Akers_EJN_2006 - University of New Mexico

advertisement
The definitive version of this manuscript is available at
http://www.blackwell-synergy.com/doi/abs/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04921.x
Early life modulators and predictors of adult synaptic plasticity.
Katherine G. Akers1, Masato Nakazawa1, Russell D. Romeo3, John A. Connor2, Bruce S.
McEwen3, & Akaysha C. Tang1,2*
1
Department of Psychology
Department of Neurosciences
University of New Mexico
Albuquerque, NM 87131
2
3
Laboratory of Neuroendocrinology
Rockefeller University
New York, NY 10021
*Corresponding author:
Akaysha C. Tang
MSC032220
Department of Psychology
1 University of New Mexico
Albuquerque, NM 87131
Tel: (505) 277-4025
Cell: (505) 610-9077
Fax: (505) 277-1394
E-mail: akaysha@unm.edu
Keywords: neonatal handling; hypoxia; open field; rat; social recognition memory.
Early experience and synaptic plasticity
Akers et al.
2
ABSTRACT
Early life experience can induce long-lasting changes in brain and behavior that are opposite in
direction, such as enhancement or impairment in regulation of stress response, structural and
functional integrity of the hippocampus, and learning and memory. To explore how multiple
early life events jointly determine developmental outcome, we investigated the combined effects
of neonatal trauma (anoxia on postnatal day 1) and neonatal novelty exposure (postnatal day 221) on adult social recognition memory (3 months of age) and synaptic plasticity in the CA1 of
the rat hippocampus (4.5-8 months of age). While neonatal anoxia selectively reduced posttetanic potentiation (PTP), neonatal novel exposure selectively increased long-term potentiation
(LTP). No interaction between anoxia and novelty exposure was found on either PTP or LTP.
These findings suggest that the two contrasting neonatal events have selective and distinct effects
on two different forms of synaptic plasticity. At the level of behavior, the effect of novelty
exposure on LTP was associated with increased social memory, and the effect of anoxia on PTP
was not accompanied by changes in social memory. Such a finding suggests a bias toward the
involvement of LTP over PTP in social memory. Finally, we report a surprising finding that an
early behavioral measure of emotional response to a novel environment obtained at 25 days of
age can predict adult synaptic plasticity measured several months later. Therefore, individual
differences in emotional responses present during the juvenile stage may contribute to adult
individual differences in cellular mechanisms that underlie learning and memory.
Early experience and synaptic plasticity
Akers et al.
3
INTRODUCTION
Early life experience can have detrimental or beneficial impact on individual brain and
behavioral development (Harlow, 1959; Levine, 1960; Bowlby, 1962; Denenberg, 1964; Rutter,
1972; Meaney et al., 1996; Chapillon et al., 2002; Pryce & Feldon, 2003). Early aversive events
such as prolonged maternal separation or disrupted maternal care impair hypothalamic-pituitaryadrenal (HPA) axis function (e.g. Ladd et al., 2004; Knuth & Etgen, 2005; Neumann et al.,
2005) and reduce hippocampal synaptic plasticity (Brunson et al., 2005) and memory function
(e.g. Lehmann et al., 1999; Huot et al., 2002; Sandstrom & Hart, 2005). In contrast, mild early
stimulation such as neonatal handling, brief isolation, or increased maternal care enhance HPA
function (e.g. Francis et al., 1999; Macri et al., 2004; Panagiotaropoulos et al., 2004),
hippocampal synaptic plasticity (Kehoe & Bronzino, 1999), and memory function (e.g. Aguilar
et al., 2002; Lehmann et al., 2002; Garoflos et al., 2005). A growing body of evidence further
indicates that early aversive events and mild stimulation interact to determine the outcome of
development (Wakshlak & Weinstock, 1990; Escorihuela et al., 1994; Maccari et al., 1995;
Francis et al., 2002; Morley-Fletcher et al., 2003; Bredy et al., 2004; Hellemans et al., 2004).
One specific early aversive event that has a profound impact on development is neonatal
anoxia, a form of birth trauma that increases hyperactivity in the open field during juvenility (e.g.
Nyakas et al., 1991; Iuvone et al., 1996), impairs performance in various memory tasks during
adulthood (e.g. Buwalda et al., 1995; Mishima et al., 2005), and increases synaptic plasticity
immediately or shortly after the trauma (Jensen et al., 1998; Zhang et al., 2005) with long-term
effects on synaptic plasticity unknown. In contrast to these effects of neonatal anoxia, an early
mild stimulation paradigm called neonatal novelty exposure decreases behavioral reactivity in
the open field (Tang, 2001), enhances performance in memory tasks (Tang, 2001; Tang et al.,
Early experience and synaptic plasticity
Akers et al.
4
2003), and increases synaptic plasticity (Tang & Zou, 2002) long after the initial neonatal
stimulations. Neonatal anoxia and neonatal novelty exposure, therefore, appear to produce
opposite effects, at least on measures of open field behavior and memory. Such opposing actions
have been confirmed for the open field measure in a recent study (Tang & Nakazawa, 2005).
Given that the two early events have opposing effects on memory when studied
separately, we hypothesize that anoxia may lead to long-term deficits in both memory and
synaptic plasticity, which may underlie learning and memory (Bliss & Collingridge, 1993;
Stevens, 1998), and that these potential detrimental effects might be reversible by subsequent
neonatal novelty exposure. Taking advantage of the fact that the present study was conducted
using the same animals whose open field behaviors were previously characterized during the
juvenile stage (Tang & Nakazawa, 2005), we test another hypothesis that open field activity
early in life may serve as a long range marker for adult synaptic plasticity.
METHODS AND MATERIALS
Animals. All procedures were in accordance with the Institutional Animal Care and Use
Committee at the University of New Mexico. Eleven pregnant Long-Evans hooded dams
(Charles River, Raleigh, NC) arrived at the vivarium 16 days prior to giving birth. Litter size at
birth ranged from 8 to 18 pups. Pups were housed with their dams until weaning on postnatal
day 22. Thereafter, rats were housed individually in plastic cages (51 x 25 x 22 cm) with a 12-h
light/dark cycle (lights on at 7:00 am) and food and water ad libitum.
Experimental design. A 2 x 2 factorial design was used with neonatal anoxia (Anoxia
vs. Control) as a between-litter factor and neonatal novelty exposure (Novel vs. Home) as a
within-litter factor. Specifically, entire litters were randomly assigned to either the Control or
the Anoxic condition, and within each litter approximately half of the pups were assigned to the
Early experience and synaptic plasticity
Akers et al.
