eaters. Certain species such as the least weasel need feeding several times a day. They will starve if just given one large feed per day. Other animals such as many reptiles eat at extremely irregular intervals. Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 28 THE ZOOKEEPER AND ANIMAL BEHAVIOR The zookeeper without knowledge of animal behavior is significantly handicapped and can be a distinct hazard to animals and their well being as well as a hazard to his workmates. Together with proper observation, knowledge of animal behavior can supply a wealth of detail about the needs, physical and mental condition of the animals. In many cases it also allows the zookeeper to deduce probable animal reactions to a variety of situations and restraints. It is an appropriate tool for determining feeding and breeding, in providing shelter or designing exhibits and can help the zookeepers in modifying their own reactions. The following are critical for the safety of the zookeeper and the animals: a) b) c) d) e) f) The zookeeper must learn to interpret animal behavior accurately. It is necessary to know what constitutes threat behavior and what is a greeting for different species, as often the zookeeper’s behavior depends upon that typical animal reaction, and a misinterpretation could mean injury or even death. The zookeeper should learn the typical behavior patterns of a species so that abnormal behavior associated with injury, sickness or fear can be recognized. Knowledge of animal behavior is needed to communicate with the animal group. The zookeeper also needs to know how the animal reacts to its environment so he can adjust his own reactions. Knowing typical animal reaction enables a zookeeper to provoke the least stress while working, and is thus safest for both the animal and the zookeeper. Learn to know the expressive behavior of your animals. Every prey species exhibits key stimuli or releasers that stimulate the attack of its predators. These releasers constitute the Prey Schema. Your behavior within the exhibit could fit the prey schema of that predator and thus elicit an attack. You must know what behavior to avoid. Most predators respond to running or sudden movements with an attack. Quick movements may elicit escape behavior in prey species too. Prey animals have an innate predator model that contains specific key stimuli that allows it to recognize a predator or potential predator. A keeper wearing a fur-trimmed parka entering a caribou enclosure is largely ignored but if he should bend over and wind ruffles the fur the caribou may panic from the resemblance to a wolf. Violent attempts to escape can injure both the animals and the zookeeper. It is of paramount importance to avoid eliciting this response thus reducing stress, danger and accidents. It is essential that the zookeeper to develop behavior patterns and rates of movement that allows animals to anticipate and respond with a minimum of stress to the zookeeper’s routine. Zookeepers should know the animal’s hierarchies, and where the keeper is viewed to fit in the group’s rank order. It is generally safest if the keeper occupies the alpha role. Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 29 g) h) i) It is important to recognize the animal’s territorial needs and the location of its territory. It is imperative to recognize indications of territorial defense and respond appropriately to such actions. It is important to recognize animal’s daily routines and behavior associated with these routines. An awareness of time and space orientation can be of inestimable value. An awareness of individual distance, flight distance and critical distance is needed as well as circumstances that can lead to a significant modification of these behavioral components. Knowledge of animal behavior can assist the zookeeper in many ways. Knowing small birds generally prefer to roost inside and high up, makes it possible to locate roosts in this position – then if you ever need to move the birds you can close the door and capture them more easily with less stress to the birds. Many animals, especially birds, follow predictable flight patterns in the enclosure when capture attempts are being made. Learning this behavior can be to your advantage, because you can position one person at each end of the cage with the capture equipment in position. If you know you are going to have to move hoofed stock in a few days, you might get them accustomed to going into the corral or loading area by feeding them in there for a few days. Then, when it is time to move them, you can put in the feed and, as they are eating, lock them in. Depending upon the zoo design, an even better method is to establish a routine in which the animals are locked in daily. This also enables inspection of the facility to take place without stressing the animals or risking injury to the keeper. KEEPER BEHAVIOR In the same way a keeper learns to recognize his animals behavior the animals learn to recognize a keeper’s deportment and react to it. An elephant that had never done so before thumped a keeper’s sore leg with its trunk the day after the keeper had injured his knee in a hockey game. Although the zookeepers (to themselves) are obviously of a different species than their animals, the animals may regard a keeper as a conspecific. If you are to work effectively and safely within a confined space or restricted area you need to be aware of how animals interpret your behavior. Although the zookeeper may mean no harm, actions may send a danger, threat or challenge signal to the animal. The keeper must avoid appeasement behavior for a challenge by any member of the group may follow. Behavior is the zookeeper’s means of communication with the animals and body language is the chief means of communication. Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 30 A zookeeper must concentrate on posture and body movements when within the exhibit. If you are to work effectively and safely within confined space or restricted area with your animal’s concepts of flight distance, critical distance and individual distance must be remembered. Animals become sensitive to your intentions, fear, apprehension, depression or glee. Knowing this, and with an understanding of body language and a good body awareness, zookeepers can work more easily and with less stress. Different animals interpret specific actions in different ways (e.g. for some species eye contact is antagonistic). Be aware of what your body and posture is communicating. Make your movements slow and deliberate, not abrupt or fast. Slow flowing movements that the animals can follow allow them to anticipate your next move and adjust their position accordingly. Many animals quickly learn set work routines within the exhibit and, once they are habituated, there is less stress. As a keeper try to practice the following guidelines: a) b) c) d) e) f) g) h) Think before you enter a cage or exhibit. Move steadily, talk softly, whistle or sing to your animals so they know where you are and that you are not a threat. Predators are quiet when hunting. Your tone of voice can communicate your alpha position. Try to follow the same routine each day. This allows the animals to relax knowing what you will do next. Be sensitive to your lack of knowledge or sensitivity toward your animals initially. It takes time to build familiarity and good rapport with those you are working with. It does not happen quickly. Know what your animals are doing and what it means. Try to interpret and understand its behavior. Avoid shouting or abrupt loud noises. Your natural advantage is brains and experience. Don’t attempt to bluff your animals or your work mates. If you, or an animal, are not in good shape treat the problem and find an alternative safe arrangement for animal care. You cannot match many animals in speed or strength. Never lose your temper with animals. Either, or both you and the animal, could end up injured. Don’t rush – be deliberate and patient. Mistakes often happen when people are not taking time to pay attention to what they are doing. Watch your eyes or wear protective eyewear. Some animals (many birds such as herons or cranes) aim for the eyes when pecking. Cultivate safe work habits and behavior. Some of these points may be made less relevant if such a safe working routine as the daily locking of animals into a restricted area is established. The points listed above should ideally be necessary only in isolated situations but these situations do occur. There is no substitute for safety, and no keeper should have to compromise safety to get the job done. Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 31 ANIMAL BEHAVIOR Learning Activities (to be submitted) Go to a zoo and observe the behavior of any non-human primate for two hours recording whether each action is social, maintenance, agonistic, reproductive, stress, displacement reaction to enrichment activity, intra specific relationships (including visitors) and spatial occupation within the enclosure. Comment on any activity you feel a keeper should be especially aware of. Keep a record of the duration of each activity. Design a check list so you can use a shorthand method of recording the various behaviors being sure to record the legend so others know what your shorthand notes mean. Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 32 MODULE 1 ZOO Post Test: Animal Behavior 1. Which of the following is an example of learned behavior? a. b. c. d. 2. Which of the following is an example of an innate form of behavior? a. b. c. d. 3. range habitat home range territory What do we call it when someone attributes human emotions to the behavior of animals? a. b. c. d. 5. instinct imprinting insight or reasoning operant conditioning What name is given to an area an animal defends against other members of the same, and sometimes different species? a. b. c. d. 4. taxes reflexes kinesis habituation ethology anthropomorphism imprinting appetitive behavior What is the distance called at which an animal begins to flee from an enemy? a. b. c. d. personal distance critical distance flight distance specific action potential Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 33 6. What is it safest for a keeper to project in the presence of dangerous or potentially dangerous animals? a. b. c. d. 7. Which of the following is the type of learning behavior used to train dolphins? a. b. c. d. 8. social behavior maintenance behavior agonistic behavior displacement behavior In which of the following groups of animals do we think most of the behavior is completely instinctive? a. b. c. d. 10. imprinting experience classical and operant conditioning insight or readings Under which of the following headings would such areas as feeding, grooming, locomotion and communication be grouped? a. b. c. d. 9. an alpha role appeasement behavior a prey schema a predator model primates canids artiodactyls snakes Which of the following types of behavior is it most important for a zookeeper to be familiar with when using ethology in managing and caring for zoo animals? a. b. c. d. expressional behavior unconditional reflexes conditioned reflexes instinct Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 34 11. What is the area called within a range that contains the environmental factors and conditions needed to support the species? a. b. c. d. 12. What name is given to a dominance between different species that compete for food, water or space? a. b. c. d. 13. it reduces agonistic encounters between members of the group it allows one male to do most of the breeding it assures all individuals will have a resting place it makes it possible for all members of the group to get an adequate amount of food What function does play behavior have in mammals? a. b. c. d. 15. personal rank social rank biological rank personal space How does having social rank benefit members of a species? a. b. c. d. 14. territory habitat niche home range it allows members from only one hierarchy to interact most of the actions can be modified into prey capture, escape or fighting later in life it reduces energy output promotes aggression between interacting animals Which of the following should keepers working with animals avoid doing? a. b. c. d. moving slowly and deliberately when with the animals setting work routines so the same tasks are conducted at the same times using bluffing with animals that show aggression to maintain your status shouting or making abrupt loud noises Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 35 16. What would be a good source of action if you notice that horned or antlered hoofed stock, have trouble reaching the feeding trough because it is mounted too close to the fence? a. b. c. d. feed the animals on the ground away from the fence move the feeding trough to a more appropriate location record the observation in your daily log and discuss the solution with the other keeper do nothing, the animals have managed so far Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 36 MODULE 1 ZOO Post Test Answer Key: Animal Behavior D 1. Which of the following is an example of learned behavior? e. f. g. h. A 2. Which of the following is an example of an innate form of behavior? e. f. g. h. D 3. 