Measurement issues in the Meyer & Allen model of organizational

advertisement
Measurement issues in the Meyer & Allen model of organizational commitment
Steve Jaros
Southern University
Paper to be presented at the 2007 Academy of Management Meetings.
The Meyer/Allen three-component model of commitment arguably dominates
organizational commitment research. Given its widespread use, the measures used to tap
the affective, continuance, and normative commitment constructs merit close scrutiny.
This paper will outline some of the key measurement problems and challenges associated
with this model, and present recommendations for future research. First, I discuss the
degree to which the three Meyer and Allen scales, the ACS, the CCS, and the NCS, tap
their associated affective commitment, continuance commitment, and normative
commitment constructs. Next, I discuss some measurement issues that pertain
specifically to normative commitment. Third, I offer a discussion of measurement issues
that pertain specifically to continuance commitment. I conclude with some comments
about the model's generalizability and relationship with recently-developed work
attitudes that may overlap its conceptual domain.
I. Degree of correspondence between the ACS, CCS, and NCS and Meyer &
Herscovitch's (2001) conceptualizations of the underlying constructs
How well do the ACS, CCS, and NCS reflect the underlying affective, normative,
and continuance commitment constructs? In this section, I argue that there are some
discrepancies between these scales and the constructs, as defined by Meyer &
Herscovitch (2001) and, with respect to normative commitment, Meyer, Becker, & Van
Dick (2006), that they are designed to reflect.
Meyer & Herscovitch's definitions of commitment. Meyer & Herscovitch (2001)
propose that commitment is “a force that binds an individual to a course of action of
relevance to one or more targets.” Employees are theorized to experience this force in the
form of three mindsets: affective, normative, and continuance, which reflect emotional
ties, perceived obligation, and perceived sunk costs in relation to a target, respectively.
In addition to these mindsets, their concept of commitment also includes the
notions of focal and discretionary behavior. A focal behavior is one believed to be
integral to the concept of commitment to a particular target, such that all three mindsets
should predict this behavior. In contrast, discretionary behaviors are 'optional', in the
sense that some mindsets, but not others, may predict these behaviors. For organizational
commitment, the focal behavior is theorized to be maintaining membership in the
organization. One key measurement difference between the two kinds of behaviors is that
Meyer and Herscovitch argue that a commitment’s focal behavior should be referenced in
the scale items used to measure each commitment mindset, whereas discretionary
behaviors should not be. Thus, the basic form of an organizational commitment item
should be that it is worded to reflect (a) the mindset, (b) the target of commitment, in this
case the organization, and (c) the focal behavior, in this case remaining a member of the
organization.
Comparing the ACS, NCS, and CCS to the Meyer and Herscovitch
conceptualization. Let's consider each scale in light of the above conceptualization:
Looking at the eight items of the original NCS (see the Appendix for the original ACS,
NCS, and CCS scales items, with my notes), it appears to me that all eight items
reference the organization, and also seemingly tap the mindset of 'obligation'.
Additionally, it appears that five of the eight items directly reference staying/leaving,
while the remaining three items indirectly reference it (they mention remaining loyal,
which many respondents might interpret to encompass remaining with the organization).
Although this scale is not reproduced here, I also looked at the 1993 revised NCS, and in
my opinion four of the six items directly reference staying/leaving, and the other two
indirectly reference it.
Similarly, all items in the original CCS (and also the revised 1993 CCS, and the
revised six-item Powell & Meyer (2004) CCS) directly reference the organization,
seemingly tap perceived costs or barriers to exit, and staying/leaving intentions. In
contrast, while all eight items of the ACS reference the organization, only one item, the
first one listed, explicitly or implicitly mentions staying/leaving. All of the other items
reference positive feelings about the organization only, such as experiencing a sense of
belonging, embracing the organization's problems, and feeling emotionally attached to
the organization. Thus, seven of the eight ACS items seemingly do not comport with
Herscovitch & Meyer's (2001) concept of affective commitment, at least with respect to
the focal behavior aspect of the construct.
All else being equal, we'd expect a commitment scale that explicitly mentions an
outcome in item wording to correlate with and predict it more strongly then a
commitment scale that does not explicitly tap that outcome in the wording of its items.