5
Novel group and the other half to the Home group. The same groups of animals were tested in
the open field, a social recognition test, and synaptic plasticity experiment in the order shown in
Fig. 1. The social recognition test was chosen as a memory test because we were interested in
the potential involvement of the hippocampus in social memory. The order each litter
experienced anoxia treatment, group membership marking, and neonatal novelty exposure was
counterbalanced among the four experimental conditions. Furthermore, within a specific test,
the order in which each rat was assessed was also counterbalanced. All testing was performed
by experimenters who were blind to the animals’ group identity.
Neonatal anoxia and neonatal novelty exposure. On postnatal day 1, after the dam was
removed from the home cage and placed into a holding cage, pups were transferred from the
home cage to a plastic airtight chamber (25 x 20 x 13 cm) equipped with an air inlet and outlet.
Pups were exposed for 25 min to a continuous flow of either 100% nitrogen gas (Anoxic
condition) or room air (21% O2/79% N2, Control condition) at a flow rate of 3 liters/min. The
chamber temperature was held at 31° C by partial immersion in water heated by a submersible
heater (Whisper, Tetra, VA). Immediately after anoxia treatment, litters were culled to 8-10
pups. Next, pups were pseudorandomly assigned to Novel and Home groups and group
membership was marked by tattooing the ventral surface of the hind paws before pups were
returned to the home cage.
Neonatal novelty exposure (Tang, 2001) was performed daily from P2-21 by an
experimenter blind to the Anoxic versus Control litter conditions. First, the dam was removed
from the home cage and placed into a separate holding cage. Next, the Novel pups were
identified and individually transferred to small non-home cages lined with fresh sawdust. After
spending 3 min in the non-home cages, Novel pups were returned to the home cage in which the
Early experience and synaptic plasticity
Akers et al.
6
Home pups remained. After the Novel and Home pups were reunited, the dam was returned to
the home cage. During the pup transfer, every time a Novel pup was touched, a yoked Home
pup was touched in a similar manner and for a similar duration. Thus, both experimenter contact
and maternal separation were matched between Novel and Home groups. This ensured that the
only difference in treatment was the brief, repeated exposures to a novel environment. Detailed
description of these behavioral manipulations can be found elsewhere (Tang & Nakazawa,
2005).
Open field testing. Initial open field activity was measured when rats were in the
juvenile stage (P25), a time in development at which perinatal anoxia/hypoxia-induced changes
in open field behavior are particularly apparent (e.g. Dell'Anna et al., 1991; Nyakas et al., 1991;
Iuvone et al., 1996). Specifically, rats (N = 74 rats) were tested in a novel open field (60 x 60 x
60 cm) during four 20-s trials with an intertrial interval of ~5 min. This short trial duration was
chosen to capture individual differences in the initial response upon entering a novel
environment rather than to obtain a more prolonged activity measure. Initial activity was
measured by the average number of squares crossed across the four trials. Novelty and Anoxia
treatment effects on this open field activity measure, as well as additional details of open field
testing, have been documented in a previous publication (Tang & Nakazawa, 2005).
Social recognition memory testing. On P100-101, habituation of social investigative
behavior was measured within Novel-Home pairs of rats over four 5-min sessions on two
consecutive days (day 1: S1-S3; day 2: S1). Pairs of rats (N = 24 pairs) were formed from the
same Anoxic vs. Control condition, matched in weight, and non-siblings. To allow group
identification, rats were marked on both sides of the body with red or green food coloring, and
colors were counterbalanced among experimental conditions. Social sessions occurred in a
Early experience and synaptic plasticity
Akers et al.
7
neutral testing cage. On each day, social sessions were preceded by a 5-min cage habituation
(Hab) session, during which rats were separated from each other by a partition placed in the
middle of the cage. During inter-session intervals (10 min between S1 and S2; 2 min between S2
and S3), rats were returned to their home cages located in the testing room. Social sessions were
videotaped for offline analysis. Social investigative behavior was defined as being oriented
toward (tip of nose within ~1 cm) or in direct contact with the conspecific while sniffing or
grooming. If social investigation was present at any time during a 5-s interval, an occurrence of
one was counted. As a large percentage of social investigation was mutual (occurring
simultaneously in both rats), only unidirectional investigation was included in the analysis.
Long-term memory was assessed by a long-term habituation (LTH) score, defined as (S1–
D2S1)/S1*100. Additional details of social recognition testing can be found elsewhere (Tang et
al., 2003).
Electrophysiology. In vitro brain slice experiments were performed when rats were
between 4.5-8 months of age (N = 21 rats). After rats were deeply anesthetized with halothane
and decapitated, the brain was quickly extracted and immediately placed into ice-cold cutting
solution (in mM: 220 sucrose, 3 KCl, 6 MgSO4, 0.2 CaCl2, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 26 NaHCO3, 10
glucose, 0.43 ketamine) that was oxygenated with 95% O2/5% CO2. Because neonatal novelty
exposure preferentially affects hippocampal synaptic plasticity in the right hemisphere (Tang,
2003), only the right hippocampus was used. The hippocampus was sliced (350μm transverse
slices) using a Vibroslice (Campden Instruments) and incubated in oxygenated artificial
cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF, in mM: 126 NaCl, 3 KCl, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 26 NaHCO3, 1 MgSO4, 10
glucose, 2 CaCl2) at 34° C for one hour. After incubation, slices were held at room temperature
until recording. All recordings were made within 10 hours of decapitation.
Early experience and synaptic plasticity
Akers et al.
8
During recording, slices were submerged in a recording chamber and continuously
perfused (2 ml/min) with oxygenated ACSF at 34° C. Field excitatory postsynaptic potentials
(EPSPs) were recorded from the CA1 stratum radiatum using glass microelectrodes (2-3 MΩ)
filled with ACSF. Evoked EPSPs were elicited by stimulating the Schaeffer collateral fibers
with a bipolar electrode. Test pulses (70 μs duration, 0.1 Hz) were set at an intensity that evoked
50% of maximum EPSP amplitude before intrusion of a population spike (0.2-0.3 mA). LTP
was induced by a high frequency stimulation (HFS) protocol consisting of 3 trains of 100 test
pulses delivered at 100 Hz with a 1-min interval between trains, an induction protocol optimized
for both early and delayed LTP (Behnisch & Reymann, 1995; Wilsch et al., 1998). Note that test
pulses were given at the same rate during the 1-min intervals between the HFS trains (6 test
pulses between each train) to allow measures of PTP.