4. 5. range habitat home range territory What do we call it when someone attributes human emotions to the behavior of animals? e. f. g. h. C instinct imprinting insight or reasoning operant conditioning What name is given to an area an animal defends against other members of the same, and sometimes different species? e. f. g. h. B taxes reflexes kinesis habituation ethology anthropomorphic imprinting appetitive behavior What is the distance called at which an animal begins to flee from an enemy? e. f. g. h. personal distance critical distance flight distance specific action potential Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 37 A 6. What is it safest for a keeper to project to dangerous or potentially dangerous animals in his presence? e. f. g. h. C 7. Which of the following is the type of learning behavior used to train dolphins? e. f. g. h. B 8. 9. 10. social behavior maintenance behavior agonistic behavior displacement behavior In which of the following groups of animals do we think most of the behavior is completely instinctual? e. f. g. h. A imprinting experience classical and operant conditioning insight or readings Under which of the following headings would such areas as feeding, grooming, locomotion and communication be grouped? e. f. g. h. D an alpha role appeasement behavior a prey schema a predator model primates canids artiodactyls snakes Which of the following types of behavior is it most important for a zookeeper to be familiar with when using ethology in managing and caring for zoo animals? e. f. g. h. expressional behavior unconditional reflexes conditioned reflexes instinct Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 38 B 11. What is the area called within a range that contains the environmental factors and conditions needed to support the species? e. f. g. h. C 12. What name is given to a dominance between different species that compete for food, water or space? a. b. e. f. A 13. f. g. h. 14. g. h. 15. it reduces agonistic encounters between members of the group it allows one male to do most of the breeding it assures all individuals will have a resting place it makes it possible for all members of the group to get an adequate amount of food What function does play behavior have in mammals? e. f. C personal rank social rank biological rank personal space How does having social rank benefit members of a species? e. B territory habitat niche home range it allows members from only one hierarchy to interact most of the actions can be modified into prey capture, escape or fighting later in life it reduces energy output promotes aggression between interacting animals Which of the following should keepers working with animals avoid doing? e. f. g. h. moving slowly and deliberately when with the animals setting work routines so the same tasks are conducted at the same times using bluffing with animals that show aggression to maintain your status shouting or making abrupt loud noises Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 39 B 16. What would be a good source of action if you notice that horned or antlered hoofed stock, have trouble reaching the feeding trough because it is mounted too close to the fence? e. f. g. h. feed the animals on the ground away from the fence move the feeding trough to a more appropriate location record the observation in your daily log and discuss the solution with the other keeper do nothing, the animals have managed so far Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 40 Conclusion The information provided in this module should make it possible for the zookeeper to recognize different types of behavior. Use of this knowledge can lead to better care of animals and reduce injury by recognizing problems between exhibit mates before injury results. It also can reduce danger to keepers by making them aware animals use body language to communicate their emotions. Recognition of these emotions can allow keepers to avoid hostile and potentially dangerous confrontations. A solid understanding of animal behavior will enable the zookeeper to perform his or her duties more safely and effectively. It is important to learn to read animal body language and posturing in order to keep yourself safe and to get to know the animals you care for as well as possible. There is no substitute for safety on your shift. Accidents and injuries to animals or keepers are often associated with trying to get things done quickly or cutting corners on established zoo routines or procedures. Zoo animals, like people react differently depending upon weather, their reproductive condition, conflict with associates and a variety of other influences. Thus it is critical that you follow the policies set up to avoid becoming an undesirable statistic. Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 41 MODULE II BREEDING AND REPRODUCTION OF ZOO ANIMALS INTRODUCTION Today, most zoos depend upon maintaining their collections of animals by raising their own display animals, or trading, or buying animals from other zoos. Thus, a breeding program is usually a necessity as well as being a factor in promoting natural behavior in the animals themselves. In many cases it is no longer practical to buy wild captured specimens for display as it was a generation ago. This module presents information about the International Species Inventory System (ISIS). That program was designed to help ensure genetic diversity, especially among rare and endangered species kept in zoos. Part of the breeding program may involve hand rearing of newborns that for one reason or another are not accepted by a parent. Suggestions for hand rearing are included in this module. Sometimes zoos need to limit reproduction of certain species and you will learn about some ways to do that. Pregnant females and those about to give birth require special handling and environment. As a zookeeper, there are several things that you can do in order to improve the survival rate of newborns. Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 42 MODULE II BREEDING AND REPRODUCTION OF ZOO ANIMALS Training Objectives Completing this module will allow you to understand why zoos carry out breeding programs, appreciate how the International Species Inventory System (ISIS) standardizes zoo breeding records and how the pedigrees of zoo animals are maintained. Enabling Objectives 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. Appreciate that unlimited supplies of wild species are no longer available to stock zoos. Be aware that the convention on International Trade in Endangered Species (CITES) to which Canada is a signatory, places restrictions on the importing or exporting of species of plants or animals that appear in the Appendices of rare or endangered species. Recognize that zoos need breeding programs to replace stock and to make stock available for sale or trading. Understand the need for limiting reproduction of certain species in the zoo and be aware of methods in use. Know the needs of animals about to give birth and conditions that optimize survival of newborns. Be sensitive to conditions that make hand rearing of newborns necessary and requirements that need to be met to make hand rearing successful Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 43 PRE TEST MODULE II: BREEDING AND REPRODUCTION OF ZOO ANIMALS T or F 1. Zoos have always depended upon breeding programs to replace animals in collections. T or F 2. The International Species Inventory System (ISIS) keeps records of the pedigree of many zoo species. T or F 3. The offspring of animals that breed in zoos are often sold or traded to other zoos. T or F 4. Castration or sterilization of zoo animals is generally forbidden. T or F 5. Zookeepers do not normally record evidence of the onset of incubation in birds or breeding activity in mammals because it is of frequent occurrence. T or F 6. When female members of carnivorous species that are typically solitary in the wild are about to give birth it is often prudent to cage them separately from the males. T or F 7. The American Association of Zookeepers made a publication listing the gestation period of various species of zoo animals available. T or F 8. Reproduction of amphibians has not been feasible in zoos. T or F 9. Hand rearing the young of rare or endangered species in zoos is usually more successful and preferred to having the female raise the baby. T or F 10. It is desirable for newborn mammals to ingest colostrum soon after birth. T or F 11. Mammalian milk is generally similar between species. T or F 12. Most young birds have imperfect temperature control of their bodies until some days after hatching. T or F 13. Precocious birds hatched in the zoo incubator may need to be taught to eat. Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 44 T or F 14. Newly hatched birds and reptiles need to be fed as soon as possible after hatching. T or F 15. Rubbing a frog or earthworms over food to be offered to newly hatched snakes sometimes induces them to feed. T or F 16. Under stress a new mother’s protective behavior may be so enhanced she may kill or injure her young. Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 45 PRE TEST ANSWER KEY MODULE II: BREEDING AND REPRODUCTION OF ZOO ANIMALS F 1. Early zoos depended upon wild caught animals as replacements. T 2. The International Species Inventory System (ISIS) keeps records of the pedigree of many zoo species. T 3. The offspring of animals that breed in zoos are often sold or traded to other zoos. F 4. Castration or sterilization is regularly practiced in zoos for species that tend to reproduce easily and for which there is little demand. F 5. Zookeepers need to record onset of incubation and mammalian breeding activity in order to know dates of hatching or birth. T 6. When female members of carnivorous species that are typically solitary in the wild are about to give birth it is often prudent to cage them separately from the males. T 7. The American Association of Zookeepers made a publication listing the gestation period of various species of zoo animals available. F 8. Several species of zoos have been successful in breeding amphibians. F 9. Nearly all zoos prefer to have the mother raise her offspring as naturally as possible. T 10. It is desirable for newborn mammals to ingest colostrum soon after birth. F 11. Mammalian milk varies greatly in concentration of fat, protein and carbohydrate in different species. T 12. Most young birds have imperfect temperature control of their bodies until some days after hatching. T 13. Precocious birds hatched in the zoo incubator may need to be taught to eat. Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 46 F 14. Birds and reptiles retain a portion of yolk inside their body so can go without feeding a day or two after hatching. T 15. Rubbing a frog or earthworms over food to be offered to newly hatched snakes sometimes induces them to feed. T 16. Under stress a new mother’s protective behavior may be so enhanced she may kill or injure her young. Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 47 MODULE II BREEDING AND REPRODUCTION OF ZOO ANIMALS The earliest zoos were stocked with wild captured animals and that avenue was open until nearly the middle of the 20th century. In the early parts of the 1900’s there were individuals who became rather well known for capturing and selling African and Asian species to zoos in North America and Europe. Today zoo animals are acquired primarily through trade with other zoos from such government departments as fish and game agencies, the Society for the Prevention of cruelty to Animals shelters and by donation from the public. The American Zoological Association (AZA) publishes a monthly newsletter that lists animals available from zoos in other regions of the world. They also send out their lists of available animals from their own member institutions. New species for display generally come from other zoos, a few from animal dealers. Because of expense, the availability of animals and stress related