We might even argue that such a scale invalidly overlaps with the outcome, thus
overstating the relationship between the outcome and commitment (cf. Bozeman &
Perrewe's 2001 analysis of intent-to-quit items in the OCQ). Yet somewhat paradoxically,
in the case of the Meyer and Allen scales, the exact opposite is true: the ACS, which
largely lacks the focal behavior (staying/leaving) wording, has been found to be a much
stronger correlate of and predictor of turnover related variables than the NCS or the CCS,
which are saturated with staying/leaving wording (Meyer et al., 2002).
There are seemingly two ways that the focal-behavior-in-scale-items discrepancy
between the ACS and the NCS/CCS can be resolved. If one believes (like Meyer &
Herscovitch, 2001) that it is appropriate, indeed necessary, to include focal behavioral
terms in commitment scale items, then seven of the eight ACS items should be re-written
to explicitly tap concepts such as staying and leaving. If one agrees with Bozeman &
Perrewe (2001) that behavioral referents are invalid, contaminating the commitment
measure with measures of other constructs, then the NCS and CCS should largely be rewritten to purge them of the staying/leaving language in their items. But, doing either is
likely to increase the already significantly large discrepancy in the turnover-related
predictive power between the ACS and the NCS/CCS, perhaps to the point where only
the ACS is a significant predictor. If so, the logic behind the three-component model, at
least with respect to the notion of focal behavior and to commitment to the organization,
might be undermined. Future research using a revised ACS that reflects the theorized
construct (i.e., all items mention staying/leaving), or else revised versions of the NCS and
CCS that omit reference to staying/leaving, should be conducted to assess this issue.
II. Issues unique to the NCS
Discriminant validity vs. the ACS. As noted by Bergman (2006), in US and
Canadian studies, neither the 8-item or 6-item NCS has been shown to have a high degree
of discriminant validity with the ACS. Both versions of the scale tend to be highly
correlated with the ACS (.77 for the 6-item and .54 for the 8-item versions). While these
correlations are not indicative of complete redundancy between affective and normative
commitment, normative commitment often offers little additional explanatory power
when modeled as a predictor of outcomes in conjunction with affective commitment.
In non-western cultures, the NCS and the ACS tend to be even more highly
correlated, and yet the NCS arguably has shown greater discriminant validity in these
settings, since it does tend to contribute significantly to outcome prediction. For example,
in a Taiwanese sample, Chang et al. (2006) found a correlation between the ACS and
NCS of .66, but nevertheless, SEM analysis showed that even when controlling for the
ACS, the NCS was a strong predictor of turnover intentions. Likewise, in a Chinese
sample, Chen & Francesco (2003) found a correlation between the ACS and NCS of .64,
but also found that the NCS played a key role in moderating the relationship between the
ACS and three dimensions of citizenship behaviors. Thus, despite high inter-correlations,
there is evidence of construct distinctiveness, at least in Eastern cultures, perhaps
reflecting the "collectivist" natures of those cultures, in which commitment based on
obligation might have more resonance (Meyer & Allen, 1997). Moving forward on this
issue, research is needed to conduct comprehensive item-level CFA or IRT analysis to
determine redundancy between ACS and NCS at the item, not just scale, level, as a
means of pin-pointing redundant items and thus clarifying the distinctiveness of the NC
construct.
Conceptual changes in normative commitment. NC’s definition has changed
since its inception (Allen, 2003). The original 1990 NCS was designed to capture NC as
based on Weiner’s (1982) work on the internalization of social loyalty norms to
organizations. In 1993, NC was reconceptualized as an obligation to stay with the
organization, without specific reference to social pressures about loyalty (Meyer et al.,
1993). This conceptual shift was built in to the revised 1993 NCS. However, recently,
NC has been altered yet again, to reflect reciprocity for a benefit (Meyer et al., 2002).