Signals were amplified 2000 times (NeuroData IR283, Cygnus Technology, Inc.), filtered
with a bandpass filter of 1 Hz-5 kHz, and digitized at a rate of 5 kHz (Digidata 1322A, Axon
Instruments). Data were collected and analyzed using pCLAMP 8.1 software (Axon
Instruments). EPSP initial slope was averaged over six successive traces to obtain minute-byminute measures for PTP and LTP. PTP and LTP were defined as the percentage change from
baseline for the minutes immediately after each of the 3 HFS trains and for the last 10 minutes of
the 30-min post-HFS recording period, respectively.
Corticosterone assay. Neonatal novelty exposure was previously found to lower basal
levels of corticosterone (CORT) (Tang et al., 2003), a stress hormone known to affect synaptic
plasticity (De Kloet et al., 1999; McEwen, 1999). Because blood samples were easily collected
at the time of sacrifice immediately before LTP experiments, we also measured basal CORT
concentration from the blood serum. Animals were sacrificed at approximately 10:00am. Blood
Early experience and synaptic plasticity
Akers et al.
9
samples were collected in EDTA-coated tubes and centrifuged. Serum was removed and stored
at -20° C until radioimmunoassay was performed. Serum CORT concentrations were measured
in duplicate in a single assay using a Coat-A-Count Corticosterone Kit (Diagnostic Products, Los
Angeles, CA). The lower limit of detectibility was 10.90 ng/ml, and the intra-assay coefficient
of variation was 5.2%.
Statistical analysis. For statistical testing of synaptic plasticity measures, animal was the
unit of analysis (i.e. multiple slice measures from each animal were averaged together). For
analysis of PTP, repeated measures ANOVA was applied to the first 3 post-baseline minutes of
data with HFS Train as a within-subject factor and Novelty and Anoxia as between-subject
factors. For analysis of LTP, ANOVA was applied to the average of the last 10 minutes of the
30-min post-HFS recording period with Novelty and Anoxia as between-subject factors. For
analysis of the social recognition memory measures, a pair of rats was the unit of analysis, with
Novelty as a within-pair factor and Anoxia as a between-pair factor. Pearson’s correlation
coefficient was used to correlate synaptic plasticity measures with a previously reported measure
of open field activity obtained from the same rats (Tang & Nakazawa, 2005). To test whether
correlations differed among treatment groups, we used univariate ANCOVA with Novelty and
Anoxia as between-subject factors and Open Field Activity as a covariate. For analysis of stress
hormone measures, univariate ANOVA was used with Novelty and Anoxia as between-subject
factors. Outliers greater than 1.5 times the interquartile range were removed from analysis
(Agresti & Finlay, 1997). Based on prior experience (Tang & Zou, 2002; Tang et al., 2003),
directional tests were used to test Novelty effects. Effect size was expressed as f and d for Ftests and t-tests, respectively (f: large: > 0.4, medium: > 0.25, small: < 0.1; d: large: > 0.8,
medium: > 0.5, small: < 0.2) (Rosenthal & Rosnow, 1984).
Early experience and synaptic plasticity
Akers et al.
10
RESULTS
Effects of neonatal anoxia and neonatal novelty exposure on synaptic plasticity.
Approximately 4.5-8 months after the neonatal anoxia treatment and subsequent novelty
exposures, synaptic plasticity was examined in hippocampal slices from adult rats using a LTP
induction protocol comprised of 3 HFS trains. EPSP slope prior to HFS did not differ among
Novelty and Anoxic conditions (p > 0.2; Table 1; representative traces shown in Fig. 3ac). The 3
HFS trains produced successively greater increases in PTP of EPSP slope (Fig. 2). Temporal
patterns across the 3 HFS trains revealed that the rate of PTP increase was lower in Anoxic than
in Control rats (F(2,32) = 4.45, p = 0.020, f = 0.53; Fig. 2 inset). This PTP difference between
Anoxia and Control rats increased across the 3 HFS trains, with pair-wise comparisons showing
a significant difference after the 3rd train (t(18) = 2.25, p = 0.037, d = 0.99; Fig. 3b). The Novelty
effect and Novelty by Anoxia interaction effect on PTP were not significant (p > 0.2).
A different pattern of treatment effects was found for LTP (Fig. 2). Overall, LTP in
Novel rats was significantly greater than in Home rats (F(1,17) = 3.65, p = 0.037, f = 0.46).
Although the Novelty by Anoxia interaction effect on LTP did not reach statistical significance
(p > 0.2), pair-wise comparisons revealed that it was only within the Control condition that
Novel rats had greater LTP than Home rats (t(7) = 1.92, p = 0.048, d = 1.30, Fig. 3d-left), with
no difference found between Novel and Home rats within the Anoxic condition (p > 0.2; Fig. 3dright). The effect of Anoxia on LTP was not significant (p > 0.2). This lack of Anoxia effect
cannot be attributed to a general failure in the anoxia treatment, which clearly affected both PTP
(present study), body weight and open field activity (Tang & Nakazawa, 2005).
Effects of neonatal anoxia and neonatal novelty exposure on social memory. Social
recognition memory testing was conducted approximately 1.5-5 months prior to synaptic
Early experience and synaptic plasticity
Akers et al.
11
plasticity experiments. Raw frequencies of social investigation during each social session and
decreases in investigation across sessions (habituation) for all four treatment groups are shown in
Fig. 4abcd. Novel rats exhibited greater LTH than Home rats (F(1,20) = 10.96, p = 0.002, f =
0.74, Fig. 4e), which replicates previous findings obtained from rats in the absence of neonatal
trauma (Tang et al., 2003). Although the Novelty by Anoxia interaction effect on LTH did not
reach statistical significance (p > 0.2), pair-wise comparisons revealed that it was only within the
Control condition that Novel rats had greater LTH than Home rats (t(9) = 2.05, p = 0.035, d =
1.44, Fig. 4e-left), with no difference found between Novel and Home rats within the Anoxic
condition (p = 0.189, Fig. 4e-right). Thus, the pattern of treatment effects on LTH is similar to
that on LTP, in that neonatal novelty exposure increased the dependent measure only in Control
but not in Anoxic rats (compare pattern of treatment effects in Fig. 4e with that in Fig. 3d).
Correlations between LTH and measures of LTP and PTP were not significant (p > 0.2)
Relationship between early life response to a novel environment and adult synaptic
plasticity. We measured early life response to a novel environment using ambulatory activity
during four 20-s exposures to an open field on P25. Detailed analysis and discussion of this open
field data were published elsewhere (Tang & Nakazawa, 2005). Here, we computed correlations
between average open field activity across the 4 trials and measures of adult synaptic plasticity.
