Can cognitive coordination be extended to explain disorganised affect in Schizophrenia? In order to identify subtypes of schizophrenia, symptoms in each subtype need to be identified. The cognitive coordination theory put forward by Phillips and Silverstein paper specifically discusses cognitive disorganisation. Neuroimaging research has discovered links between brain areas involved in affect and cognition. My suggestion is that cognitive coordination could also be extended to explain disorganised affect, which is commonly categorised under disorganisation syndrome. A link between these theories supports the notion that disorganisation syndrome is a subtype of schizophrenia. Phillips and Silverstein (2003) provide a fascinating insight into the ways in which cognitive context models can be combined with neuroscience research to extend knowledge of schizophrenia. The link between difficulties in perceptual grouping and contextual disambiguation provides a logical explanation for cognitive disorganisation symptoms of schizophrenia. However, definitions of disorganisation syndrome often include disorganised emotion as well as speech difficulties and problems with thought processes. Phillips and Silverstein’s theory does not explain how affect could be disrupted in disorganisation syndrome. I suggest that, based on the findings of recent research linking cognition and emotion, effects of cognitive disorganisation on affect should be considered. A review by Ochsner and Gross (2005) of neuroimaging research shows that there are many ways emotion can be modulated by cognitive factors such as attention and memory. The findings suggested that pre-frontal cortex (PFC) and orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) systems could be involved in interpreting context of stimuli and directing emotional reactions. Ochsner and Gross found two main ways emotion can be directed by cognition. These are attentional control and cognitive change. Research reviewed showed that attending to emotional stimuli and being distracted from emotional stimuli could affect the way a person responds emotionally. Also changing the way an emotional stimulus is perceived could cause a healthy individual to perceive a harmless stimulus as painful or induce placebo effects in pain relief. These findings could be applied to the study of schizophrenia because they show that changing cognitive responses can influence emotional responses. In the studies reviewed, changes were brought about by conscious activities. My argument is that these influences could also be induced by chemical changes in cognitive structures. This could provide the link between NMDA activity and disorganised affect. The OFC is likely to link perception and emotion as it receives information about taste, odours and visual stimuli (Rolls, 1996) and is linked with reward behaviour and punishment behaviour. Therefore, sensory information is processed in this part of the brain to influence behaviour. The process is likely to be mediated by positive and negative emotions. Previous research suggests that the OFC and PFC are both involved in a process of linking emotion and attention (Fragopanagos and Taylor, 2004). The PFC is also important for emotional processing and links with cognitive processes (Gray, Braver and Raichle, 2002). Gray et al. suggest that this part is the centre of where emotion and cognition integrate and each have a role in modulating the other. Ochsner and Gross (2005) discuss the many ways in which the PFC can be linked to the control of emotion. These include regulating amygdala activity in attention tasks and tasks where participants had to interpret stimuli to manipulate emotional responses. The OFC can be impaired in schizophrenia (Pantellis and Brewer, 1995). Pantellis and Brewer found that schizophrenics were unable to identify smells despite being able to distinguish between smells. This suggests that schizophrenics are receiving the sensory information but there is a problem in processing it. Further investigation suggested an executive function problem. This could be compared to the cognitive coordination model as it involves the inability to identify a stimulus. The information required to identify the smell (the external stimulus and internal knowledge) would need to be coordinated in order for recognition to occur. If inability to recognise the smell resulted from a lack of coordination, the NMDA model should be relevant to abnormalities of affect in schizophrenia. This also applies if a combination of longterm memory and attentional deficits disrupt perception. If either of these apply an NMDA antagonist, which disrupts cognitive coordination, should also change the way an individual responds to emotional stimuli. This has been found in a number of cases. NMDA receptors in the amygdala play an important role in conditioning of fear (Walker and Davis, 2000). Campeau et al. (1992, cited in Walker and Davis, 2000) and other researchers (Fanselow & Kim, 1994; Miserendino et al., 1990, cited in Walker and Davis, 2000) showed that when auditory, visual and contextual cues stimuli were used, conditioned fear was affected by blocking NMDA receptors. Other research into the effects of NMDA on emotion implicates the ventral tegmental area, frontal regions (Borowski and Kokkinidis) and hippocampal regions (Young, Bohenek and Fanselow, 1994) in fear responses. Given that existing research focuses mainly on fear responses, further research into other emotional responses would be required to clarify these findings. Also, the emotional influences of the OFC and PFC have not specifically been investigated with NMDA antagonists. A possible link between the affective and cognitive symptoms of disorganisation syndrome support the theory of a subtype of schizophrenia. My suggestion is that the link should be investigated further. If no link is found, the way the symptoms are classified needs to be reconsidered. Further research into a wider range of emotions and sensations beyond pain and fear would be required to increase knowledge of cognition and emotion. Also, more knowledge is required about interactions between the ORC, PFC and emotional centres (such as the amygdala) in relation to NMDA receptors. The cognitive coordination model could provide more detail to clarify how cognitive change and attentional control could be altered by cognitive disorganisation. It is possible that investigating cognition and affect in schizophrenia could provide insight into the cognitive architecture to regulate emotion. References Fragopanagos JG. Taylor N (2004) Modelling the Interaction of Attention and Emotion, BICS, Aug 29 - Sept 1 Ochsner KN, Gross JJ (2005) The cognitive control of emotion, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, Vol. 9 (5), p242-249 Pantelis C, Brewer W (1995) Neuropsychological and olfactory dysfunction in schizophrenia: relationship of frontal syndromes to syndromes of schizophrenia, Schizophrenia Research, 17, p35-45 Phillips WA and Silverstein SM (2003) Convergence of biological and psychological perspectives on cognitive coordination of schizophrenia, Brain and Behaviour Sciences, Vol. 26, p65-138 Rolls ET (1996) The orbitofrontal cortex, Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci., 351 (1346), p1433-1444 Walker DL and Davis M (2000) Involvement of NMDA Receptors Within the Amygdala in Short- Versus Long-Term Memory for Fear Conditioning as Assessed With Fear-Potentiated Startle, Behavioral Neuroscicnce, Vol. 114 (6) p1019-1033