Core Vocabulary in a Mainstream Second Grade: A Case Study of Two AAC Users Baker, B. Abstract Changing vocabulary demands on AAC systems and their users in mainstream settings are a focus. Understanding the demands in terms of core and extended vocabulary can bring some clarity. This case study follows the progress, with language data, of two boys using speech-generating devices through kindergarten, first, and second grades. Introduction The distinction between core and extended vocabulary was first made for special populations in the groundbreaking article by Mein & O’Connor (1961). It was discovered that the speech of people with moderate to severe cognitive impairments, living in a residential center, could be divided into words of great frequency (core vocabulary) and words used infrequently (fringe or extended vocabulary). Such a distinction can be made across typically developed populations and clinical populations including augmented communicators. Core vocabulary is like the proverbial 80/20 rules so popular in self-help and business books. Twenty percent of our vocabulary (or even less) accounts for 80 percent (or more) of what we say. Eighty percent of the words we use never make up more than 20 percent of what we say. Banajee, et al. (2001) found that 96 percent of the total words uttered by typically developing toddlers were made up of 25 simple words. Words Percentage I 9.5 No 8.5 Yes/yea 7.6 my 5.8 the 5.2 want 5.0 is 4.9 it 4.9 that 4.9 a 4.6 go 4.4 mine 3.8 you 3.2 what 3.1 on 2.8 in 2.7 here 2.7 more 2.6 out 2.4 off 2.3 some 2.3 help 2.1 all done/finished 1.0 96.3% Beukelman, Jones, & Rowan (1989) found the same patterns in the speech of typically developing pre-schoolers. In the present study of preschool children, the 50 most frequently occurring words represent approximately 60% of the total sample, while the 100 most frequently occurring words accounted for 73% of the total sample. Vanderheiden & Kelso (1988) found the same phenomenon in the elicited speech corpus of middle aged adults by Howes (1966). ... the 50 most frequently occurring words of a sample account for 40 to 50% of the total words communicated, while the 100 most frequently occurring words accounted for 60% of the total sample. Balandin & Iacono (1999) collected conversational samples from 34 nondisabled subjects at four worksites during meal breaks. They analyzed the total sample for core and fringe vocabulary and found that a small stable core of 347 words accounted for 78 percent of the total words (276,000) in the sample. Hill (2001) conducted 40 hours of interviews with 20 adult, self-taught augmented communicators, using speech generating devices (SGD). Data analysis indicates the 100 most frequently occurring words account for 62.3 percent of the total words used. The most frequently occurring 250 words comprised 78 percent of the total words used. Stuart, et al. (1997) identified the vocabulary use patterns of 65 to 74 year old and 75 to 85 year old adults. The percentage of the total sample represented by the 250 most common words was 78 percent for both groups. The importance of core vocabulary and its statistical domination of speech and prose are quite counter intuitive. When people think about their own speech and language, they tend to notice unusual words rather than the background tapestry of core words. If one asks colleagues what words they have said before breakfast, they will reflect upon the unusual words not upon the simple, common words. They may say, “Toaster.” “I had to ask my spouse where the new toaster was.” Or, he or she will begin naming nouns like “toothbrush,” “toothpaste.” When the questioner asks whether they thought about toothbrush, or actually said it, the answer is usually that the words went unsaid. The next question is: “Did you say “of”? The reply is, “Yes, but I can’t remember, nor do I remember how many times.” Thus people have a skewed perception of the words they use. The special, the unusual become salient. The common remains invisible. To help us get a feel for how the dominance of core vocabulary actually works, examples will make the data come alive. What follows are five samples of childhood speech. The ages range from two years, six months to over six years. In the following data, bolding (blue) indicates core words from the Banajee 26-word toddler vocabulary, while italics (red) indicates core words from the Marvin 333-word preschool vocabulary. Bill (2 yr. 6 mo.) Want that Go down Me do it Truck Go Uhoh, go down Want that one I see it It fall What that He fall Crash Big one Go up I do More go down My car Oh yeah No more Gimme it He crash it Broken I need it You go down TD - Tim (3 yr. 3 mo.) I put it on I gonna put it up here You do it Up there Gimme it I put it up there No, you don’t put it there You have it Now you put it up I did it I put it way up Where this go? This one a cow It can’t go It go right here He’s a horse Not here Quack a duck It fits I put it right here Where this one go? He goes here You do it Not there Jump I saw this book They crawl Wow Those are funny They look like grapes They could That was a good story Like if you think he runned away Grasshopper starts with a g” My favorite color is green Make ‘em ride it Yeah but . . . I can’t sit there cause I talk too much Me and Jennifer are friends.. I have to do this one again When I’m done I’m gonna go outside Are you gonna go outside? I know that one Where is it? This one is hard to do cause it’s so messy I got it You have three more to do You want this? There’s more over there TD - Rosey (4yr. 8 mo.) I don’t see it I do I