Still more recently, Meyer, Becker, & Van Dick (2006) refined this new reciprocity
theme further, positing a two-dimensional concept of normative commitment: an
"indebted obligation" aspect that reflects the perceived need to meet other's expectations,
which is theorized to be correlated with continuance commitment; and a "moral
imperative" aspect that reflects striving to meet valued outcomes, which is theorized to be
correlated with affective commitment. Thus, the current NCS, which hasn't been revised
since 1993, has not been modified to keep up with the recent conceptual revisions, and
thus probably does not adequately reflect the theorized construct, which now bears a
strong resemblance to social-exchange based constructs such as the psychological
contract (Rousseau & Wade-Benzoni, 1995).
NC as an antecedent of CC? Lastly, Powell & Meyer (2004) found that contrary
to expectations, social-cost side bets predicted NC stronger than CC, and speculated that
NC might be a "special form" of continuance commitment. This idea has face validity,
since an "obligation to remain" with the organization, particularly one based on
reciprocity, could very well be experienced by the employee as a psychic "cost", in the
form of a guilty conscience, that would have to be incurred if the employee were to break
the obligation and leave. Similarly, Wasti (2003), in noting that Becker's original
continuance commitment theory contemplated "generalized cultural expectations" about
remaining as an antecedent, says that "Becker has in effect proposed that continuance
commitment does not develop from calculative costs only, but has normative bases as
well" (p.529). This further confounds the issue of the discriminant validity of the NCS as
measuring a distinct form of commitment. Clearly, additional empirical work, using a
revised NCS that comports with the latest conceptualization, is needed to address whether
NC really is a form of commitment, or an antecedent of commitment.
III. CCS issues
Specificity of CCS items. As Allen (2003) noted, CC is the degree to which the
employee recognizes, or is aware, that she or he is staying because of the costs associated
with leaving—not the existence of the costs themselves. Further, and critically, this
awareness can stem from various events or perceptions, the nature or substance of which
can be quite idiosyncratic to the individual. For this reason, Allen argues that "the best
CC items are those that capture the recognition of perceived costs but do so without
reference to their specific source." The emphasis on perceived costs is important; because
of the accumulated evidence that the CCS is bi-dimensional (it includes "high sacrifices"
and "low alternatives" items). Thus, the conclusion to be drawn is that only the three
"high sacrifice" items from the original CCS reflect the construct of continuance
commitment, and that only these items should used to measure it. Powell & Meyer
(2004) have taken this advice developed a new CCS that includes the three HS items,
plus three additional items also designed to tap sunk costs but not low perceived
alternatives. This scale merits additional research to assess its validity.
However, Wasti (2003), while agreeing that continuance commitment should
reflect high sacrifice side-bets and not low perceived alternatives, adopts a perspective on
item specificity that parts company with Allen's notion that the best CC items capture
perceived costs sans reference to their specific source. Wasti argues that the CCS should
not consist of "vague items expressing awareness of unspecified costs (p.547)" because
doing so means the CCS will be influenced by affective and normative factors, thus
conflating continuance commitment with affective and normative commitment, and thus
precluding the development of a pure three-component model, in which each form of
commitment has unique antecedents and unique implications for outcomes. She
concludes by claiming that the current "vague" CCS does not "reliably represent a
calculative attachment to the organization" and recommends re-writing the CCS to
consist entirely of items that narrowly reflect calculative, instrumental costs as opposed
to broader normative/affective social and cultural costs.
In one sense, I am sympathetic to Wasti's position. There is a rich research
tradition on "calculative" commitment, commitment based on economic exchange, which
traces its theoretical lineage back to March and Simon (cf. Mayer & Schoorman, 1996)
that has been shunted aside over the past 15 or so years as the Meyer/Allen model has
gained ascendancy in the literature. Perhaps measures of calculative commitment of the
kind Wasti has in mind could be pitted against Meyer & Allen's CCS to determine which
kind of "cost-based commitment" has the most explanatory power. But within the context
of the Meyer/Allen model, Allen's (2003) perspective makes more sense. Continuance
commitment is based on Becker's theory, and as Wasti herself acknowledges, Becker
contemplated commitment based on a broad array of costs, ranging from economic to
cultural and social/psychological. Thus, the CCS should include items that tap this full
range of possible costs.