A significant correlation between open field activity and LTP was found within Novel Anoxic
rats (r(5) = -0.830, p = 0.041); correlations within the other three treatment groups were not
significant (p > 0.1). Differences in correlations between treatment groups were tested using
ANCOVA. Novel and Home rats showed a significant difference in this correlation (F(1,13) =
5.02, p = 0.043, f = 0.62), with a large negative correlation found in Novel rats (r(10) = -0.802, p
= 0.003, Fig. 5a) but no correlation found in Home rats (r(9) = 0.052, p > 0.2, Fig. 5b). Anoxic
Early experience and synaptic plasticity
Akers et al.
12
and Control rats did not differ in this correlation (p > 0.2; Fig 5ab, Anoxic: filled squares,
Control: open triangles). Correlations between open field activity and PTP were not significant
in any of the four treatment groups, which showed no differences from each other (p > 0.2).
Basal CORT concentration did not differ among treatment groups (p > 0.2; in ng/ml:
Control Novel: 58.0±16.2, Control Home: 44.9±10.6, Anoxic Novel: 54.4±16.3, Anoxic Home:
62.4±13.6). This should not be taken as evidence that there was no difference in HPA function
among groups, as both neonatal trauma and mild neonatal stimulation are known to result in
changes in HPA function (e.g. Meaney et al., 1988; Boksa et al., 1996) that may be more readily
detected in evoked than in basal CORT measures. Furthermore, it has been shown that the effect
of early mild stimulation on basal CORT may not be expressed until animals have reached
senescence (Meaney et al., 1988; Tang et al., 2003). The correlations between basal CORT
concentration and PTP or LTP measures were not significant (p > 0.2).
DISCUSSION
We investigated the combined effects of neonatal anoxia and neonatal novelty exposure
on adult hippocampal synaptic plasticity (PTP and LTP) and social memory. We found that
anoxia selectively reduced PTP, and novelty exposure selectively increased LTP. No significant
interaction between anoxia and novelty exposure was found on either PTP or LTP. These
findings suggest that short- and long-term synaptic plasticity are selectively and differentially
modulated by neonatal trauma and neonatal mild stimulation, perhaps via separate cellular
mechanisms. Furthermore, we found that the novelty exposure-induced increase in LTP was
accompanied by an enhancement in social memory. In contrast, the anoxia-induced reduction in
PTP was not accompanied by a change in social memory. These findings therefore suggest a
bias toward the involvement of LTP over PTP in social memory. Finally, we evaluated open
Early experience and synaptic plasticity
Akers et al.
13
field activity as a predictor of adult synaptic plasticity. Among rats that experienced neonatal
novelty exposure, initial activity in a novel open field during juvenility correlated with LTP
during adulthood. Such a long-range correlation across developmental stages demonstrates that
emotional response to novelty early in life may serve as a potential behavioral marker for adult
synaptic plasticity.
Neonatal anoxia effect on PTP. We found that neonatal anoxia selectively affected
hippocampal PTP during adulthood. Specifically, anoxia reduced the rate of PTP over repeated
HFS trains, and this effect was present regardless of whether rats experienced neonatal novelty
exposure. That is, the anoxia effect was not reversed by subsequent novelty exposure. This
finding contrasts with previous reports that mild post-trauma stimulation effectively blocked the
impact of neonatal anoxia/hypoxia on open field activity, learning and memory, and
hippocampal structure (Iuvone et al., 1996; Chou et al., 2001; Rodrigues et al., 2004; Tang &
Nakazawa, 2005). Thus, it appears that mild stimulation may alleviate symptoms of neonatal
trauma at the levels of behavior and gross brain morphology but not at the level of synaptic
plasticity, perhaps due to the two early events having selective effects on distinct mechanisms
underlying two different forms of synaptic plasticity (PTP and LTP). Because PTP is
presynaptic in origin (Zucker & Regehr, 2002), the decreased PTP in Anoxic rats suggests that
neonatal anoxia may produce an enduring impairment in some presynaptic mechanism
underlying short-term plasticity, such as Ca2+ influx or activity of Ca2+-binding proteins. The
inability of novelty exposure to counteract the effect of neonatal anoxia on PTP may be
explained by a selective effect of this mild stimulation on mechanisms specific to LTP that are
not involved in PTP. Further studies are required to elucidate the precise mechanisms by which
Early experience and synaptic plasticity
Akers et al.
14
neonatal anoxia and novelty exposure exert their selective effects on short- and long-term
synaptic plasticity.
A selective PTP deficit may have consequences for learning and memory. For example,
mice lacking the presynaptic protein synapsin II showed a selective reduction in PTP but not
LTP (Rosahl et al., 1995; Spillane et al., 1995). Despite the presence of normal LTP, these mice
displayed no evidence of contextual fear conditioning following training, suggesting that PTP
plays a role in learning and memory in this task independently of LTP (Silva et al., 1996). In
another study, rats with poor performance in a water maze task exhibited less PTP than higherperforming rats, again despite having comparable LTP (Diana et al., 1994). The fact that the
anoxia-induced PTP reduction in the present study was not accompanied by a decrease in social
recognition memory suggests that a PTP-memory connection may be specific to test situations
involving explicit stress, as in the case of water maze and fear conditioning tasks but not in tests
involving brief interactions with a conspecific. Alternatively, it is possible that the lack of PTPsocial memory connection is specific to the induction protocol used in the present study.
Nevertheless, the finding that neonatal anoxia reduced PTP may offer a possible mediating
mechanism for previously reported perinatal anoxia/hypoxia-induced learning and memory
deficits in water maze and fear conditioning tests (e.g. Nyakas et al., 1989; Dell'Anna et al.,
1991; Buwalda et al., 1995; Simonova et al., 2003; Yang et al., 2004; Mikati et al., 2005;
Mishima et al., 2005). It should be noted that because PTP is indicative of presynaptic
neurotransmitter release (Zucker & Regehr, 2002), a PTP deficit may have a broad range of
effects on basic sensory/motor processes apart from learning and memory.
Perinatal anoxia/hypoxia has been thought to have little or no effect on many cellular
measures at adulthood. For example, while neonatal anoxia on the day of birth led to significant
Early experience and synaptic plasticity
Akers et al.