problems relating to transportation as well as permit requirements many zoos carry out their own breeding programs. Many zoos in Canada belong to the International Species Inventory System (ISIS) a computer-based information system for animals in captivity and more details of this program will be given later. By breeding animals in the zoo and having the ISIS resources available, zoos can maintain genetic diversity in the zoo animals. Breeding programs thus keep the captive populations viable and keeping the sexes together allows visitors to observe natural reproductive behavior. Reproductive biology is an area of considerable interest and an opportunity for research in many zoos. Reproduction of zoo menagerie members is extremely important for several reasons. Diminishing numbers of wild animals are available for capture. Habitat destruction has reduced their numbers to the point many are listed as rare or endangered. Most countries of the western world are signatories to the Convention on International Trade in Endangered Species which places tight restrictions on either importing or exporting any species of wild animal or plant that is listed in the appendices CITES has published. Thus zoos find it extremely difficult to replace specimens native to other countries, and must rely on breeding their own animals and trading with other zoos. In the past the option of replenishing zoo stock from the wild was an option that today is largely absent. A well-managed breeding program provides more than just a chance for visitors to observe the animals mating behavior. Newborn and young animals provide a focus for visitor interest, and excitement too, as they are often more active than the adults. Their presence helps encourage natural behavior, increases the value of the zoo collection and they may serve as a source of income through sales or serve as stock for trades. For many species that have become extirpated or endangered this is the only source. It needs to be emphasized that breeding programs fulfill a zoo’s role in conservation and zoo born animals have Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 48 been reintroduced into native habitats from which they were extirpated. Membership in ISIS helps ensure that genetic diversity is maintained so animals reintroduced into the wild carry a maximum assortment of alleles. This helps maintain a diverse gene pool in the species. All zoos have limited space for displaying animals. The increasing success in breeding animals has frequently led to new problems relating to surplus animals. Some species that were once difficult or impossible to breed in captivity have become avid breeders when introduced into environmentally correct habitats. The result has been an overabundance of some animals thus decreasing their price value or making it difficult to sell or trade them. Thus zoos have had to engage in methods of population control. These have included castration or sterilization of male animals, chemical implants in females to prevent estrus, separation of males and females, culling herds of herbivores and feeding the carcasses to carnivores, or euthanasia of extra animals. Most birds and those female mammals that have an estrus period breed at specific times of the year. Knowing the breeding season is important as it allows the keeper to separate males and females during the time of nesting or sexual receptivity and thus exercise control over whether or not reproduction will be allowed. There are many species however that may breed at any season and this strategy is inappropriate for them. From mating observations the date of birth can be estimated providing the gestation period of the species is known. The American Association of Zookeepers had as its first project the publication of the gestation period for various species of zoo mammals. This organization has also been active in producing an Infant Development Notebook that contains a wealth of information on mother reared young. Males of certain species of mammals also become sexually active at certain times of the year and this may be associated with increased aggressiveness. This period is called the rut and antlered and horned hoofed species as well as male elephants may be very dangerous to be around when they are in rut. A keeper should be sensitive to when rut occurs and be especially careful then. More members of the deer family kill and/or injure zookeepers than do carnivores and many of these accidents are attributed to males in rut. This is a major reason for locking in hoofed stock daily while inspection, cleaning and other activity is taking place within the enclosure. Remember there is no substitute for safety. For many birds the date incubation commences can serve as a guide to hatching dates. Knowing the date offspring are expected allows appropriate preparation for the new arrival. For some species of birds a diet change suitable for feeding chicks may be necessary. A dark den with suitable bedding needs to be available to many carnivores. Some female hoofed stock may be moved to birthing stalls or paddocks where they remain apart from the herd for some time. Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 49 In many cases, especially in those carnivores that are solitary in nature, the female and young should be kept separate from the male. Modifications to the environment of a pregnant mammal could potentially impact the development of her offspring as increased stress levels cause certain hormone levels to be elevated in the mother’s blood. These increased hormone levels eventually reach the fetus and can affect its own hormone production and development. Thus all pregnant mammals should be closely monitored when changes that might be stressful are made to their environment. The final success of any zoos breeding program depends upon the keepers. It is their knowledge, skills, care and attention that assure the program will be successful. Knowledge of breeding dates, incubation times and gestation periods means the new arrivals can be anticipated. This is far better than being unprepared for the first hours after birth or hatching are often most critical to survival of the infant. By carefully observing the animals and recording their behavior the keeper supplements information available for the individual animal