Other possible CCS models. Finally, concerning the earlier argument against
inclusion of "low alternatives" items in favor of "high sacrifice" items in the CCS: one
other possibility that has not been tested is that both the HS and LA subscales are firstorder factors that reflect a higher, 2nd order, continuance commitment construct. It might
be the case that an employee experiences continuance commitment (perhaps as a result of
a felt need to maintain consistency in behavior, to avoid losing face, etc.), and as a result,
this causes them to believe that they lack alternatives and that leaving the organization
would be difficult/disruptive. This possibility could be modeled using 2nd-order CFA,
with CC modeled as a 2nd order factor that underlies the HS and LA sub- dimensions.
Conversely, it's possible that the HS and LA subscales are formative dimensions
of CC, i.e., they 'cause' the formation of a continuance commitment construct. It might be
the case that an employee perceives that he/she lack alternatives and have incurred sunk
costs that would be difficult to sacrifice, and as a result, they experience continuance
commitment to the organization. This possibility would be assessed by modeling CC as a
latent construct that is formed by the combined influence of latent LA and HS constructs.
Note that this is not the same as simply adding the scores on all of the LA and HS items
to create a single CC scale (as has been the usual practice in the literature). The CC
construct would include a construct-level error term, which implies that the CC construct
has "surplus meaning" (cf. Mackenzie et al., 2005), including the possibility that perhaps
some third factor, in addition to HS and LA, contributes to the formation of CC as well.
Thus, the aggregate CC construct cannot be reduced to a simple mathematical sum of its
indicators, in this case the HS and LA constructs.
Thus, tests of these models may reveal that the LA items may yet be found to play
a role in the formation or reflection of continuance commitment.
Conclusion: Fitting the Meyer and Allen model in a nomological net
Two remaining issues that are important to the further refinement of the
Meyer/Allen measures are (1) the generalizability of the model in a 'micro' sense, and (2)
the relationship between the commitment constructs and recently developed work
attitudes that appear to tap at least part of the same conceptual space.
Micro generalizability. To clarify: I use the term "macro generalizability" to
refer to the generalizability of the Meyer/Allen commitment measures to populations
beyond that, or broader in scope, than the Western population in which it was originally
developed - namely to non-Western social cultures. While investigating the validity of
the measures across cultures is of obvious import, a robust program is currently being
carried out by researchers studying the validity of the model in East Asian cultures (cf.
Cheng & Stockdale, 2003; Chang, 2006), Mid-Eastern cultures (Wasti 2003, Cetin 2006),
and Eastern Europe (Vandenburghe et al., 2001). No doubt, the model will soon be tested
on South American and African populations as well.
In contrast, by "micro generalizability" I mean the validity of the model within
smaller sub-populations within the broader Western culture in which the model was first
developed. Here, research is lacking, though some initial steps have been taken. For
example, Dawley et al, (2005) extended the analysis of the Meyer/Allen constructs to one
such sub-population, volunteers, specifically chamber of commerce board members, and
found that the 'high sacrifice" CC subscale did not measure a meaningful construct.
Dawley et al. explained this by noting that volunteer chamber board members often have
paid employment with other organizations, and would not suffer monetary losses to the
same degree as regular paid employees. They argue that their findings should not be
interpreted as meaning that "perceived sacrifices" is not an important basis of
commitment for volunteer chamber of commerce board members, but instead that the
CCS contains items that are too narrowly focused on economic sacrifices, to the
exclusion of the kinds of social and psychological sacrifices (e.g., the prestige and public
visibility of being a board member) that quitting a chamber board would entail. Thus,
they argue for a refined measure of continuance commitment that would tap these noneconomic costs (recall that this view of the nature of the current CCS is the opposite of
Wasti (2003), who argues that the CCS is characterized by vague, catch-all items and
isn't focused enough on economic costs).
Dawley et al.'s findings have to be viewed with some caution, because (a) Dawley
et al. altered the wording of the three high-sacrifice items in a way that might have
caused respondents to disregard non-economic sacrifices. For example, CC-HS item 3
was altered to remove wording that refers to "considerable personal sacrifices" that would
have to be made, and CC-HS item 2 was altered to remove wording that refers to "life
disruptions"; and (b) chamber of commerce volunteers may not be representative of
volunteers generally, since most members are businessmen who join and serve at least in
part for instrumental reasons (to make business contacts, network with customers, etc.),
as opposed to purely altruistic reasons. Perhaps a sample of Red Cross or battered
women's shelter volunteers would have yielded different results.