15
CA1 cell loss between P3-P13 compared to controls, this difference disappeared by P20 due to
generation of new neurons (Daval et al., 2004). The neonatal anoxia effect on hippocampal LTP
seems to follow a similar pattern, with an increase in LTP relative to controls observed after a
post-trauma delay of 10 min (Jensen et al., 1998) and 8 days (Zhang et al., 2005) but a lack of
effect when the delay was a few months (Jensen et al., 1998). Perhaps for this reason, the
majority of perinatal anoxia/hypoxia studies have predominantly been limited to functional
assessment immediately or shortly after the traumatic episode, ranging from a few minutes to a
maximum of several days (for reviews, see Nyakas et al., 1996; Calvert & Zhang, 2005). Our
finding that PTP was reduced among anoxic rats aged 4.5-8 months clearly indicates that
neonatal anoxia has long-lasting impact on hippocampal synaptic plasticity.
Neonatal novelty exposure effect on LTP. We found that neonatal novelty exposure
produced a selective enhancement of LTP during adulthood, a finding that provides non-trivial
extension and replication of our previous study (Tang & Zou, 2002). First, the effect of
experimental manipulation (ex. exposure to stress) on LTP is known to differ depending on the
specific LTP induction protocol used (Mesches et al., 1999). Second, experimental
manipulations (ex. brain trauma) can differentially affect animals of the same strain but from
different suppliers (Oliff et al., 1995). In the present study, we replicated the novelty exposureinduced enhancement of LTP despite using a different LTP induction protocol and animal
supplier as those used in our previous study. These two extensions, therefore, increase the
validity of the finding that adult LTP can be enhanced by brief neonatal exposures to novelty.
The observation that the neonatal novelty exposure-induced increase in LTP was
significant only in control rats but not in anoxic rats might hint at a blocking of the novelty effect
on LTP by anoxia. However, as the interaction effect between the two treatments was not
Early experience and synaptic plasticity
Akers et al.
16
statistically significant, one does not have evidence for such a conclusion. Interestingly,
however, the pattern of LTP across the four treatment groups mirrored that of LTH, a measure of
social recognition memory. Specifically, both LTP and LTH were increased by neonatal novelty
exposure only in control rats (compare Fig. 3d to Fig. 4e). This association between a novelty
exposure effect on LTP and social memory contrasts with the dissociation between an anoxia
effect on PTP and a lack of anoxia effect on social memory, suggesting that LTP may more
likely be involved in social recognition memory than PTP. The lack of individual-level
correlation between LTP and social memory is not surprising as there was a large and varying
time delay between the two measures (from several days to several months apart). Thus, this
should not be taken as evidence that hippocampus is not involved in social memory.
Early emotional response to novelty as a long-range predictor for adult synaptic
plasticity. We found that neonatal novelty exposure but not neonatal anoxia had a significant
effect on the correlation between open field activity at the juvenile stage (P25) and LTP at
adulthood (4.5-8 months). Specifically, a large correlation was found only in Novel but not in
Home rats. Ample evidence suggests that open field activity is related to circulating CORT, the
end product of the HPA axis (see brief summary in Tang & Nakazawa, 2005). For example,
open field activity has been shown to correlate with circulating CORT level (Piazza et al., 1991;
Vallee et al., 1997). Therefore, the presence of a correlation between open field activity and
LTP within Novel rats may reflect an increased sensitivity of LTP to circulating CORT in
stressful situations, such as exposure to a novel environment (e.g. open field). Such an increase
in sensitivity between Novel and Home rats has been demonstrated by a greater modulation of
LTP by stress levels of CORT in Novel rats (Zou et al., 2001).
Early experience and synaptic plasticity
Akers et al.
17
The present finding of a correlation between open field activity and LTP is in agreement
with that reported by Schulz et al. (2002; 2004) in that individual differences in open field
activity can predict individual differences in measures of synaptic plasticity. While the direction
of correlation in the present study was opposite to that reported in Schulz et al.’s studies, this
most likely reflects several differences between studies that could change the direction of
correlation, including the duration of activity measure, age of animals, and the difference in
delays between measures. Importantly, in the present study, the delay between the measure of
open field activity—often considered a measure of emotional response to novelty (Denenberg,
1969; Chapillon et al., 2002)—and the measure of synaptic plasticity spanned two
developmental stages: juvenility and adulthood. Thus, the present correlation indicates that
individual differences in emotional response to novelty present early in life may reflect stable
underlying differences in neural mechanisms that influence adult synaptic plasticity. This longrange predictive power mirrors the well-established finding in humans that behavioral response
to novelty at childhood can predict a range of individual differences in adult psychological and
brain function (Schwartz et al., 2003).
Early experience and synaptic plasticity
Akers et al.
18
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
We thank Elise McHugh for editorial assistance. This publication was made possible by Grant
Number #5P20-RR015636-04 from the National Center for Research Resources (NCRR), a
component of the National Institutes of Health (NIH). Its contents are solely the responsibility of
the authors and do not necessarily represent the official views of NCRR or NIH.
Early experience and synaptic plasticity
Akers et al.
19
ABBREVIATIONS
CA, cornu Ammonis; CORT, corticosterone; EPSP, excitatory post-synaptic potential; HFS, high
frequency stimulation; HPA, hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal; LTH, long-term habituation; LTP,
long-term potentiation; P, postnatal day; PTP, post-tetanic potentiation
Early experience and synaptic plasticity
Akers et al.
20
REFERENCES
Agresti, A. & Finlay, B. (1997) Introduction to multivariate relationships. In Finlay, B. (ed.)
Statistical methods for the social sciences. Prentice-Hall, Upper Saddle River, NJ, pp.
356-372.
Aguilar, R., Escorihuela, R.M., Gil, L., Tobena, A. & Fernandez-Teruel, A. (2002) Differences
between two psychogenetically selected lines of rats in a swimming pool matching-toplace task: long-term effects of infantile stimulation. Behav Genet, 32, 127-134.
Behnisch, T. & Reymann, K.G. (1995) Thapsigargin blocks long-term potentiation induced by
weak, but not strong tetanisation in rat hippocampal CA1 neurons. Neurosci Lett, 192,
185-188.
Bliss, T.V. & Collingridge, G.L. (1993) A synaptic model of memory: long-term potentiation in
the hippocampus. Nature, 361, 31-39.
Boksa, P., Krishnamurthy, A. & Sharma, S. (1996) Hippocampal and hypothalamic type I
corticosteroid receptor affinities are reduced in adult rats born by a caesarean procedure
with or without an added period of anoxia. Neuroendocrinology, 64, 25-34.
Bowlby, J. (1962) Attachment and loss. Basic Books, New York.
Bredy, T.W., Zhang, T.Y., Grant, R.J., Diorio, J. & Meaney, M.J. (2004) Peripubertal
environmental enrichment reverses the effects of maternal care on hippocampal
development and glutamate receptor subunit expression. Eur J Neurosci, 20, 1355-1362.