as well as for the species. Sometimes obtaining this information will involve spending extra time with the animal conducting observations and recording the activity. Copulation will not always be seen so the keeper must be alert for a different posture, especially in female hoofed stock, that indicates breeding has occurred. In some cases behavioral changes and visible swelling indicates pregnancy and this should be recorded even if the breeding date is unknown. Many species are easy to breed and zoos can expect regular additions to their population. Other species have been difficult to get to breed with any consistency. Better record keeping and exchange of information has occasionally resulted in brilliant successes. A few dozen years ago it was considered impossible to breed falcons or condors in captivity. Today it is rather routine. Recent successes in breeding amphibians and snakes of the boid family are noteworthy. Alligators are routinely bred in captivity today and large primates have responded to better enclosures by not only reproducing but also raising their young to adulthood. As knowledge of what is necessary to encourage successful breeding and raising young increases, and the standards of husbandry rise, further successes may occur but it will only happen when conscientious keepers observe behavior and record the conditions that encouraged birth and survival. The role of the animal’s environment and captive conditions is undoubtedly critical to breeding success. ISIS Earlier the International Species Inventory System was mentioned. This computer-based information system for animals in captivity began in 1973 when Drs. U.S. Seal and D.G. Makey received seed money from The American Association of Zoological Parks and Aquariums, the U.S. Department of the Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 50 Interior, a private grant and other funds to begin the project at the Minnesota Zoo. The system now includes data on nearly three hundred thousand living animals and 1.4 million of their ancestors belonging to 7500 species. Over 550 institutions in 54 countries are contributing members. Basic biological data such as age, sex, parentage, place of birth and circumstances of death is collected and used in reporting the status of many different kinds of captive populations. Basic laboratory data (blood chemistry and hematology) of normal individuals may assist in disease detection. The mammal data collection started in 1974 with birds added in 1975. Participating institutions receive directories of the world’s mammals and birds as part of the ISIS material. These include all possible species and many subspecies of those groups on record. ISIS has its own web site at http://www.ISIS.org and further help can be obtained at support@isis.org Their e mail address is isis@isis.org It is the intent of ISIS to provide inbreeding evaluation, and demographic analysis as well as to.assist zoos in setting up their computerized record systems. The origin of ISIS was engendered by a realization that a spectacular loss of habitat for naturally occurring populations was accelerating. This suggested captive populations needed to be self-sustaining and zoos could not rely on wild caught stock for replacements. Thus it became apparent good biological data on all zoo animals needed to be available to everyone. Having such data also made it easier to find animals for loans as breeders and a standard computerized inventory system helps each zoo with their own collection’s management requirements. New keepers should contact the primary ISIS representative. It is possible that soon captive populations may be all we have left of many species, as is the case with cattle, horses and dromedaries today. For those captive populations, future existence will depend upon careful cooperative management of the population’s genetics and demography. This requires good, available biological information covering several generations. ISIS accumulates and disperses this information. To create these records each participating institution uses a two-part form provided by ISIS, and either submits a computer disc or e mails the information to ISIS. ISIS in turn distributes reports on each institution’s own collection. A Species Distribution Report on the ISIS website shows all known individuals of every species and also makes special reports available on request. The more than 550 zoos, primate centers, animal farms and related facilities are critical for providing data. A small staff manages and operates the system out of Minnesota and they are funded by a number of zoo related organizations that contribute financially to ISIS. Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 51 Learning Activity (to be submitted) Use a computer to access ISIS on the Internet, determine the following and list the contact information available from the respective institutions for: 1. Four zoos that may have White-handed Gibbon or Lar gibbon (Hylobates lar) genealogy available in Canada. 2. Any zoos in North America that have successfully bred the Takin (Budorcas taxicolor). 3. Any North American zoos that have successfully bred the Puerto Rican Crested Frogs (Peltophyrne lemur). 4. Name any Canadian zoos that have succeeded in breeding Moose in captivity. Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 52 BREEDING STRATEGIES The mechanics of getting animals to breed in captivity is not always easily solved. With species that have a period of estrus or heat the female reaches a point where she will tolerate copulation for a brief period. If males and females are penned in adjacent cages the male will typically be sensitive through olfaction to when estrus occurs and will rub against the females fence and try to reach her. When the female approaches the male as well it is usually safe to allow them together but watch carefully and usually allow the male into the females enclosure or put them both into a new enclosure so territorial defense does not precipitate conflict. Female bears have been killed when introduced into a cage that the male defended as his territory. Species that exhibit a solitary existence in nature are those most apt to fight when penned together although many zoos are able to keep mates penned together continuously. Most zoos prefer to have animals