The Neurobiology and Culture of Adolescent Development ANTHRO M293S/APPLING M233/EDUC M286/PSYCH M247 Integrative Seminar for the Center for Culture, Brain and Development Winter, 2011 Thursdays, 12:00 – 2:50 Instructors: Andrew J. Fuligni B7-436 Semel Institute Bldg. 794-6033 afuligni@ucla.edu Mirella Dapretto Brain Mapping Ctr., Room 215 206-2960 mirella@loni.ucla.edu Office Hours: By appointment Course Overview This seminar is intended for doctoral students who are interested in research into the role of neurobiology, social experience, and culture in development during the second decade of life. The course is organized around a selected set of topics that have driven theory and research about the adolescent period and for which there has been recent efforts to integrate neurological development, social experience, and cultural factors. This class is primarily a research seminar, which means that the issues and topics will be discussed from an empirical perspective. Students should be able to critically evaluate research, as well as formulate original empirical questions. The seminar is held in conjunction with the weekly CBD speaker series. Each week, an invited speaker will present a talk that is open for the public. After the talk and a brief break, the students will meet separately with the speaker to discuss issues relevant to the talk and the assigned readings. Requirements 1. Class Participation This course will be conducted in a seminar format, and students are expected to complete all readings prior to class and actively contribute to the discussion of the topics each week. Students are expected to submit two questions or comments for discussion by 9:00 am on the Wednesday before each class to the discussion leaders for the week (CCing both instructors). 2. Leading Discussion Each week, two students are expected to organize the submitted discussion questions into a one-page discussion guide for that week’s class. These students are also expected to lead the discussion with the guest speaker that week. PAGE 1 3. Research Proposal At the end of the course, students will prepare a 7-page, single-spaced research proposal pertaining to adolescent development. The proposal will be prepared according to the format of the NIH R21 mechanism for exploratory and developmental research. First drafts of the proposal are due March 3 and will be given to pre-assigned student reviewers, who will then present their critiques of the proposal on the last day of class, March 10. Revisions that are responsive to these critiques will be due March 16 (see below for more detailed instructions). Course Schedule January 6 Introduction to the Course Readings Spear, L.P. (2010).The Brain, Its Development, and the Neuroscience of Adolescence. The Behavioral Neuroscience of Adolescence (Ch. 4). New York: Norton Casey, B.J., Duhoux, S., & Cohen, M.M. (2010). Adolescence: What Do Transmission, Transition, and Translation Have to Do with It? Neuron, 67, 749-760. January 13 Imaging the Developing Human Brain Elizabeth Sowell University of California, Los Angeles Readings Bramen, J.E., Hranilovich, J.A., Dahl, R.E., Forbes, E.E., Chen, J., Toga, A.W., Dinov, I.D., Worthman, C.M., & Sowell, E.R (In Press). Puberty Influences Medial Temporal Lobe and Cortical Gray Matter Maturation Differently in Boys Than Girls Matched for Sexual Maturity. Cerebral Cortex. Colby, J.B., Van Horn, J.D., & Sowell E.R. (2011). Quantitative In Vivo Evidence for Broad Regional Gradients in the Timing of White Matter Maturation During Adolescence. Neuroimage, 54, 25-31. Giedd, J.N., & Rapaport, J.L. (2010). Structural MRI of Pediatric Brain Development: What Have We Learned and Where Are We Going? Neuron, 67, 728-734. Discussion Leaders: Bahiyyih Hardacre PAGE 1 January 20 Self-Development: Neural and Cultural Effects Jennifer Pfeifer University of Oregon Readings Pfeifer, J.H., Mahy, C.E.V., Chen, C., Masten, C.L., Fuligni, A.J., Lieberman, M.D., Lessard, J., Dong, Q., & Chen, C. (under review). Exploring an Extended Neural Network for Self-Appraisals in Chinese Adults. Pfeifer, J.H., Masten, C.L., Borofsky, L.A., Dapretto, M., Fuligni, A.J., & Lieberman, M.D. (2009). Neural Correlates of Direct and Reflected Self-Appraisals in Adolescents and Adults: When Social Perspective-Taking Informs SelfPerception. Child Development, 80, 1016-1038. Wang, Q. (2006). Culture and the Development of Self-Knowledge. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 182-187. Discussion Leaders: January 27 Nicole Garcia & Katie Hale Social Identity and the Motivation and Well Being of Adolescents Andrew Fuligni University of California, Los Angeles Readings Fuligni, A.J. (2010). Social identity, motivation, and well being among adolescents from Asian and Latin American backgrounds. In G. Carlo, N. J. Crockett, & Carranza, M. (Eds.) Health Disparities in Youth and Families: Research and Applications. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (Vol. 57). New York, NY: Springer. Fuligni, A. J., Telzer, E. H., Bower, J., Kiang, L., Irwin, M. R., & Cole, S. W. (2009). Daily family assistance and inflammation among adolescents from Latin American and European backgrounds. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 23, 803809. Telzer, E.H., Masten, C.L., Berkman, E.T., Lieberman, M.D., & Fuligni, A.J. (2010). Gaining while giving: An fMRI study of the rewards of family assistance. Social Neuroscience, 5, 508-515. PAGE 1 Discussion Leaders: February 3 Seinenu Thein & Gail Fox Adams Neural Correlates of Social Exclusion During Adolescence Mirella Dapretto University of California, Los Angeles Readings Masten, CL, Eisenberger, NI, Borofsky, L., Pfeifer, J, McNealy, K, Mazziotta, JC, & Dapretto, M (2009). Neural correlates of social exclusion during adolescence: Understanding the distress of peer rejection. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci., 4, 143157. Masten C.L., Eisenberger, N.I., Pfeifer, J.H., & Dapretto, M. (In Press). Witnessing Peer Rejection during Adolescence: Neural Correlates of Empathy for Experiences of Social Exclusion. Social Neuroscience. Nishina, A., & Juvonen, J. (2005). Daily reports of witnessing and experiencing peer harassment in middle school. Child Development, 76, 435-450. Discussion Leaders: Afaf Nash & Martin Romero February 10 Connectedness to Parents in Early Adolescence in the United States and China Eva Pomerantz University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign Readings Qin, L., Pomerantz, E. M., & Wang, Q. (2009). Are gains in decision-making autonomy during early adolescence beneficial for emotional functioning? The case of the United States and China. Child Development, 80, 1705–1721. Pomerantz, E. M., & Wang, Q. (2009). The role of parental control in children’s development in Western and East Asian countries. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 285-289. Wang, Q., & Pomerantz, E. M. (2009). The motivational landscape of early adolescence in the United States and China: A longitudinal investigation. Child Development, 80, 1272–1287. PAGE 1 Discussion Leaders: Shayna Tasoff & Heejung Park February 17 Brain/Behavior/Social Context Interactions in Adolescence: A Framework for Understanding Risk, Vulnerabilities, and Opportunities Ronald Dahl University of California, Berkeley Readings Dahl, R. E. (2008). Biological, developmental, and neurobehavioral factors relevant to adolescent driving risks. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 35, S278S284. Forbes, E. E., & Dahl, R. E. (2010). Pubertal development and behavior: Hormonal activation of social and motivational tendencies. Brain and Cognition, 72, 66-72. Steinberg, L. (2008). A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk-taking. Developmental Review, 28, 78-106. Discussion Leaders: Jon Gillespie February 24 Functional Neurodevelopment of Cognitions about Peers Amanda Guyer University of California, Davis Readings Nelson, E. E., Leibenluft, E., McClure, E., & Pine, D. S. (2005). The social reorientation of adolescence: A neuroscience perspective on the process and its relation to psychopathology. Psychological Medicine, 35, 163-174. Guyer, A. E., McClure-Tone, E. B., Shiffrin, N. D., Pine, D. S., & Nelson, E. E. (2009). Probing the neural correlates of anticipated peer evaluation in adolescence. Child Development, 80, 1000-1015. Nelson, E. A., & Guyer, A. E. (under review). The development of the ventral prefrontal cortex and social flexibility. Discussion Leaders: Lauren Sherman PAGE 1 March 3 Decision Making and Risk Taking during Adolescence Adriana Galvan University of California, Los Angeles Readings Galvan, A. (in press). Risky Behavior in Adolescents: The Role of the Developing Brain. Galvan, A., Hare, T.A., Parra, C.E., Penn, J., Voss, H., Glover, G., & Casey, B.J. (2006). Earlier Development of the Accumbens Relative to Orbitofrontal Cortex Might Underlie Risk-Taking Behavior. The Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 6885-6892. Discussion Leaders: March 10 Monika Abels Research Proposal Critiques PAGE 1 Guidelines for Your Research Proposal (Adapted from NIH Guidelines for R21 grants) The R21 mechanism is intended to encourage new exploratory and developmental research projects. It supports investigation of novel scientific ideas, tools, or technologies that have the potential for significant impact on biomedical or biobehavioral research. For example, such projects could assess the feasibility of a novel area of investigation or a new experimental system that has the potential to enhance health-related research. Another example could include the unique and innovative use of an existing methodology to explore a new scientific area. These studies may involve considerable risk but may lead to a breakthrough in a particular area, or to the development of novel techniques, models, or applications that could have a major impact on a field of biomedical, behavioral, or clinical research. Specific Aims (1 page) State concisely the goals of the proposed research and summarize the expected outcome(s), including the impact that the results of the proposed research will exert on the research field(s) involved. List succinctly