Brunson, K.L., Kramar, E., Lin, B., Chen, Y., Colgin, L.L., Yanagihara, T.K., Lynch, G. &
Baram, T.Z. (2005) Mechanisms of late-onset cognitive decline after early-life stress. J
Neurosci, 25, 9328-9338.
Early experience and synaptic plasticity
Akers et al.
21
Buwalda, B., Nyakas, C., Vosselman, H.J. & Luiten, P.G. (1995) Effects of early postnatal
anoxia on adult learning and emotion in rats. Behav Brain Res, 67, 85-90.
Calvert, J.W. & Zhang, J.H. (2005) Pathophysiology of an hypoxic-ischemic insult during the
perinatal period. Neurol Res, 27, 246-260.
Chapillon, P., Patin, V., Roy, V., Vincent, A. & Caston, J. (2002) Effects of pre- and postnatal
stimulation on developmental, emotional, and cognitive aspects in rodents: a review. Dev
Psychobiol, 41, 373-387.
Chou, I.C., Trakht, T., Signori, C., Smith, J., Felt, B.T., Vazquez, D.M. & Barks, J.D. (2001)
Behavioral/environmental intervention improves learning after cerebral hypoxia-ischemia
in rats. Stroke, 32, 2192-2197.
Daval, J.L., Pourie, G., Grojean, S., Lievre, V., Strazielle, C., Blaise, S. & Vert, P. (2004)
Neonatal hypoxia triggers transient apoptosis followed by neurogenesis in the rat CA1
hippocampus. Pediatr Res, 55, 561-567.
De Kloet, E., Oitzl, M. & Joels, M. (1999) Stress and cognition: are glucocorticoids good or bad
guys? Trends Neurosci, 22, 422-426.
Dell'Anna, M.E., Calzolari, S., Molinari, M., Iuvone, L. & Calimici, R. (1991) Neonatal anoxia
induces transitory hyperactivity, permanent spatial memory deficits and CA1 cell density
reduction in developing rats. Behav Brain Res, 45, 125-134.
Denenberg, V.H. (1964) Critical periods, stimulus input, and emotional reactivity: a theory of
infantile stimulation. Psychol Rev, 71, 335-351.
Denenberg, V.H. (1969) Open field behavior in the rat: what does it mean? Ann N Y Acad Sci,
159, 852-859.
Early experience and synaptic plasticity
Akers et al.
22
Diana, G., Scotti de Carolis, A., Frank, C., Domenici, M.R. & Sagratella, S. (1994) Selective
reduction of hippocampal dentate frequency potentiation in aged rats with impaired place
learning. Brain Res Bull, 35, 107-111.
Escorihuela, R.M., Tobena, A. & Fernandez-Teruel, A. (1994) Environmental enrichment
reverses the detrimental action of early inconsistent stimulation and increases the
beneficial effects of postnatal handling on shuttlebox learning in adult rats. Behav Brain
Res, 61, 169-173.
Francis, D.D., Champagne, F.A., Liu, D. & Meaney, M.J. (1999) Maternal care, gene expression,
and the development of individual differences in stress reactivity. Ann N Y Acad Sci, 896,
66-84.
Francis, D.D., Diorio, J., Plotsky, P.M. & Meaney, M.J. (2002) Environmental enrichment
reverses the effects of maternal separation on stress reactivity. J Neurosci, 22, 7840-7843.
Garoflos, E., Panagiotaropoulos, T., Pondiki, S., Stamatakis, A., Philippidis, E. &
Stylianopoulou, F. (2005) Cellular mechanisms underlying the effects of an early
experience on cognitive abilities and affective states. Ann Gen Psychiatry, 4, 8.
Harlow, H.F. (1959) Love in infancy monkeys. Sci Amer, 200, 68-74.
Hellemans, K.G., Benge, L.C. & Olmstead, M.C. (2004) Adolescent enrichment partially
reverses the social isolation syndrome. Brain Res Dev Brain Res, 150, 103-115.
Huot, R.L., Plotsky, P.M., Lenox, R.H. & McNamara, R.K. (2002) Neonatal maternal separation
reduces hippocampal mossy fiber density in adult Long Evans rats. Brain Res, 950, 5263.
Early experience and synaptic plasticity
Akers et al.
23
Iuvone, L., Geloso, M.C. & Dell'Anna, E. (1996) Changes in open field behavior, spatial
memory, and hippocampal parvalbumin immunoreactivity following enrichment in rats
exposed to neonatal anoxia. Exp Neurol, 139, 25-33.
Jensen, F.E., Wang, C., Stafstrom, C.E., Liu, Z., Geary, C. & Stevens, M.C. (1998) Acute and
chronic increases in excitability in rat hippocampal slices after perinatal hypoxia In vivo.
J Neurophysiol, 79, 73-81.
Kehoe, P. & Bronzino, J.D. (1999) Neonatal stress alters LTP in freely moving male and female
adult rats. Hippocampus, 9, 651-658.
Knuth, E.D. & Etgen, A.M. (2005) Corticosterone secretion induced by chronic isolation in
neonatal rats is sexually dimorphic and accompanied by elevated ACTH. Horm Behav,
47, 65-75.
Ladd, C.O., Huot, R.L., Thrivikraman, K.V., Nemeroff, C.B. & Plotsky, P.M. (2004) Long-term
adaptations in glucocorticoid receptor and mineralocorticoid receptor mRNA and
negative feedback on the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis following neonatal maternal
separation. Biol Psychiatry, 55, 367-375.
Lehmann, J., Pryce, C.R., Bettschen, D. & Feldon, J. (1999) The maternal separation paradigm
and adult emotionality and cognition in male and female Wistar rats. Pharmacol Biochem
Behav, 64, 705-715.
Lehmann, J., Pryce, C.R., Jongen-Relo, A.L., Stohr, T., Pothuizen, H.H. & Feldon, J. (2002)
Comparison of maternal separation and early handling in terms of their neurobehavioral
effects in aged rats. Neurobiol Aging, 23, 457-466.
Levine, S. (1960) Stimulation in infancy. Sci Amer, 202, 81-86.
Early experience and synaptic plasticity
Akers et al.
24
Maccari, S., Piazza, P.V., Kabbaj, M., Barbazanges, A., Simon, H. & Le Moal, M. (1995)
Adoption reverses the long-term impairment in glucocorticoid feedback induced by
prenatal stress. J Neurosci, 15, 110-116.
Macri, S., Mason, G.J. & Wurbel, H. (2004) Dissociation in the effects of neonatal maternal
separations on maternal care and the offspring's HPA and fear responses in rats. Eur J
Neurosci, 20, 1017-1024.