But beyond these specifics, this study is exemplary in that it highlights the need to
conduct research that tests the validity of the model, amongst non-traditional organization
members, such as volunteers, contract workers, and temporary workers (cf. CoyleShapiro & Morrow, 2006). Ultimately, formal tests of measurement invariance should be
conducted to determine the relative fit of the ACS, CCS, and NCS across these subpopulations.
Relationships with other recently-developed work attitude constructs.
Recently, several new work-related attitudes have been introduced in the literature that
seemingly tap similar conceptual space of or otherwise challenge the validity of the
Meyer and Allen model. For example, Harrison et al. (2006) note that the meta-analytic
correlations between the ACS and measures of Job Satisfaction are greater than the
correlations between the ACS and the CCS and between the ACS and NCS, implying that
the construct tapped by the ACS, affective commitment, is more closely related to JS than
to the other Meyer/Allen commitment constructs. Thus, they propose that the ACS should
be broken off from the three-component model and coupled with JS as sub-dimensions
of a broader job attitude construct reflecting a "fundamental evaluation of one's job
experiences" (p.306), and which should predict behaviors such as turnover and job
performance better than the three-component model.
Harrison et al. (2006) did find some support for their contention: A meta-analytic
SEM model that posited JS and AC as first-order latent indicators of a 2nd-order job
attitude construct fit the data well, and the job attitude construct predicted behaviors such
as job performance and absenteeism. However, they did not test the comparative utility of
their formulation versus alternatives, such as modeling JS and AC as having direct effects
on behavior, or including normative and continuance commitment in their analyses. Still,
Harrison et al.'s findings challenge the basic structure of the Meyer/Allen model: is
affective commitment one of three commitment mindsets, or is it its affective component
so strong that it actually is distinct from normative and continuance commitment, and
subsumed in a general orientation towards one's job?
Similarly, Lee and Mitchell and colleagues (cf. Mitchell et al., 2001; Lee et al.,
2004) have recently developed a job embeddedness construct, which they propose as
reflecting the idea of the employee being situated "within a social web" that encompasses
the extent to which one has links to other people, the extent to which one's
job/community fit with their life-spaces, and the sacrifices incurred if one wanted to
break these links and leave the organization. In developing this construct, Mitchell et al.
(2001) compared it to the Meyer and Allen commitment model. They noted that (a)
whereas job embeddedness captures both organizational and non-work linkages,
organizational commitment is focused on organizational linkages only, (b) that normative
and affective commitment are "conceptually quite different" from job embeddedness
because NC and AC reflect emotional ties whereas JE is a largely cognitive construct, but
(c) job embeddedness does share considerable conceptual space with continuance
commitment, as both try to capture sunk costs and ties that bind one to the organization.
But, they also noted some differences, such as the CCS's inclusion of "perceived
alternatives" items, and the CCS's use of "general items", whereas their JE scale was
designed to tap specific side-bets. They conclude by saying their JE measure is "more
specific and includes elements not typically included within the side bet idea" (p.1106).
Since JE was designed to reflect an employee's level of enmeshment in the
organization and community, Mitchell et al. argued that their JE construct should be a
powerful predictor of voluntary turnover, above and beyond the predictive power of job
satisfaction and Meyer and Allen's three components of organizational commitment.
Results from the 2001 study were partially supportive: Controlling for the three forms of
commitment and job satisfaction, JE significantly predicted turnover amongst a sample of
hospital workers, but not for a sample of grocery workers. Also, contrary to their
conceptual argument, job embeddedness was strongly correlated with AC (.65) and NC
(.39) but not significantly correlated with CC (.12), indicating that JE is saturated with
affective/evaluative content.