raise their own young as naturally as possible; hand reared, human-oriented young often imprint on humans and do not adapt well later to living with conspecifics. Sometimes it becomes necessary to handraise an animal because the baby has been rejected (or the mother is dead). Then it is best to hand-raise the youngster, wean it as soon as practical and then introduce it to the herd or group in order that it can learn natural behavior. Sometimes in the case of hoofed stock the young one is placed in with the herd or near by in an adjacent pen and bottle-fed. When the mother rejects the offspring at birth, begins killing some of the litter, dies at or soon after birth while still caring for or feeding the offspring, or if she or the infant require medical treatment hand-rearing may be prescribed. If the delivery requires human intervention through pulling, in an assisted delivery or through caesarian section, keepers should try to get the young to suckle from the female. This is so the baby will get the colostrum, as the first milk produced by the dam after birth is called. This milk contains an abundance of antibodies and proteins that assist the infant in developing its immune system. If the baby cannot or will not suckle try to hand-milk or use a breast pump on the female to extract colostrum to feed the baby. A baby mammal that must be hand-reared should be taken to the nursery and if cold given a warm bath followed by rubbing it with a towel and blowing it dry with hot air from a hair dryer. If the baby does not voluntarily suckle fluids may be given but a finger dipped in milk and introduced to the mouth will often initiate suckling. Depending upon the age and kind of animal different nipple and bottle sizes are required and feeding frequency has to be that appropriate for each species. The fed formula must be that suitable for each different species. Baby seals require milk that is nearly half oil. The American Association of Zoological Parks and Aquariums has published an Infant Care Diet Handbook on handraising techniques for mammals, including feeding schedules and milk formulae. Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 53 Where necessary vitamin injections may be prescribed and in areas where selenium deficiency is present all young hoofed babies are given a selenium injection shortly after birth. Young carnivores may require vaccination against feline and canine distemper and rabies. Babies should be kept in warm, draft free areas for at least a few days. If a baby animal has not received colostrum from its mother, cow or goat colostrum extracted soon after the birth of the calf or kid and kept frozen until needed can be used. If the mother can accept the baby for feeding try to encourage them to be together at least for that time. That allows you to monitor the condition of both the mother and the baby. Sometimes young altricial birds may be abandoned in the nest or adults die before the young can care for themselves. Hand-raising altricial birds, especially insectivorous ones is difficult and time consuming as they need to be fed every half hour to an hour during the day. Freeze dried hamburger can substitute for insects but it should be hydrated and mixed with vitamins before being fed. A human simply is not as efficient at collecting insects as parent birds are. Young precocial birds raised from eggs hatched in the incubator do not require as much attention as altricial birds. However, keepers need to ensure that the young learn how to feed, sometimes by picking up bits of food and dropping it. The diet needs to be watched so growth is not too rapid or leg bending and deformed wings may result. Young precocious birds do not need to feed for the first day or two after hatching as they have an internal yolk reserve. However, they are not hatched with good temperature control so must be kept warm for the first week or more. A sudden rainstorm that soaks and chills young precocious birds may appear to have killed them but if they are brought inside, dried with a hair dryer and placed under warm – not hot – heat lamps they may miraculously recover. DO NOT TRY TO DRY OR WARM ANY ANIMAL IN A MICROWAVE OVEN! Newly emerged reptiles and amphibians also retain an internal supply of yolk and thus do not need to feed immediately. Amphibians with their prolonged larval period require daily monitoring of food and water quality. Cage mates may quickly consume smaller animals, so try to separate them by size. Extra effort may be needed to get newly emerged reptiles to feed. Be certain they are warm, then, try a number of different prey items. Sometimes it helps to rub earthworms or a frog over the proffered item, as reptiles seem quite sensitive to smell. Some snakes prefer dark objects and will refuse white mice others prefer baby hairless rodents for their initial meal. Some reptiles will only feed when alone, others seem to be stimulated when littermates are present. Although baby reptiles may require extra attention the satisfaction of success is well worth the effort. Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 54 MODULE II Post Test Breeding and Reproduction of Zoo Animals 1. What is a major function of the International Species Inventory System? a. b. c. d. 2. Under what circumstances is it not advisable to pen a birthing female mammal or hatching bird separate from her mate? a. b. c. d. 3. when the members of the species are generally solitary in nature when the species members only come together during copulation when both mates assist in gathering food for or rearing the young when the male has many female mates Which organization produced a publication listing the gestation period of various species of zoo animals? a. b. c. d. 4. counting all the animals in member zoos keeping records if the pedigrees of most zoo species in member zoos making certain appropriate permits for sending animals internationally are available maintaining records of vaccination and sterilization of animals in member zoos Canadian Association of Zoos and Aquariums International Species Inventory System Convention on International Trade in Endangered Species American Association of Zookeepers What is the main reason for keeping newly hatched precocious birds warm for several days? a. b. c. d. they have not developed independent