McEwen, B.S. (1999) Stress and hippocampal plasticity. Annu Rev Neurosci, 22, 105-122.
Meaney, M.J., Aitken, D.H., van Berkel, C., Bhatnagar, S. & Sapolsky, R.M. (1988) Effect of
neonatal handling on age-related impairments associated with the hippocampus. Science,
239, 766-768.
Meaney, M.J., Diorio, J., Francis, D., Widdowson, J., LaPlante, P., Caldji, C., Sharma, S., Seckl,
J.R. & Plotsky, P.M. (1996) Early environmental regulation of forebrain glucocorticoid
receptor gene expression: implications for adrenocortical responses to stress. Dev
Neurosci, 18, 49-72.
Mesches, M.H., Fleshner, M., Heman, K.L., Rose, G.M. & Diamond, D.M. (1999) Exposing rats
to a predator blocks primed burst potentiation in the hippocampus in vitro. J Neurosci,
19, RC18.
Mikati, M.A., Zeinieh, M.P., Kurdi, R.M., Harb, S.A., El Hokayem, J.A., Daderian, R.H.,
Shamseddine, A., Obeid, M., Bitar, F.F. & El Sabban, M. (2005) Long-term effects of
acute and of chronic hypoxia on behavior and on hippocampal histology in the
developing brain. Brain Res Dev Brain Res, 157, 98-102.
Early experience and synaptic plasticity
Akers et al.
25
Mishima, K., Ikeda, T., Aoo, N., Takai, N., Takahashi, S., Egashira, N., Ikenoue, T., Iwasaki, K.
& Fujiwara, M. (2005) Hypoxia-ischemic insult in neonatal rats induced slowly
progressive brain damage related to memory impairment. Neurosci Lett, 376, 194-199.
Morley-Fletcher, S., Rea, M., Maccari, S. & Laviola, G. (2003) Environmental enrichment
during adolescence reverses the effects of prenatal stress on play behaviour and HPA axis
reactivity in rats. Eur J Neurosci, 18, 3367-3374.
Neumann, I.D., Wigger, A., Kromer, S., Frank, E., Landgraf, R. & Bosch, O.J. (2005)
Differential effects of periodic maternal separation on adult stress coping in a rat model
of extremes in trait anxiety. Neuroscience, 132, 867-877.
Nyakas, C., Buwalda, B. & Luiten, P.G. (1996) Hypoxia and brain development. Prog
Neurobiol, 49, 1-51.
Nyakas, C., Markel, E., Kramers, R.J.K., Gaspar, E., Bohus, B. & Luiten, P.G.M. (1989) Effects
of nimodipine on hypoxia-induced learning and memory deficits. In Traber, J., Gispen,
W.H. (eds.) Nimodipine and central nervous system functions: New vistas. Schattauer,
Stuttgart, pp. 175-194.
Nyakas, C., Markel, E., Schuurman, T. & Luiten, P.G. (1991) Impaired Learning and Abnormal
Open-field Behaviours of Rats After Early Postnatal Anoxia and the Beneficial Effect of
the Calcium Antagonist Nimodipine. Eur J Neurosci, 3, 168-174.
Oliff, H.S., Weber, E., Eilon, G. & Marek, P. (1995) The role of strain/vendor differences on the
outcome of focal ischemia induced by intraluminal middle cerebral artery occlusion in
the rat. Brain Res, 675, 20-26.
Early experience and synaptic plasticity
Akers et al.
26
Panagiotaropoulos, T., Papaioannou, A., Pondiki, S., Prokopiou, A., Stylianopoulou, F. &
Gerozissis, K. (2004) Effect of neonatal handling and sex on basal and chronic stressinduced corticosterone and leptin secretion. Neuroendocrinology, 79, 109-118.
Piazza, P.V., Maccari, S., Deminiere, J.M., Le Moal, M., Mormede, P. & Simon, H. (1991)
Corticosterone levels determine individual vulnerability to amphetamine selfadministration. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 88, 2088-2092.
Pryce, C.R. & Feldon, J. (2003) Long-term neurobehavioural impact of the postnatal
environment in rats: manipulations, effects and mediating mechanisms. Neurosci
Biobehav Rev, 27, 57-71.
Rodrigues, A.L., Arteni, N.S., Abel, C., Zylbersztejn, D., Chazan, R., Viola, G., Xavier, L.,
Achaval, M. & Netto, C.A. (2004) Tactile stimulation and maternal separation prevent
hippocampal damage in rats submitted to neonatal hypoxia-ischemia. Brain Res, 1002,
94-99.
Rosahl, T.W., Spillane, D., Missler, M., Herz, J., Selig, D.K., Wolff, J.R., Hammer, R.E.,
Malenka, R.C. & Sudhof, T.C. (1995) Essential functions of synapsins I and II in
synaptic vesicle regulation. Nature, 375, 488-493.
Rosenthal, R. & Rosnow, R.L. (1984) Essentials of behavioral research: Methods and data
analysis. McGraw-Hill Book Company, New York.
Rutter, M. (1972) Maternal deprivation reassessed. Penguin, Harmondsworth.
Sandstrom, N.J. & Hart, S.R. (2005) Isolation stress during the third postnatal week alters radial
arm maze performance and corticosterone levels in adulthood. Behav Brain Res, 156,
289-296.
Early experience and synaptic plasticity
Akers et al.
27
Schulz, D., Huston, J.P., Jezek, K., Haas, H.L., Roth-Harer, A., Selbach, O. & Luhmann, H.J.
(2002) Water maze performance, exploratory activity, inhibitory avoidance and
hippocampal plasticity in aged superior and inferior learners. Eur J Neurosci, 16, 21752185.
Schulz, D., Sergeeva, O.A., Ianovskii, E., Luhmann, H.J., Haas, H.L. & Huston, J.P. (2004)
Behavioural parameters in aged rats are related to LTP and gene expression of ChAT and
NMDA-NR2 subunits in the striatum. Eur J Neurosci, 19, 1373-1383.
Schwartz, C.E., Wright, C.I., Shin, L.M., Kagan, J. & Rauch, S.L. (2003) Inhibited and
uninhibited infants "grown up": adult amygdalar response to novelty. Science, 300, 19521953.
Silva, A.J., Rosahl, T.W., Chapman, P.F., Marowitz, Z., Friedman, E., Frankland, P.W., Cestari,
V., Cioffi, D., Sudhof, T.C. & Bourtchuladze, R. (1996) Impaired learning in mice with
abnormal short-lived plasticity. Curr Biol, 6, 1509-1518.