In their 2004 study, Lee et al. conducted an exploratory factor analysis of their job
embeddedness items, a measure of job satisfaction, and Meyer/Allen's ACS. Results
showed a three-factor solution in which items for JS, AC, and the "fit" and "sacrifice"
dimensions of on-the-job embeddedness loaded on one factor, the "fit" and "sacrifice"
dimensions of off-the-job embeddedness on a second factor, and the "links" items for
both on and off the job embeddedness loaded on a third factor. These findings muddle the
waters as to what "job embeddedness" is and how it relates to Meyer and Allen's model.
Future research is needed (cf. Ng & Feldman, 2007), using CFA to sort out factor
structure, and SEM to evaluate their comparative ability to predict outcomes, such as
turnover, that will provide firmer answers to these questions.
Finally, Jackson et al. (2006) have introduced an individual-difference concept
they call psychological collectivism (PC), a personality trait that reflects a preference for
and reliance on group membership, a tendency to adhere to group norms, and
prioritization of in-group goals over individual goals. Jackson et al. succeeded in
developing scales for 5 facets of PC, and found that overall PC positively predicted
citizenship behavior and task performance, and negatively predicted withdrawal
behavior, although they did not control for other causes such as organizational
commitment. The key issues here is whether PC is a factor that might "compete" with
Meyer and Allen's constructs in the prediction of these outcomes, or is PC an antecedent
of commitment? Usually, the search for antecedents to AC, NC, and CC has focused on
extra-individual factors such as side bets (CC), work experiences (AC), and socialcultural normative pressures (NC). But, perhaps PC represents a dispositional tendency to
commit to collectives, including the organization, the team, etc. and therefore might
predict all three forms of commitment. Future research should assess this notion and the
other nomological-net issues discussed in this section, as a means of further refining the
meaning and measures of the Meyer/Allen commitment constructs.
References
Allen, N. 2003. Organizational commitment in the military: A discussion of theory and practice.
Military Psychology, 15: 237-253.
Allen, N. J., & Meyer, J. P. (1990). The measurement and antecedents of
affective, continuance and normative commitment to the organization.
Journal of Occupational Psychology, 63, 1–18.
Bergman, M. (2006). The relationship between affective and
normative commitment: review and research agenda. Journal of Organizational
Behavior, 27: 645–663.
Bozeman, D.B., & Perrewe, P.L. (2001). The effect of item content overlap on
Organisational
Commitment Questionnaire - turnover cognitions relationships.
Journal of Applied
Psychology, 86: 16-25.
Chang, H., Chi, N., & Miao, M. (2007). Testing the relationship between threecomponent organizational/occupational commitment and
organizational/occupational turnover intention using a non-recursive
model. Journal of Vocational Behavior, xxx (2006) xxx–xxx
(FORTHCOMING).
Chen, Z. & Francesco, A. (2003). The relationship between 3-component commitment
employee performance in China. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 62: 490- 510.
and
Coyle-Shapiro, J.A.-M., Morrow, P.C. (2006). Organizational and client commitment
among contracted employees. Journal of Vocational Behavior. 68, 416-431.
Dawley, D., Stephens, R. & Stephens, D. (2005). Dimensionality of organizatio commitment
in volunteer workers: Chamber of commerce board members and role fulfillment.
Journal of Vocational Behavior, 67: 511–525.
Harrison, D., Newman, D. & Roth, P. (2006). How important are job attitudes? Metaanalytic comparisons of integrative behavioral outcomes and time sequences.
Academy of Management Journal, 49: 305-325.
Jackson, C., Colquitt, J., Wesson, M., & Zapata-Phelan, C. (2006). Psychological collectivism:
A measurement validation and linkage to group performance.
Journal of Applied
Psychology, 884-899.
Mackenzie, S., Podsakoff, P., & Jarvis, C. (2005). The Problem of measurement mod
misspecification in behavioral and organizational research and some
recommended
solutions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90: 710–730.
Lee, T., Mitchell, T., Sablynski, C., Burton, J., & Holtom, B. (2004). The effects of j
embeddedness on organizational citizenship, job performance, volitional absences, and
voluntary turnover. Academy of Management Journal, 47: 711-722.
Mayer, R. & Schoorman, F. (1996). Differentiating antecedents of organizational commitment:
a test of March and Simon's model. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 19: 15-28.
