The third meeting of the Verona Network on Sequence Analysis Finding common grounds in defining patient cues and concerns and the appropriateness of provider responses Verona, 4th – 5th February 2005 Lidia Del Piccolo, Claudia Goss & Christa Zimmermann Department of Medicine and Public Health, Section of Psychiatry and Clinical Psychology, University of Verona, 37134 Verona, Italy. Corresponding address: Tel +39 - 045 8076412 Fax +39 - 045 585871 On 4th – 5th February this year twenty researchers from seven different countries (England, The Netherlands, Norway, Switzerland, Germany, Italy and USA) met in Verona to attend the third invitational workshop of the “Verona Network on Sequence Analysis”, hosted by the University of Verona, and organized by Christa Zimmermann and her colleagues. Differently from the previous two meetings (see PEC 50, 107-109 (2003) and PEC 53, 395-396 (2004)) this year’s attention was focused on finding a common ground on the definition of cue and concern and on what to consider an appropriate provider response to these patient expressions. Include picture here Why finding a consensus on cue concern definition. Cues and concerns refer to the emotional aspect of patient-provider communication and they represent one of the most challenging topics for the interaction analyses of medical consultations. Indeed the most frequently analyzed patient-provider sequences in the studies presented at the 2004 Workshop on sequence analysis in Verona regarded patient cues or concerns and the subsequent provider responses. Despite there seemed to be an unspoken common understanding among the participants on what a cue or a concern is, the adopted definitions applied divergent and sometimes conflicting criteria to define these two phenomena. Attempts to compare results from different studies became quite difficult and the participants decided in the closure session of the 2004 workshop to launch a comparative coding exercise, based on a sample of six transcribed interviews in English language. The aim was to identify patient cues and/or concerns according to the respective classification systems adopted. Twelve colleagues, each familiar with one of eleven classification methodologies, provided the codings: Lidia Del Piccolo (Verona Medical Interview Classification System (VR-MICS), Young-Mi Kim (Provider Responsiveness to Client Active Communication), Linda Zandbelt (Patient-centred Behaviour Coding Instrument (PBCI), Johanna Ruusavuori (Conversation Analysis), Ian Fletcher and Cathy Heaven (Medical Interview AURAL Rating System (MIARS), Phyllis Butow (Psychological distress in Oncology classification,) Sandra van Dulmen and Wolfgang Langewitz (Roter Interaction Analysis System (RIAS), Arnstein Finset (Empathic Opportunities), Ludwien Meeuwesen (a combination of RIAS and the Verbal Response Mode (RIAS/VRM) and Greg Makoul (Empathic Communication Coding System (ECCS). The results of the comparative coding exercise showed how differently sensitive and specific the different systems were in picking out what was considered cue/concern. A revision and the attempt to find common ground was felt as urgent and the agenda of the 2005 workshop was dedicated to this enterprise. Arnstein Finset and Christa Zimmermann chaired the workshop. After a welcome by Christa Zimmermann, Arnstein Finset gave a short introduction and a summary of the results of the comparative coding exercise and nine participants exposed briefly the cue/concern definitions of the adopted systems. Then three work sessions, each followed by a plenary session, took place involving four parallel work groups, with changing members at each session. Session 1: consensus finding on cue/concern definition (Rapporteurs Young-Mi Kim and Hanneke de Haes) The aim of the first parallel work session for each of the four subgroups was to arrive at a definition of cue/concern and the criteria that would allow their identification by analyzing two transcribed interviews chosen from the comparative coding exercise. In the subsequent plenary session the four group outcomes were compared and discussed. Some consensus criteria for the definition of cues and concerns emerged, although no agreement on a single definition was reached. A cue was roughly defined as a hint, which might be an expression or signal, mostly verbal but also non verbal (particularly emotionally charged expressions such as crying), which indirectly indicates an issue of presumed importance (unresolved, current, relevant?) for the patient and implies an emotion, worry or uncertainty that the patient would like to bring up, or a move to another topic, that should demand an exploration from the provider. A concern was defined as a verbal expression, which explicitly indicates an issue of importance for the patient. The main discussion points were: The degree of importance to the patient All agreed that cues and concerns should be something of importance to the patient that deserved health carers’ attention. The importance of a cue for the patient would not always be evident. There was also a discussion about relevance and urgency as criterion but no conclusions were drawn. The degree of explicitness All agreed that concern is a more explicit and cue a more implicit expression. A cue is a hint, signal or trigger of a patient’s concern and tends to be ambiguous or incomplete, whereas concerns tend to be unambiguously or clearly stated. Emotional expression or content After a long discussion participants did not come to an agreement on whether cues and concerns have to include words expressing feelings. All agreed that expressions about a sensory aspect (“It hurts”) should not be considered a concern. Patient initiated or doctor elicited All agreed that the definitions should include all patient initiated cues and concerns. However, there was no agreement on whether cues and concerns that are elicited by the doctor should also be included. Possibility to introduce a new definition “psych” This referred to expressions of intense emotional distress without a stated issue of importance, which is not linked to a particular topic of the consultation and cannot be defined as either cue or concern, but is a category of its own. There were also suggestions to make efforts to create definitions that were not overly dependent on subtle connotations of a specific word in a particular language – definitions have to be freely translatable among the various languages of the group. Session 2: application of consensus criteria of cue and concern definition (Rapporteur Linda Zandbelt) The aim of the second parallel work session was to verify the applicability of the consensus-defined criteria in identifying cues/concerns on two other interview transcripts. Each group had to identify cues/concerns according to the consensus criteria defined during the previous plenary session, and to signal those expressions where doubts emerged, and the reasons of uncertainty. The groups then compared their results in the plenary session and revised the consensus criteria defined during the first parallel session. The main