saw a butterfly today They’re scared They might get hurt Ooh, what is that? They’re like that tiny I can see them TD - Sylvia (5 yr. 2 mo.) Right here next to me Did they move seats? I need to get my pencil Are you going to sit there? Why is she going to sit there? The central thesis of this paper has three parts. The first is that core vocabulary dominates everyday speech. The second is almost all concepts can be simply expressed using core vocabulary, and the third is that we can avoid the need to build pages endlessly for academic words by teaching students to express academic concepts using core vocabulary. The implications of these three notions have enormous importance in augmentative communication. If students can use core vocabulary in academic settings, then the speech therapist’s job can become that of teaching core vocabulary rather than adding specialized vocabulary. For the past two years, this process has been followed for second grade twin boys (Joshua H. and Caleb H.) with cerebral palsy in the public schools of Pennsylvania. The results of this educational process predict that what works for these boys will work for others. The road has been a bit bumpy. Quite a few common but not high frequency words have been added to the core vocabulary. These words include animal names, activity names, storybook characters and features, teachers’ names, clothes, school subjects, body parts, and disability equipment. Teachers have sometimes insisted that certain non-core technical words be added when a clear and easy circumlocution was available, e.g., patriotism (love my country/where I live), vote (tell who I want), mayor (person in charge of city), germinate (wake up and grow). At other times, academic words have been added, because they were deemed to be good parts of a permanent vocabulary, e.g., north, south, east, west, tax, law, solid, liquid, gas. 1. Joshua- 3-29-06 (Josh wrote the story to go along with existing pictures and title “What can Kim do?”) Kim can play music in the house. Mrs. Dartnell, Can I have the dress and shoes and hat? She is playing with the shoes and box. Kim plays dressup. I am snow white. I am a queen. Dad says hi. It’s nice to meet you. 2. Caleb 5-06 (Caleb wrote this story to go along with existing pictures and title: “Tom turkey gets wet.” -- animals are standing at a bus stop in the rain) Good morning. Hi Tom. How are you? I’m fine. I’m not so good. Kelsey, do you have a nickel? The turtle has a nickel. The tiger does not have a nickel. The turkey does not have a nickel. Put oodles of money in the pocket. He will be rich. (note: he found oodles via word prediction, shouting a big “yes” when he got his word! You will also note that he does not yet use possessive pronouns) The bird gets angry. He is wet. (a dog shakes water all over him in the picture.) 3. Caleb3-20-06 (another story to go with pictures. Here you can see the initial idea and the editing process to get the sentence. Title: who wears a hat? Caleb gets fired up about writing and wants to get his thoughts out quickly, even if jumbled. Then he goes back and fixes the sentences, because he knows that they don’t sound right. Occasionally, the staff gets too involved and “directs” the editing a lot, but I really don’t think that happened much in this story.) Cake sing to girl happy birthday. They sing happy birthday to the girl. Baseball. Boys and girls are playing baseball. Swimming boy. They are swimming in the pool. I wish I was playing at the beach. (I have a feeling this sentence was prompted by the staff!) The boy eat. The boy is cooking. He wants to eat. He horse jumping. (an attempt at possessive pronouns?) The boy is jumping on the horse. The girl in the power chair sings to the boy. Mrs. H. and Katya Hill, Ph.D., University of Pittsburgh are collaborating in the full analysis of Joshua and Caleb’s language production through meticulously maintained documentation using the Language Activity Monitor (LAM).* They have been looking at utterance generation methods (spontaneous novel utterance generation or pre-stored sentences), language representation methods (single meaning pictures, spelling, semantic compaction), and vocabulary frequency. These data are being analyzed for mean length of utterance (MLU), both word and morpheme, grammatical morphemes, syntax (SALT). This material is still in manuscript form. The teaching staff have slowly adapted to asking questions that can be answered descriptively rather than referentially for the boys. The school speech pathologist is enthusiastic and plans to be adopting the core vocabulary approach for some of her other clients. Both boys have a diagnosis of cerebral palsy and moderate cognitive impairment. Preliminary analyses of the data show increase in core vocabulary frequency as well as increased frequency in use of the communication aid outside school environments. The boys exhibit a strong preference for novel utterance generation. Their MLUs have grown significantly as has their overall communication rate on the devices and their use of vocalizations. References Balandin, Susan & Iacono, Teresa. (1999) Adolescent and Young Adult Vocabulary Usage, Augmentative and Alternative Communication (AAC), Volume 14, No. 3, September. Banajee, Meher, Dicarlo, C., & Stricklin, S. B. (2003). Core Vocabulary Determination for toddlers. Augmentative and Alternative Communication (AAC), 19, 67-73. Sheela Stuart, David R. Beukelman, and Julia King, Vocabulary Use during Extended Conversations by Two Cohorts of Older Adults AAC Augmentative and Alternative Communication, Volume 13, 40-47, March, 1997