Meyer, J. & Allen, N. (1997). Commitment in the Workplace. Sage: Thousand Oaks.
Meyer, J. P., Allen, N. J., & Smith, C., (1993). Commitment to organizations
and occupations: Extension and test of a three-component
conceptualization. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 538–551.
Meyer, J., Becker, T., & Van Dick, R. (2006). Social identities and commitments at
work: toward an integrative model. Journal of Organizational Behavior: 27, 665– 683.
Meyer, J. P., & Herscovitch, L. (2001). Commitment in the workplace:
Toward a general model. Human Resource Management Review, 11,
299–326.
Meyer, J. P., Stanley, D. J., Herscovitch, L., & Topolnytsky, L. (2002).
Affective, continuance and normative commitment to the organization:
A meta-analysis of antecedents, correlates, and consequences. Journal of
Vocational Behavior, 61, 20–52.
Mitchell, T. R., Holtom, B. C., Lee, T. W., Sablynski, C. J., & Erez, M. (2001). Why
stay: Using job embeddedness to predict voluntary turnover. Academy of
Management Journal, 44: 1102–1121.
people
Ng, T., & Feldman, D. 2007. Organizational embeddedness and occupational embeddedness
across career stages. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 70: 336-351.
Powell, D. & Meyer, J. 2004. Side-bet theory and the three-component
model of organizational commitment. Journal of Vocational Behavior,
65: 157–177.
Rousseau, D. M., & Wade-Benzoni, K. A. (1995). Changing individualorganizational attachments: A two-way street. In A. Howard (Ed.), The
Changing Nature of Work. New York: Jossey-Bass.
Wasti, S. (2003). Affective and continuance commitment to the organization: Test of an
integrated model in the Turkish context. International Journal of Intercultural
Relations, 26: 525-550.
Wasti, S. A. (2005). Commitment profiles: Combinations of organizational commitment forms
and job outcomes. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 67, 290-308.
Wiener, Y. 1982. Commitment in organizations: A normative view. Academy of Management
Review, 7, 418–428.
Appendix: Commitment Scale Items (from Allen & Meyer, 1990):
Affective Commitment Scale items
1. I would be very happy to spend the rest of my career
with this organization *
2. I enjoy discussing my organization with people outside it
3. I really feel as if this organization's problems are my own
4. I think that I could easily become as attached to another
organization as I am to this one (R)
5. I do not feel like 'part of the family' at my organization (R)
6. I do not feel 'emotionally attached' to this organization (R)
7. This organization has a great deal of
personal meaning for me
8. I do not feel a strong sense of belonging to my
organization (R)
Continuance Commitment Scale items
1. I am not afraid of what might happen if I quit my job without
having another one lined up (R)
2. It would be very hard for me to leave my organization right
now, even if I wanted to
3. Too much in my life would be disrupted if I decided I
wanted to leave my organization now
4. It wouldn't be too costly for me to leave my
organization now (R)
5. Right now, staying with my organization is a matter of
necessity as much as desire
6. I feel that I have too few options to consider
leaving this organization
7. One of the few serious consequences of leaving this
organization would be the scarcity of available alternatives
8. One of the major reasons I continue to work for this
organization is that leaving would require considerable
personal sacrifice — another organization may not match the
overall benefits I have here
Normative Commitment Scale items
1. I think that people these days move from company to
company too often. *
2. I do not believe that a person must always be loyal to his or
her organization (R)
3. Jumping from organization to organization does not seem at
all unethical to me (R) *
4. One of the major reasons I continue to work for this
organization is that I believe that loyalty is important and
therefore feel a sense of moral obligation to remain *
5. If i got another offer for a better job elsewhere I would not
feel it was right to leave my organization *
6. I was taught to believe in the value of remaining loyal to
one organization
7. Things were better in the days when people stayed with
one organization for most of their careers *
8. I do not think that wanting to be a 'company man' or
'company woman' is sensible anymore (R)
Key:
* directly reflect the focal behavior for organizational commitment, staying/leaving (omitted for
the CCS, since in my opinion all items reflect staying/leaving)
Download