Simonova, Z., Sterbova, K., Brozek, G., Komarek, V. & Sykova, E. (2003) Postnatal hypobaric
hypoxia in rats impairs water maze learning and the morphology of neurones and
macroglia in cortex and hippocampus. Behav Brain Res, 141, 195-205.
Spillane, D.M., Rosahl, T.W., Sudhof, T.C. & Malenka, R.C. (1995) Long-term potentiation in
mice lacking synapsins. Neuropharmacol, 34, 1573-1579.
Stevens, C.F. (1998) A million dollar question: does LTP = memory? Neuron, 20, 1-2.
Tang, A.C. (2001) Neonatal exposure to novel environment enhances hippocampal-dependent
memory function during infancy and adulthood. Learn Mem, 8, 257-264.
Tang, A.C. (2003) A hippocampal theory of lateralization. In Hugdahl, K.H., Davidson, R.J.
(eds.) The asymmetrical brain. The MIT Press, Cambridge, pp. 37-68.
Early experience and synaptic plasticity
Akers et al.
28
Tang, A.C. & Nakazawa, M. (2005) Neonatal novelty exposure ameliorates anoxia-induced
hyperactivity in the open field. Behav Brain Res, 163, 1-9.
Tang, A.C., Reeb, B.C., Romeo, R.D. & McEwen, B.S. (2003) Modification of social memory,
hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis, and brain asymmetry by neonatal novelty exposure.
J Neurosci, 23, 8254-8260.
Tang, A.C. & Zou, B. (2002) Neonatal exposure to novelty enhances long-term potentiation in
CA1 of the rat hippocampus. Hippocampus, 12, 398-404.
Vallee, M., Mayo, W., Dellu, F., Le Moal, M., Simon, H. & Maccari, S. (1997) Prenatal stress
induces high anxiety and postnatal handling induces low anxiety in adult offspring:
correlation with stress-induced corticosterone secretion. J Neurosci, 17, 2626-2636.
Wakshlak, A. & Weinstock, M. (1990) Neonatal handling reverses behavioral abnormalities
induced in rats by prenatal stress. Physiol Behav, 48, 289-292.
Wilsch, V.W., Behnisch, T., Jager, T., Reymann, K.G. & Balschun, D. (1998) When are class I
metabotropic glutamate receptors necessary for long-term potentiation? J Neurosci, 18,
6071-6080.
Yang, S.N., Huang, C.B., Yang, C.H., Lai, M.C., Chen, W.F., Wang, C.L., Wu, C.L. & Huang,
L.T. (2004) Impaired SynGAP expression and long-term spatial learning and memory in
hippocampal CA1 area from rats previously exposed to perinatal hypoxia-induced insults:
beneficial effects of A68930. Neurosci Lett, 371, 73-78.
Zhang, J.X., Chen, X.Q., Du, J.Z., Chen, Q.M. & Zhu, C.Y. (2005) Neonatal exposure to
intermittent hypoxia enhances mice performance in water maze and 8-arm radial maze
tasks. J Neurobiol, 65, 72-84.
Early experience and synaptic plasticity
Akers et al.
29
Zou, B., Golarai, G., Connor, J.A. & Tang, A.C. (2001) Neonatal exposure to a novel
environment enhances the effects of corticosterone on neuronal excitability and plasticity
in adult hippocampus. Brain Res Dev Brain Res, 130, 1-7.
Zucker, R.S. & Regehr, W.G. (2002) Short-term synaptic plasticity. Annu Rev Physiol, 64, 355405.
Early experience and synaptic plasticity
Figure 1
Akers et al.
30
Early experience and synaptic plasticity
Figure 2
Akers et al.
31
Early experience and synaptic plasticity
Figure 3
Akers et al.
32
Early experience and synaptic plasticity
Figure 4
Akers et al.
33
Early experience and synaptic plasticity
Figure 5
Akers et al.
34
Early experience and synaptic plasticity
Akers et al.
Table 1. Baseline EPSP slope did not differ among treatment groups (N: number of rats;
M: number of slices).
Control
Anoxia
Novel
N/M
Slope (mV/ms)
(5/15)
-0.77±0.10
Home
(4/9)
-0.76±0.10
Novel
(6/17)
-0.77±0.08
Home
(6/19)
-0.79±0.10
35
Early experience and synaptic plasticity
Akers et al.
36
FIGURE LEGENDS
Figure 1. Experimental timeline. Neonatal anoxia: on postnatal day 1 (P1), half the rat litters
were exposed to 100% N2 gas for 25 min (Anoxia), while the other half were exposed to room
air (Control). Neonatal novelty exposure: from P2-21, half the pups within each litter were
exposed to a novel cage for 3 min a day (Novel), while the other half remained in the home cage
(Home). Open field activity: initial activity within a novel open field was assessed during the
juvenile stage (P25). Social recognition memory: upon entering adulthood (P100-101), social
recognition memory was assessed by habituation of social investigation across repeated sessions
with a conspecific. Synaptic plasticity: in vitro hippocampal PTP and LTP were measured
between 4.5-8 months of age, long after the initial neonatal events.
Figure 2. High frequency stimulation (HFS)-induced potentiation of EPSPs in the CA1 of the
hippocampus. Slope of EPSPs (% of baseline) before and after HFS (three 1-s trains at 100 Hz,
1-min inter-train interval). HFS trains indicated by arrows, and rates of potentiation across trains
between Anoxic and Control rats are shown in inset. For all figures, data are displayed as mean
± sem; * p< 0.05.
Figure 3. Neonatal anoxia and neonatal novelty exposure selectively modulated short- and longterm synaptic plasticity. (ac) Representative EPSP traces for PTP and LTP, with black and gray
lines for before and after HFS, respectively. (b) Neonatal anoxia reduced PTP after the 3rd HFS
train across Novel and Home groups. (d) Neonatal novelty exposure increased LTP only in the
Control group.
Early experience and synaptic plasticity
Akers et al.
37
Figure 4. Effects of neonatal novelty exposure on long-term (24-hr) social recognition memory.
(a-d) Raw frequencies of social investigation across 4 sessions (Day1: S1-3; Day2: D2S1) for
each treatment group. (e) Neonatal novelty exposure increased long-term habituation (LTH) of
social investigative behaviors in the Control but not in the Anoxic group.
Figure 5. Emotional response to novelty during the juvenile stage (P25) as a predictor of adult
synaptic plasticity (4.5-8 months of age). The correlation between open field activity (# of
squares crossed) and adult LTP was significant in the Novel group (a) but not in the Home group
(b). Anoxic rats: filled squares; Control rats: open triangles.
Download