temperature control on hatching they grow much faster when they are warm if they are chilled there is a high risk of sex reversal abnormal behavior patterns often develop in chilled chicks Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 55 5. What name is given to the time of male sexual activity in mammals that have a distinct breeding time? a. b. c. d. 6. What is the most acceptable method of warming a newborn mammal that is chilled and may need to be hand-reared? a. b. c. d. 7. b. c. d. increased activity as a result of modifying the environment may reduce the nutrients available to the fetus often modifying the environment increases stress levels which cause certain hormone levels to be elevated in the dam and this can affect the hormone production of the fetus. Modifying the environment creates anxiety in the pregnant female and this may lead to abortion Often the stress associated with environmental modification of the pregnant female’s area prevents her from producing milk. What defects are sometimes associated with overfeeding hand-reared precocious birds? a. b. c. d. 9. bathe in warm water, towel, then dry with a hair dryer leave it with the mother and let her care for it put in a microwave for a couple of minutes give warm milk using the appropriate sized nipple How can modifying the environment of a pregnant mammal potentially impact the development of her offspring? a. 8. niche estrus rut heat lack of proper feather development endocrine imbalance leading to gigantism inability to move properly indicated by sitting in one place development of bent legs and deformed wings What precautions need to be addressed when raising larval amphibians? a. b. c. d. keeping them at a constant temperature avoid changing their water assort them by size so smaller ones are not eaten by larger ones keeping them on a normal night and day light regime Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 56 10. What is the main way zoos replace animals in their collection today? a. b. c. d. purchase from or trade with other zoos purchase from wild animal catchers purchase from game farms acquisitions from government agencies Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 57 MODULE II Post Test Answer Key Breeding and Reproduction of Zoo Animals B 1. What is a major function of the International Species Inventory System? a. b. counting all the animals in member zoos keeping records if the pedigrees of most zoo species in member zoos making certain appropriate permits for sending animals internationally are available maintaining records of vaccination and sterilization of animals in member zoos c. d. C 2. Under what circumstances is it not advisable to pen a birthing female mammal or hatching bird separate from her mate? a. when the members of the species are generally solitary in nature when the species members only come together during copulation when both mates assist in gathering food for or rearing the young when the male has many female mates b. c. d. D 3. Which organization produced a publication listing the gestation period of various species of zoo animals? a. b. c. Canadian Association of Zoos and Aquariums International Species Inventory System Convention on International Trade in Endangered Species American Association of Zookeepers d. Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 58 A 4. What is the main reason to keep newly hatched precocious birds warm for several days? a. b. c. d. C A 5. What name is given to the time of male sexual activity in mammals that have a distinct breeding time? a. b. c. d. niche estrus rut heat 6. What is an acceptable method of warming a newborn mammal that is chilled and may need to be handreared? a. bathe it in warm water, towel, then dry with a hair dryer leave it with the mother and let her care for it put it in a microwave for a couple of minutes give it warm milk using the appropriate sized nipple b. c. d. B they have not developed independent temperature control on hatching they grow much faster when they are warm if they are chilled there is a high risk of sex reversal abnormal behavior patterns often develop in chilled chicks 7. How can modifying the environment of a pregnant mammal potentially impact the development of her offspring? a. increased activity as a result of modifying the environment may reduce the nutrients available to the fetus often modifying the environment increases stress levels which cause certain hormone levels to be elevated in the dam and this can affect the hormone production of the fetus Modifying the environment creates anxiety in the pregnant female and this may lead to abortion Often the stress associated with environmental modification of the pregnant female’s area prevents her from producing milk. b. c. d. Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 59 D 8. What defects are sometimes associated with overfeeding hand-reared precocious birds? a. b. c. d. C 9. What precautions need to be addressed when raising larval amphibians? a. b. c. keeping them at a constant temperature avoid changing their water assort them by size so smaller ones are not eaten by larger ones keeping them on a normal night and day light regime d. A lack of proper feather development endocrine imbalance leading to gigantism inability to move properly indicated by sitting in one place development of bent legs and deformed wings 10. What is the main way zoos replace animals in their collection today? a. b. c. d. purchase from or trade with other zoos purchase from wild animal catchers purchase from game farms acquisitions from government agencies Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 60 CONCLUSION Breeding and reproduction is an important aspect of zookeeping. Many zoo species require special conditions and care in order to breed successfully in captivity. The information on animal breeding provides knowledge of how to access the International Species Information System that provides a registry for zoo species. Tips were provided on initial care for animals that need to be hand reared. A professional zookeeper needs to understand the important role breeding and reproduction play in maintaining the zoo population. Pregnant females require special handling and care in order to ensure healthy, live offspring. Animal Behavior and Breeding © Donald Pattie 2001 61