the specific objectives of the research proposed, e.g., to test a stated hypothesis, create a novel design, solve a specific problem, challenge an existing paradigm or clinical practice, address a critical barrier to progress in the field, or develop new technology. Research Strategy (6 pages) Organize the Research Strategy in the specified order and using the instructions provided below. Start each section with the appropriate section heading – Significance, Innovation, Approach. Cite published experimental details in the Research Strategy section and provide the full reference in the References Cited section (this section is not included in the 6 pp limit) (a) Significance • Explain the importance of the problem or critical barrier to progress in the field that the proposed project addresses. • Explain how the proposed project will improve scientific knowledge, technical capability, and/or clinical practice in one or more broad fields. • Describe how the concepts, methods, technologies, treatments, services, or preventative interventions that drive this field will be changed if the proposed aims are achieved. (b) Innovation • Explain how the application challenges and seeks to shift current research or clinical practice paradigms. • Describe any novel theoretical concepts, approaches or methodologies, instrumentation or interventions to be developed or used, and any advantage over existing methodologies, instrumentation, or interventions. • Explain any refinements, improvements, or new applications of theoretical concepts, approaches or methodologies, instrumentation, or interventions. (c) Approach • Describe the overall strategy, methodology, and analyses to be used to accomplish the specific aims of the project. Include how the data will be collected, analyzed, and interpreted. • Discuss potential problems, alternative strategies, and benchmarks for success anticipated to achieve the aims. • If the project is in the early stages of development, describe any strategy to establish feasibility, and address the management of any high risk aspects of the proposed work. • Include information on Preliminary Studies, if available (for R21 Grants preliminary data are PAGE 1 not required). Guidelines about Reviewing a Research Proposal (Adapted from Current NIH Guidelines) Evaluation Criteria: An R21 grant application need not have extensive background material or preliminary data. Accordingly, reviewers should focus their evaluation on the conceptual framework, the level of innovation, and the potential to significantly advance our knowledge or understanding. Appropriate justification for the proposed work can be provided through literature citations, data from other sources, or, when available, from investigator-generated data. Preliminary data are not required for R21 applications; however, they may be included if available. Overall Impact. Provide an overall impact/priority score to reflect your assessment of the likelihood for the project to exert a sustained, powerful influence on the research field(s) involved, in consideration of the following 5 core review criteria and additional review criteria (as applicable). Core Review Criteria. Consider each of the 5 review criteria below in the determination of scientific and technical merit, and give a separate score for each. An application does not need to be strong in all categories to be judged likely to have major scientific impact. For example, a project that by its nature is not innovative may be essential to advance a field. 1. Significance: Does the project address an important problem or a critical barrier to progress in the field? If the aims of the project are achieved, how will scientific knowledge, technical capability, and/or clinical practice be improved? How will successful completion of the aims change the concepts, methods, technologies, treatments, services, or preventative interventions that drive this field? 2. Innovation: Does the application challenge and seek to shift current research or clinical practice paradigms by utilizing novel theoretical concepts, approaches or methodologies, instrumentation, or interventions? Are the concepts, approaches or methodologies, instrumentation, or interventions novel to one field of research or novel in a broad sense? Is a refinement, improvement, or new application of theoretical concepts, approaches or methodologies, instrumentation, or interventions proposed? 3. Approach: Are the overall strategy, methodology, and analyses well-reasoned and appropriate to accomplish the specific aims of the project? Are potential problems, alternative strategies, and benchmarks for success presented? If the project is in the early stages of development, will the strategy establish feasibility and will particularly risky aspects be managed? If the project involves clinical research, are the plans for 1) protection of human subjects from research risks, and 2) inclusion of minorities and members of both sexes/genders, as well as the inclusion of children, justified in terms of the scientific goals and research strategy proposed? PAGE 1