points of discussion were: Content and emotion A first question was how to combine the two different aspects -emotion and content- that contribute to define a cue or a concern. There could be different levels of explicitness of an emotion (no emotion, implicit or explicit emotion) and specificity of a content (unspecified issue and specified issue). The combination of the different levels of these two dimensions may contribute to clarify what a cue or a concern is. A concern should have both, a content and an expression of emotion. Another topic of discussion, related to these two levels, was the issue of their priority (is the emotion more important than the content?). Consequently, a new category was suggested and called “explicit emotion”, referring to an emotion expressed without a specified issue of importance e.g. “I’m quite anxious”. Another debate emerged about how to intend words such as “tense”, “tired” or “relaxed”. Are they emotional” or not? Patient or doctor initiated It was agreed that both cues and concerns might be doctor or patient initiated and it was therefore suggested to add the coding “d” and “p” to differentiate them Repeated concerns and cues The question was whether repeatedly expressed cues or concerns should be coded each time they occurred. It was agreed to code a given topic only once when repeated within a turn, to give separate codes to new topics or emotion arising within a turn, but also to topics, which the patient is clearly presenting again after a doctor’s non-response. A suggestion was also to code as “elaboration of previous concern” when the patient continued to express the same concern in a new turn and was facilitated in doing so by the doctor. The difficulty was mentioned to code on transcripts since non verbal aspects, like intonation of voice, could make a big difference when deciding for example to code “I’m so tired” as concern or not. Session 3: definition of appropriateness of provider responses (Rapporteurs Sandra van Dulmen and Helge Skirbekk) The aim for each group was to indicate the appropriateness of provider responses to cues and concerns, which had been selected in the two transcripts of the second session and on which a consensus had emerged in the second plenary session. During this evaluation task, the group had to define the criteria for appropriateness and inappropriateness on the basis of a ranking where: –1 was inappropriate; 0 was neutral/non applicable and 1 was the appropriate response. Each group was also asked to formulate alternative appropriate expressions when absent. In the final plenary discussion group findings were compared with the attempt to set common criteria for appropriateness on the basis of provider responses identified as correct by most participants. The feedback from the four subgroups reflected a broad variety of issues related to the definition of what constitute appropriate provider responses to patient cues and concerns. At the end, although most of participants concurred which were the appropriate/inappropriate responses, no common definition on what to consider appropriate emerged and therefore clear rules were not established. The main points of discussion were: Coding appropriateness Three problems were raised: to consider utterances or turns; to incorporate or not the subsequent provider expression in defining the former as appropriate or not, and if to take into account patient’s reaction to provider’s expression. Another argument was the role of silence, (when it is appropriate) and the difficulty to recognize the appropriateness on transcripts without the support of non-verbal information. The aim, which the provider pursues in the consultation, should be taken into account when defining appropriateness. There was full agreement that minimizing, normalizing or avoiding the patient’s problem were inappropriate responses. The “neutral” category This category should be used when it is not clear what the doctor said or when it is ambiguous Individual factors The belief system of the rater determines what s/he considers appropriate or not. Individual differences between patients may ask for different responses. Changing topic It was not clearly stated whether a change of topic or an interruption was to consider always as inappropriate. Moving away from the present topic against the patient’s wishes is clearly inappropriate. An interruption can be acceptable if the intent is to clarify or is a return to the most important matter. As a help in deciding if a change of topic was appropriate it was proposed to observe patient’s subsequent response. Responding to an emotion An educated guess can be appropriate when the provider thereby moves to the heart of the matter. The concomitant non-verbal communication is also very important in handling patient’s emotions appropriately. Difficulty in applying rules Sometimes the rater could judge the expression as clinically inappropriate but following strictly the rules such expression would have to be coded as appropriate. A similar issue regarded the observation that a provider may return to a previously discussed topic which may be appropriate for that patient, but this provider expression will be coded as inappropriate taken alone as it is. Session 4: closure of the meeting Jozien Bensing chaired the closure of the meeting on Saturday afternoon. She appreciated the collaborative atmosphere of the meeting and encouraged future collaborations by suggesting, in continuity with this year’s workshop, some stimulating research initiatives, for example the development of a common approach in coding non-verbal cues by observing videotapes of interviews, and to examine such cues in relation to concomitant verbal expressions and subsequent verbal and non verbal provider responses. In synthesis, the two days workshop has been intensive; the discussions were very productive and raised many questions that left some of the main issues related to the definitions of cue/concern and appropriateness unresolved. For this reason the participants delegated a small group of participants to reconsider the conceptual framework and to prepare a draft of a core definition of cue and concern, that could conciliate the main aspects which had emerged in the first and second parallel sessions. This draft would be sent to all participants for feedback, in order to refine a final agreement. Such an ultimate “filtered “definition could then be used for another coding exercise or for a collaborative reliability study. The discussion on appropriateness raised much more questions than answers. A main argument regarded the relationship between the judgment of an appropriate response to cue/concern and the phase of the medical consultation in which this response occurred. From this point of view the concept of appropriateness seems then strictly related to what is the ideal model of a medical consultation. The conclusion was that the issue of appropriateness needed to be reconsidered more thoroughly and therefore might be one possible topic for the fourth meeting of the Verona Network on Sequence Analysis in 2006.