Psych 526B / Ed Psych 556B – Sentence Comprehension Spring 2006, Thur 10-11:50 am, Room 608 Psychology Adams, B. C., Clifton, C., & Mitchell, D. C. (1998). Lexical guidance in sentence processing. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5(2), 265-270. Ahrens, K. (2003). Verbal integration: The interaction of participant roles and sentential argument structure. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 32(5), 497-516. Ahrens, K., & Swinney, D. (1995). Participant roles and the processing of verbs during sentence comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 24(6), 533-547. Ainsworth-Darnell, K., Shulman, H. G., & Boland, J. E. (1998). Dissociating brain responses to syntactic and semantic anomalies: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 112-130. Allbritton, D. W., Mckoon, G., & Ratcliff, R. (1996). Reliability of prosodic cues for resolving syntactic ambiguity. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition, 22, 714-735. Allefeld, C., Frisch, S., & Schlesewsky, M. (2005). Detection of early cognitive processing by event-related phase synchronization analysis. Neuroreport, 16(1), 13-16. Allen, M., Badecker, W., & Osterhout, L. (2003). Morphological analysis in sentence processing: An ERP study. Language & Cognitive Processes, 18(4), 405-430. Altmann, G., & Steedman, M. (1988). Interaction with context during human sentence processing. Cognition, 30, 191-238. Altmann, G. T. (1998). Ambiguity in sentence processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 146-152. Altmann, G. T. M. (1994). Regression-contingent analyses of eye movements during sentence processing Reply to Rayner and Sereno. Memory & Cognition, 22(3), 286-290. Altmann, G. T. M. (2001). The language machine: Psycholinguistics in review. British Journal of Psychology, 92(Part 1), 129-170. Altmann, G. T. M., Garnham, A., & Dennis, Y. (1992). Avoiding the garden path: Eye movements in context. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 685-712. Altmann, G. T. M., Garnham, A., & Henstra, J. (1994). Effects of syntax in human sentence parsing Evidence against a structure-based proposal mechanisim. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 209-216. Altmann, G. T. M., & Kamide, Y. (1999). Incremental interpretation at verbs: Restricting the domain of subsequent reference. Cognition, 73(3), 247-264. Altmann, G. T. M., van Nice, K. Y., Garnham, A., & Henstra, J.-A. (1998). Late closure in context. Journal of Memory & Language, 38(4), May 1998, 1459-1484. Anderson, J. E., & Holcomb, P. J. (2005). An electrophysiological investigation of the effects of coreference on word repetition and synonymy. Brain & Language, 94(2), 200-216. Angrilli, A., Penolazzi, B., Vespignani, F., De Vincenzi, M., Job, R., Ciccarelli, L., et al. (2002). Cortical brain responses to semantic incongruity and syntactic violation in Italian language: An event-related potential study. Neuroscience Letters, 322(1), 5-8. Aoshima, S., Phillips, C., & Weinberg, A. (2004). Processing filler-gap dependencies in a head-final language. Journal of Memory and Language, 51(1), 23-54. Arnold, J. E., Wasow, T., Asudeh, A., & Alrenga, P. (2004). Avoiding attachment ambiguities: The role of constituent ordering. Journal of Memory and Language, 51(1), 55-70. Ashby, J., & Clifton Jr., C. (2005). The prosodic property of lexical stress affects eye movements during silent reading. Cognition, 96(3), B89-B100. Bader, M., & Meng, M. (1999). Subject-object ambiguities in German embedded clauses: An across-theboard comparison. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 28(2), 121-143. Bailey, K. G. D., & Ferreira, F. (2003). Disfluencies affect the parsing of garden-path sentences. Journal of Memory and Language, 49(2), 183-200. 1 Balconi, C., & Pozzoli, U. (2005). Comprehending semantic and grammatical violations in Italian. N400 and P600 comparison with visual and auditory stimuli. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 34(1), 71-98. Barber, H., & Carreiras, M. (2005). Grammatical gender and number agreement in Spanish: An ERP comparison. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(1), 137-153. Barr, D. J., & Keysar, B. (2002). Anchoring comprehension in linguistic precedents. Journal of Memory and Language, 46(2), 391-418. Bastiaansen, M. C. M., van Berkum, J. J. A., & Hagoort, P. (2002a). Event-related theta power increases in the human EEG during online sentence processing. Neuroscience Letters, 323(1), 13-16. Bastiaansen, M. C. M., van Berkum, J. J. A., & Hagoort, P. (2002b). Syntactic processing modulates the theta rhythm of the human EEG. Neuroimage, 17(3), 1479-1492. Bates, E., Devescovi, A., & D'Amico, S. (1999). Processing complex sentences: A cross-linguistic study. Language & Cognitive Processes, 14(1), 69-123. Bates, E., McNew, S., MacWhinney, B., Devescovi, A., & S.Smith. (1982). Functional constraints on sentence processing: A cross-linguistic study. Cognition, 11, 245-299. Baum, S. R., & Dwivedi, V. D. (2003). Sensitivity to prosodic structure in left-and right-hemisphere-damaged individuals. Brain & Language, 87(2), 278-289. Baumgaertner, A., Weiller, C., & Buchel, C. (2002). Event-related fMRI reveals cortical sites involved in contextual sentence integration. Neuroimage, 16(3 Part 1), 736-745. Bavelier, D., Corina, D., Jezzard, P., Padmanabhan, S., Clark, V. P., Karni, A., et al. (1997). Sentence reading - A functional MRI study at 4 Tesla. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9(5), 664-686. Beach, C. M. (1991). The interpretation of prosodic patterns at points of syntactic structure ambiguity: Evidence for cue trading relations. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 644-663. Beckman, M. E. (1996). The parsing of prosody. Language & Cognitive Processes, 11(1-2), 17-67. Bencini, G. M. L., & Goldberg, A. E. (2000). The contribution of argument structure constructions to sentence meaning. Journal of Memory and Language, 43, 640-651. Ben-Shachar, M., Hendler, T., Kahn, I., Ben-Bashat, D., & Grodzinsky, Y. (2003). The neural reality of syntactic transformations: Evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging. Psychological Science, 14(5), 433-440. Ben-Shachar, M., Palti, D., & Grodzinsky, Y. (2004). Neural correlates of syntactic movement: converging evidence from two fMRI experiments. Neuroimage, 21(4), 1320-1336. Besson, M., Faita, F., Czternasty, C., & Kutas, M. (1997). What's in a pause: Event-related potential analysis of temporal disruptions in written and spoken sentences. Biological Psychology, 46(1), 3-23. Bever, T. G., & McElree, B. (1988). Empty categories access their antecedents during comprehension. Linguistic Inquiry, 19, 35-43. Bever, T. G., & Townsend, D. J. (1979). Perceptual mechanisms and formal properties of main and subordinate clauses. In W. E. C. E. C. T. Walker (Ed.), Sentence processing (pp. 159-226). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Binder, K. S., Duffy, S. A., & Rayner, K. (2001). The effects of thematic fit and discourse context on syntactic ambiguity resolution. Journal of Memory & Language, 44(2), 297-324. Birch, S., & Clifton, C. (1995). Focus, accent, and argument structure: Effects on language comprehension. Language and Speech, 38, 365-391. Birch, S., & Clifton, C. (2002). Effects of varying focus and accenting of adjuncts on the comprehension of utterances. Journal of Memory and Language, 47(4), 571-588. Blodgett, A., & Boland, J. E. (2004). Differences in the timing of implausibility detection for recipient and instrument prepositional phrases. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 33(1), 1-24. Bock, J. K., & Mazzella, J. R. (1983). Intonational marking of given and new information: Some consequences for comprehension. Memory & Cognition, 11(1), Jan 1983, 1964-1976. Boland, J. E. (1997). The relationship between syntactic and semantic processes in sentence comprehension. Language & Cognitive Processes, 12(4), 423-484. Boland, J. E. (2005). Visual arguments. Cognition, 95(3), 237-274. 2 Boland, J. E., & Boehmjernigan, H. (1998). Lexical constraints and prepositional phrase attachment. Journal of Memory & Language, 39(4), 684-719. Boland, J. E., & Cutler, A. (1996). Interaction with autonomy - multiple output models and the inadequacy of the great divide. Cognition, 58, 309-320. Bornkessel, I., McElree, B., Schlesewsky, M., & Friederici, A. D. (2004). Multi-dimensional contributions to garden path strength: Dissociating phrase structure from case marking. Journal of Memory and Language, 51(4), 495-522. Bornkessel, I., Schlesewsky, M., & Friederici, A. D. (2002). Beyond syntax: Language-related positivities reflect the revision of hierarchies. Neuroreport, 13(3), 361-364. Bornkessel, I., Schlesewsky, M., & Friederici, A. D. (2002). Grammar overrides frequency: evidence from the online processing of flexible word order. Cognition, 85(2), B21-B30. Bornkessel, I., Schlesewsky, M., & Friederici, A. D. (2003a). Contextual information modulates initial processes of syntactic integration: The role of inter-versus intrasentential predictions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 29(5), 871-882. Bornkessel, I., Schlesewsky, M., & Friederici, A. D. (2003b). Eliciting thematic reanalysis effects: The role of syntax-independent information during parsing. Language & Cognitive Processes, 18(3), 269-298. Bornkessel, I., Zysset, S., Friederici, A. D., von Cramon, D. Y., & Schlesewsky, M. (2005). Who did what to whom? The neural basis of argument hierarchies during language comprehension. Neuroimage, 26(1), 221-233. Bornkessel, I. D., Fiebach, C. J., & Friederici, A. D. (2004). On the cost of syntactic ambiguity in human language comprehension: an individual differences approach. Cognitive Brain Research, 21(1), 11-21. Branigan, H. P., Pickering, M. J., & McLean, J. F. (2005). Priming prepositional-phrase attachment during comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 31(3), 468481. Braze, D., Shankweiler, D., Ni, W. J., & Palumbo, L. C. (2002). Readers' eye movements distinguish anomalies of form and content. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 31(1), 25-44. Britt, M. A. (1994). The interaction of referential ambiguity and argument structure in the parsing of prepositional phrases. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 251-283. Britt, M. A., Perfetti, C. A., Garrod, S., & Rayner, K. (1992). Parsing in discourse: Context effects and their limits. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 293-314. Brown, C. M., van Berkum, J. J. A., & Hagoort, P. (2000). Discourse before gender: An event-related brain potential study on the interplay of semantic and syntactic information during spoken language understanding. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29(1), 53-68. Brown-Schmidt, S., Byron, D. K., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2005). Beyond salience: Interpretation of personal and demonstrative pronouns. Journal of Memory and Language, 53(2), 292-313. Brysbaert, M., & Mitchell, D. C. (1996). Modifier attachment in sentence parsing: Evidence from Dutch. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A Human Experimental Psychology, 49A, 664695. Capek, C. M., Bavelier, D., Corina, D., Newman, A. J., Jezzard, P., & Neville, H. J. (2004). The cortical organization of audio-visual sentence comprehension: An fMRI study at 4 Tesla. Cognitive Brain Research, 20(2), 111-119. Caplan, D. (2001). Functional neuroimaging studies of syntactic processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30(3), 297-320. Caplan, D., Alpert, N., & Waters, G. (1998). Effects of syntactic structure and propositional number on patterns of regional cerebral blood flow. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10(4), 541-552. Caplan, D., Vijayan, S., Kuperberg, G., West, C., Waters, G., Greve, D., et al. (2000). Localization of syntactic processing in sentence comprehension by event-related fMRI. Brain & Language, 74(3), 400-402. Caplan, D., Vijayan, S., Kuperberg, G., West, C., Waters, G., Greve, D., et al. (2002). Vascular responses to syntactic processing: Event-related fMRI study of relative clauses. Human Brain Mapping, 15(1), 2638. 3 Caplan, D., & Waters, G. (1999). Issues regarding general and domain-specific resources. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 22(1), 114-126. Caplan, D., & Waters, G. (2002). Working memory and connectionist models of parsing: A reply to MacDonald and Christiansen (2002). Psychological Review, 109(1), 66-74. Caplan, D., Waters, G., & Alpert, N. (2003). Effects of age and speed of processing on rCBF correlates of syntactic processing in sentence comprehension. Human Brain Mapping, 19(2), 112-131. Caplan, D., & Waters, G. S. (1999). Verbal working memory and sentence comprehension. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 22(1), 77-+. Carey, P. W., Mehler, J., & Bever, T. G. (1970). Judging the veracity of ambiguous sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 9, 243-254. Carlson, K. (2001). The effects of parallelism and prosody in the processing of gapping structures. Language & Speech, 44(Part 1), 1-26. Carlson, K., Clifton, C., & Frazier, L. (2001). Prosodic boundaries in adjunct attachment. Journal of Memory & Language, 45(1), 58-81. Carpenter, P. A., Just, M. A., Keller, T. A., Eddy, W. F., & Thulborn, K. R. (1999). Time course of fMRIactivation in language and spatial networks during sentence comprehension. Neuroimage, 10(2), 216224. Carreiras, M., & Clifton, C. (1993). Relative clause interpretation preferences in Spanish and English. Language and Speech, 36, 353-372. Carreiras, M., & Clifton, C. (1999). Another word on parsing relative clauses: Eyetracking evidence from Spanish and English. Memory & Cognition, 27(5), 826-833. Casado, P., Martin-Loeches, M., Munoz, F., & Fernandez-Frias, C. (2005). Are semantic and syntactic cues inducing the same processes in the identification of word order? Cognitive Brain Research, 24(3), 526-543. Chambers, C. G., Tanenhaus, M. K., Eberhard, K. M., Filip, H., & Carlson, G. N. (2002). Circumscribing referential domains during real-time language comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 47(1), 30-49. Chambers, C. G., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Magnuson, J. S. (2004). Actions and affordances in syntactic ambiguity resolution. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 30(3), 687-696. Chen, E., Gibson, E., & Wolf, F. (2005). Online syntactic storage costs in sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 52(1), 144-169. Choi, Y., & Mazuka, R. (2003). Young children's use of prosody in sentence parsing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 32(2), 197-217. Christiansen, M. H., & Chater, N. (2001). Connectionist psycholinguistics: Capturing the empirical data. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(2), 82-88. Christianson, K., Hollingworth, A., Halliwell, J. F., & Ferreira, F. (2001). Thematic roles assigned along the garden path linger. Cognitive Psychology, 42(4), 368-407. Clahsen, H., & Featherston, S. (1999). Antecedent priming at trace positions: Evidence from German scrambling. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 28(4), 415-437. Clifton, C. (1991). Syntactic modularity in sentence comprehension. In R. R. H. D. S. Palermo (Ed.), Cognition and the symbolic processes: Applied and ecological perspectives (pp. 95-113). Hillsdale, NJ: LEA. Clifton, C., & Duffy, S. A. (2001). Sentence and text comprehension: Roles of linguistic structures. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 167-196. Clifton, C., & Frazier, L. (1986). The use of syntactic information in filling gaps. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 15, 209-224. Clifton, C., Frazier, L., & Connine, C. (1984). Lexical expectations in sentence comprehension. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23, 696-708. 4 Clifton, C., Kennison, S. M., & Albrecht, J. E. (1997). Reading the words "her", "his", "him": Implications for parsing principles based on frequency and on structure. Journal of Memory & Language, 36(2), 276292. Clifton, C., Speer, S., & Abney, S. P. (1991). Parsing arguments: Phrase structure and argument structure as determinants of initial parsing decisions. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 251-271. Clifton, C., .Traxler, M. J., Mohamed, M. T., Williams, R. S., Morris, R. K., & Rayner, K. (2003). The use of thematic role information in parsing: Syntactic processing autonomy revisited. Journal of Memory and Language, in press. Constable, R. T., Pugh, K. R., Berroya, E., Mencl, W. E., Westerveld, M., Ni, W. J., et al. (2004). Sentence complexity and input modality effects in sentence comprehension: an fMRI study. Neuroimage, 22(1), 11-21. Cooke, A., Grossman, M., DeVita, C., Gonzalez-Atavales, J., Moore, P., Chen, W., et al. (2006). Large-scale neural network for sentence processing. Brain & Language, 96(1), 14-36. Cooke, A., Zurif, E. B., DeVita, C., Alsop, D., Koenig, P., Detre, J., et al. (2002). Neural basis for sentence comprehension: Grammatical and short-term memory components. Human Brain Mapping, 15(2), 8094. Corbett, A. T., & Chang, F. R. (1983). Pronoun disambiguation: Accessing potential antecedents. Memory and Cognition, 11, 283-294. Cornish, F., Garnham, A., Cowles, H. W., Fossard, M., & Andre, V. (2005). Indirect anaphora in English and French: A cross-linguistic study of pronoun resolution. Journal of Memory and Language, 52(3), 363376. Coulson, S., Federmeier, K. D., Van Petten, C., & Kutas, M. (2005). Right hemisphere sensitivity to wordand sentence-level context: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 31(1), 129-147. Coulson, S., King, J., & Kutas, M. (1998). Expect the unexpected: Event-related brain response to morphosyntactic violations. Language and Cognitive Processes, 13, 21-58. Crocker, M. W., & Brants, T. (2000). Wide-coverage probabilistic sentence processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29(6), 647-669. Cuetos, F., & Mitchell, D. (1988). Cross-linguistic differences in parsing: Restrictions on the use of the Late Closure strategy in Spanish. Cognition, 30, 73-105. Cupples, L. (2002). The structural characteristics and on-line comprehension of experiencer-verb sentences. Language & Cognitive Processes, 17(2), 125-162. Cutler, A. (1976). Beyond parsing and lexical look-up: An enriched description of auditory sentence comprehension. In R. J. Wales & E. Walker (Eds.), New approaches to language mechanisms (pp. 133-149). Amsterdam: North Holland. Cutler, A., & Norris, D. (1979). Monitoring sentence comprehension. In W. E. Cooper & E. C. T. Walker (Eds.), Sentence processing (pp. 113-134). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Dahan, D., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2004). Continuous mapping from sound to meaning in spoken-language comprehension: Immediate effects of verb-based thematic constraints. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 30(2), 498-513. Dahan, D., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Chambers, C. G. (2002). Accent and reference resolution in spoken-language comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 47(2), 292-314. Dapretto, M., & Bookheimer, S. Y. (1999). Form and content: Dissociating syntax and semantics in sentence comprehension. Neuron, 24(2), 427-432. D'Arcy, R. C. N., Connolly, J. F., Service, E., Hawco, C. S., & Houlihan, M. E. (2004). Separating phonological and semantic processing in auditory sentence processing: a high-resolution event-related brain potential study. Human Brain Mapping, 22(1), 40-51. De Vincenzi, M., Job, R., Di Matteo, R., Angrilli, A., Penolazzi, B., Ciccarelli, L., et al. (2003). Differences in the perception and time course of syntactic and semantic violations. Brain & Language, 85(2), 280296. 5 De_Vincenzi, M. (1991). Filler-gap dependencies in a null subject language: Referential and non-referential WHs. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 20, 197-214. De_Vincenzi, M. (1996). Syntactic analysis in sentence comprehension: Effects of dependency types and grammatical constraints. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 25(1), 117-133. DeLong, K. A., Urbach, T. P., & Kutas, M. (2005). Probabilistic word pre-activation during language comprehension inferred from electrical brain activity. Nature Neuroscience, 8(8), 1117-1121. Desmet, T., & Gibson, E. (2003). Disambiguation preferences and corpus frequencies in noun phrase conjunction. Journal of Memory and Language, 49(3), 353-374. Deutsch, A., & Bentin, S. (2001). Syntactic and semantic factors in processing gender agreement in Hebrew: Evidence from ERPs and eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 45(2), 200-224. Dooling, D. J. (1974). Rhythm and syntax in sentence perception. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13, 255-264. Dopkins, S., & Ngo, C. T. (2005). The role of recognition memory in anaphor identification. Journal of Memory and Language, 53(2), 186-203. Eberhard, K. M., Spivey-Knowlton, M. J., Sedivy, J. C., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1995). Eye movements as a window into real-time spoken language comprehension in natural contexts. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 24, 409-436. Eckstein, K., & Friederici, A. D. (2005). Late interaction of syntactic and prosodic processes in sentence comprehension as revealed by ERPs. Cognitive Brain Research, 25(1), 130-143. Ehrlich, K., & Rayner, K. (1983). Pronoun assignment and semantic integration during reading: Eye movements and immediacy of processing. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 7587. Embick, D., Marantz, A., Miyashita, Y., O'Neil, W., & Sakai, K. L. (2000). A syntactic specialization for Broca's area. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 97(11), 6150-6154. Engle, R. W., Cantor, J., & Carullo, J. J. (1992). Individual differences in working memory and comprehension: A test of four hypotheses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 972-992. Federmeier, K. D., & Kutas, M. (2001). Meaning and modality: Influences of context, semantic memory organization, and perceptual predictability on picture processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 27(1), 202-224. Federmeier, K. D., Mai, H., & Kutas, M. (2005). Both sides get the point: Hemispheric sensitivities to sentential constraint. Memory & Cognition, 33(5), 871-886. Ferreira, F. (2003). The misinterpretation of noncanonical sentences. Cognitive Psychology, 47(2), 164-203. Ferreira, F., Christianson, K., & Hollingworth, A. (2001). Misinterpretations of garden-path sentences: Implications for models of sentence processing and reanalysis. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30(1), 3-20. Ferreira, F., & Clifton, C. (1986). The independence of syntactic processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 25, 348-368. Ferreira, F., & Henderson, J. M. (1990). Use of verb information in syntactic parsing: Evidence from eye movements and word-by-word self-paced reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 555-568. Ferreira, F., & Henderson, J. M. (1991). Recovery from misanalyses of garden-path sentences. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 725-745. Ferreira, V. S. (2003). The persistence of optional complementizer production: Why saying "that" is not saying "that" at all. Journal of Memory and Language, 48(2), 379-398. Ferretti, T. R., McRae, K., & Hatherell, A. (2001). Integrating verbs, situation schemas, and thematic role concepts. Journal of Memory & Language, 44(4), 516-547. Ferstl, E. C., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2001). The role of coherence and cohesion in text comprehension: an event-related fMRI study. Cognitive Brain Research, 11(3), 325-340. 6 Fiebach, C. J., Schlesewsky, M., & Friederici, A. D. (2001). Syntactic working memory and the establishment of filler-gap dependencies: Insights from ERPs and fMRI. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30(3), 321-338. Fiebach, C. J., Schlesewsky, M., & Friederici, A. D. (2002). Separating syntactic memory costs and syntactic integration costs during parsing: the processing of German WH-questions. Journal of Memory and Language, 47(2), 250-272. Fiebach, C. J., Schlesewsky, M., Lohmann, G., von Cramon, D. Y., & Friederici, A. D. (2005). Revisiting the role of Broca's area in sentence processing: Syntactic integration versus syntactic working memory. Human Brain Mapping, 24(2), 79-91. Fiebach, C. J., Vos, S. H., & Friederici, A. D. (2004). Neural correlates of syntactic ambiguity in sentence comprehension for low and high span readers. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(9), 1562-1575. Filik, R., Paterson, K. B., & Liversedge, S. P. (2005). Parsing with focus particles in context: Eye movements during the processing of relative clause ambiguities. Journal of Memory and Language, 53(4), 473495. Fodor, J. D., & Frazier, L. (1980). Is the human sentence parsing mechanism an ATN? Cognition, 8, 417-459. Fodor, J. D., & Inoue, A. (1995). Information-based parsing of Japanese. In R. Mazuka & N. Nagai (Eds.), Japanese sentence processing (pp. 9-63). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, Assoc. Fodor, J. D., & Inoue, A. (2000a). Garden path repair: Diagnosis and triage. Language & Speech, 43(Part 3), 261-271. Fodor, J. D., & Inoue, A. (2000b). Syntactic features in reanalysis: Positive and negative symptoms. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29(1), 25-36. Ford, M. (1983). A method for obtaining measures of local parsing complexity throughout sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 203-218. Ford, M. (1986). A computational model of human parsing processes. In N. E. Sharkey (Ed.), Advances in cognitive science (pp. 259-275). Chichester: Ellis Horwood, Ltd. Ford, M., Bresnan, J., & Kaplan, R. M. (1982). A competence-based theory of syntactic closure. In J. Bresnan (Ed.), The mental representation of grammatical relations (pp. 727-796). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Ford, M., & Dalrymple, M. (1988). A note on some psychological evidence and alternative grammars. Paper presented at the Cognition. Forster, K. I., & Olbrei, I. (1973). Semantic heuristics and syntactic analysis. Cognition, 2, 319-347. Forster, K. I., & Stevenson, B. J. (1987). Sentence matching and well-formedness. Cognition, 26, 171-186. Foss, D. J. (1969). Decision processes during sentence comprehension: Effects of lexical item difficulty and position upon decision times. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8, 457-462. Foss, D. J., & R. H. Lynch, J. (1969). Decision processes during sentence comprehension: Effects of surface structure on decision times. Perception and Psychophysics, 5, 145-148. Frauenfelder, U., Segui, J., & Mehler, J. (1980). Monitoring around the relative clause. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 328-337. Frazier, L. (1995). Constraint satisfaction as a theory of sentence processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 24, 437-468. Frazier, L. (1998). Getting there (slowly). Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 27(2), 123-146. Frazier, L. (2001). The pronoun bias effect strikes again: A reply to Pynte and Colonna. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30(6), 601-604. Frazier, L., Carminati, M. N., Cook, A. E., Majewski, H., & Rayner, K. (2006). Semantic evaluation of syntactic structure: Evidence from eye movements. Cognition, In Press, Corrected Proof. Frazier, L., & Clifton, C. (1997). Construal - Overview, motivation, and some new evidence. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 26(3), 277-295. Frazier, L., Clifton, C., & Randall, J. (1983). Filling gaps: Decision principles and structure in sentence comprehension. Cognition, 13, 187-222. Frazier, L., & Fodor, J. D. (1978). The sausage machine: A new two-stage parsing model. Cognition, 6, 291325. 7 Frazier, L., & Rayner, K. (1982). Making and correcting errors during sentence comprehension: Eye movements in the analysis of structurally ambiguous sentences. Cognitive Psychology, 14, 178-210. Frenck-Mestre, C., & Pynte, J. (1997). Syntactic ambiguity resolution while reading in second and native languages. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50A, 119-148. Friederici, A. D. (1995). The time course of syntactic activation during language processing: A model based on neuropsychological and neurophysiological data. Brain and Language, 50, 259-281. Friederici, A. D. (2001). Syntactic, prosodic, and semantic processes in the brain: Evidence from event-related neuroimaging. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30(3), 237-250. Friederici, A. D. (2002). Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(2), 78-84. Friederici, A. D., & Frisch, S. (2000). Verb argument structure processing: The role of verb-specific and argument-specific information. Journal of Memory & Language, 43(3), 476-507. Friederici, A. D., Hahne, A., & Mecklinger, A. (1996). Temporal structure of syntactic parsing: Early and late event-related brain potential effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 22(5), 1219-1248. Friederici, A. D., Hahne, A., & Saddy, D. (2002). Distinct neurophysiological patterns reflecting aspects of syntactic complexity and syntactic repair. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 31(1), 45-63. Friederici, A. D., & Jacobsen, T. (1999). Processing grammatical gender during language comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 28(5), 467-484. Friederici, A. D., & Mecklinger, A. (1996). Syntactic parsing as revealed by brain responses: First-pass and second-pass parsing processes. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 25(1), 157-176. Friederici, A. D., Mecklinger, A., Spencer, K. M., Steinhauer, K., & Donchin, E. (2001). Syntactic parsing preferences and their on-line revisions: A spatio-temporal analysis of event-related brain potentials. Cognitive Brain Research, 11, 305-323. Friederici, A. D., Meyer, M., & Yves von Cramon, D. (2000). Auditory language comprehension: An eventrelated fMRI study on the processing of syntactic and lexical information. Brain & Language, 74(2), 289-300. Friederici, A. D., Pfeifer, E., & Hahne, A. (1993). Event-related brain potentials during natural speech processing: Effects of semantic, morphological, and syntactic violations. Cognitive Brain Research, 1, 183-192. Friederici, A. D., Steinhauer, K., & Frisch, S. (1999). Lexical integration: Sequential effects of syntactic and semantic information. Memory & Cognition, 27(3), 438-453. Friederici, A. D., Wang, Y. H., Herrmann, C. S., Maess, B., & Oertel, U. (2000). Localization of early syntactic processes in frontal and temporal cortical areas: A magnetoencephalographic study. Human Brain Mapping, 11(1), 1-11. Friederici, i. D., Steinhauer, K., Mecklinger, A., & Meyer, M. (1998). Working memory constraints on syntactic ambiguity resolution as revealed by electrical brain responses. Biological Psychology, 47, 193-221. Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2004). The reading span test and its predictive power for reading comprehension ability. Journal of Memory and Language, 51(1), 136-158. Friedrich, M., & Friederici, A. D. (2005). Semantic sentence processing reflected in the event-related potentials of one- and two-year-old children. Neuroreport, 16(16), 1801-1804. Frisch, S., Hahne, A., & Friederici, A. D. (2004). Word category and verb-argument structure information in the dynamics of parsing. Cognition, 91(3), 191-219. Frisch, S., & Schlesewsky, M. (2001). The N400 reflects problems of thematic hierarchizing. Neuroreport, 12(15), 3391-3394. Frisch, S., Schlesewsky, M., Saddy, D., & Alpermann, A. (2002). The P600 as an indicator of syntactic ambiguity. Cognition, 85(3), B83-B92. Frishkoff, G. A., Tucker, D. M., Davey, C., & Scherg, M. (2004). Frontal and posterior sources of eventrelated potentials in semantic comprehension. Cognitive Brain Research, 20(3), 329-354. 8 Frisson, S., Rayner, K., & Pickering, M. J. (2005). Effects of contextual predictability and transitional probability on eye movements during reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 31(5), 862-877. Gennari, S. P. (2004). Temporal references and temporal relations in sentence comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 30(4), 877-890. Gibson, E. (1998). Linguistic complexity: Locality of syntactic dependencies. Cognition, 68(1), 1-76. Gibson, E., Desmet, T., Grodner, D., Watson, D., & Ko, K. (2005). Reading relative clauses in English. Cognitive Linguistics, 16(2), 313-353. Gibson, E., Hickok, G., & Schutze, C. T. (1994). Processing empty categories - A parallel approach. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 23(5), 381-405. Gibson, E., & Pearlmutter, N. J. (1998). Constraints on sentence comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 262-268. Gibson, E., & Pearlmutter, N. J. (2000). Distinguishing serial and parallel parsing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29(2), 231-240. Gibson, E., Schutze, C. T., & Salomon, A. (1996). The relationship between the frequency and the processing complexity of linguistic structure. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 25(1), 59-92. Gibson, E., & Thomas, J. (1999). Memory limitations and structural forgetting: The perception of complex ungrammatical sentences as grammatical. Language & Cognitive Processes, 14(3), 225-248. Gilboy, E., Sopena, J.-M., Cliftrn, J., Charles, & Frazier, L. (1995). Argument structure and association preferences in Spanish and English complex NPs. Cognition, 54(2), 131-167. Gordon, P. C., Hendrick, R., & Johnson, M. (2001). Memory interference during language processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 27(6), 1411-1423. Gordon, P. C., Hendrick, R., & Johnson, M. (2004). Effects of noun phrase type on sentence complexity. Journal of Memory and Language, 51(1), 97-114. Gordon, P. C., Hendrick, R., Ledoux, K., & Yang, C. L. (1999). Processing of reference and the structure of language: An analysis of complex noun phrases. Language & Cognitive Processes, 14(4), 353-379. Gordon, P. C., Hendrick, R., & Levine, W. H. (2002). Memory-load interference in syntactic processing. Psychological Science, 13(5), 425-430. Gorrell, P. (1989). Establishing the loci of serial and parallel effects in syntactic processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 18, 61-73. Gorrell, P. (1996). Parsing theory and phrase-order variation in German V2 clauses. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 25, 135-156. Green, D. W. (1977). The immediate processing of sentences. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 29, 135-146. Grewe, T., Bornkessel, I., Zysset, S., Wiese, R., von Cramon, D. Y., & Schlesewsky, M. (2005). The emergence of the unmarked: A new perspective on the language-specific function of Broca's area. Human Brain Mapping, 26(3), 178-190. Grodner, D., & Gibson, E. (2005). Consequences of the serial nature of linguistic input for sentenial complexity. Cognitive Science, 29(2), 261-290. Grodner, D., Gibson, E., Argaman, V., & Babyonyshev, M. (2003). Against repair-based reanalysis in sentence comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 32(2), 141-166. Grodner, D., Gibson, E., & Tunstall, S. (2002). Syntactic complexity in ambiguity resolution. Journal of Memory and Language, 46(2), 267-295. Grodner, D., Gibson, E., & Watson, D. (2005). The influence of contextual contrast on syntactic processing: evidence for strong-interaction in sentence comprehension. Cognition, 95(3), 275-296. Grosjean, F. (1983). How long is the sentence? Prediction and prosody in the on-line processing of language. Linguistics, 21, 501-529. Gunter, T. C., & Friederici, A. D. (1999). Concerning the automaticity of syntactic processing. Psychophysiology, 36(1), 126-137. Gunter, T. C., Friederici, A. D., & Hahne, A. (1999). Brain responses during sentence reading: Visual input affects central processes. Neuroreport, 10(15), 3175-3178. 9 Gunter, T. C., Friederici, A. D., & Schriefers, H. (2000). Syntactic gender and semantic expectancy: ERPs reveal early autonomy and late interaction. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 556-568. Gunter, T. C., Stowe, L. A., & Mulder, G. (1997). When syntax meets semantics. Psychophysiology, 34(6), 660-676. Haarmann, H. J., Cameron, K. A., & Ruchkin, D. S. (2002). Neural synchronization mediates on-line sentence processing: EEG coherence evidence from filler-gap constructions. Psychophysiology, 39(6), 820-825. Haarmann, H. J., Cameron, K. A., & Ruchkin, D. S. (2003). Short-term semantic retention during on-line sentence comprehension. Brain potential evidence from filler-gap constructions. Cognitive Brain Research, 15(2), 178-190. Hagoort, P. (2003). Interplay between syntax and semantics during sentence comprehension: ERP effects of combining syntactic and semantic violations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(6), 883-899. Hagoort, P., Brown, C., & Groothusen, J. (1993). The syntactic positive shift (SPS) as an ERP measure of syntactic processing. Language & Cognitive Processing, 8, 439-484. Hagoort, P., & Brown, C. M. (1999). Gender electrified: ERP evidence on the syntactic nature of gender processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 28(6), 715-728. Hagoort, P., & Brown, C. M. (2000). ERP effects of listening to speech compared to reading: The P600/SPS to syntactic violations in spoken sentences and rapid serial visual presentation. Neuropsychologia, 38, 1531-1549. Hagstrom, P., & Rhee, J. (1997). Differences between Korean and Japanese processing overload. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 26, 189-206. Hahne, A. (2001). What's different in second-language processing? Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30(3), 251-266. Hahne, A., Eckstein, K., & Friederici, A. D. (2004). Brain signatures of syntactic and semantic processes during children's language development. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(7), 1302-1318. Hahne, A., & Friederici, A. D. (1999). Electrophysiological evidence for two steps in syntactic analysis: Early automatic and late controlled processes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11(2), 194-205. Hahne, A., & Friederici, A. D. (2002). Differential task effects on semantic and syntactic processes as revealed by ERPs. Cognitive Brain Research, 13(3), 339-356. Hahne, A., & Jescheniak, J. D. (2001). What's left if the Jabberwock gets the semantics? An ERP investigation into semantic and syntactic processes during auditory comprehension. Cognitive Brain Research, 11(2), 199-212. Hahne, A., Schroger, E., & Friederici, A. D. (2002). Segregating early physical and syntactic processes in auditory sentence comprehension. Neuroreport, 13(3), 305-309. Hakes, D. T. (1971). Does verb structure affect sentence comprehension? Perception and Psychophysics, 10, 229-232. Hakes, D. T. (1972). Effects of reducing complement constructions on sentence comprehension. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 278-286. Hakes, D. T., & Cairns, H. S. (1970). Sentence comprehension and relative pronouns. Perception and Psychophysics, 8, 5-8. Hakes, D. T., Evans, J. S., & Brannon, L. L. (1976). Understanding sentences with relative clauses. Memory and Cognition, 4, 283-290. Hakes, D. T., & Foss, D. J. (1970). Decision processes during sentence comprehension: Effects of surface structure reconsidered. Perception and Psychophysics, 8, 413-416. Hald, L. A., Bastiaansen, M. C. M., & Hagoort, P. (2006). EEG theta and gamma responses to semantic violations in online sentence processing. Brain & Language, 96(1), 90-105. Hanley, J. R. (1987). Semantic heuristics, syntactic analysis, and case-role assignment. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 16, 329-334. Hanna, J. E., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Trueswell, J. C. (2003). The effects of common ground and perspective on domains of referential interpretation. Journal of Memory and Language, 49(1), 43-61. Hare, M., McRae, K., & Elman, J. L. (2003). Sense and structure: Meaning as a determinant of verb subcategorization preferences. Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 281-303. 10 Hare, M., McRae, K., & Elman, J. L. (2004). Admitting that admitting verb sense into corpus analyses makes sense. Language & Cognitive Processes, 19(2), 181-224. Hasegawa, M., Carpenter, P. A., & Just, M. A. (2002). An fMRI study of bilingual sentence comprehension and workload. Neuroimage, 15(3), 647-660. Hayashi, R., Imaizumi, S., Mori, K., Niimi, S., Ueno, S., & Kiritani, S. (2001). Elicitation of N400m in sentence comprehension due to lexical prosody incongruity. Neuroreport, 12(8), 1753-1756. Herriot, P. (1968). The comprehension of sentences as a function of grammatical depth and order. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 7, 938-941. Hirose, Y. (2002). Resolution of reanalysis ambiguity in Japanese relative clauses: Early use of thematic compatibility information and incremental processing. In M. Nakayame (Ed.), Sentence processing in East Asian Languages (pp. 31-52). Stanford: CSLI Publications. Hirose, Y. (2003). Recycling prosodic boundaries. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 32(2), 167-195. Hirose, Y., & Chun, S. A. (1999). Attachment ambiguity in head final languages. Japanese and Korean Linguistics, 7. Hirose, Y., & Inoue, A. (1998). Ambiguity of reanalysis in parsing complex sentences in Japanese. Syntax and Semantics, 31, 71-93. Hoeks, J. C. J., Vonk, W., & Schriefers, H. (2002). Processing coordinated structures in context: The effect of topic-structure on ambiguity resolution. Journal of Memory and Language, 46(1), 99-119. Hoen, M., & Dominey, P. F. (2000). ERP analysis of cognitive sequencing: a left anterior negativity related to structural transformation processing. Neuroreport, 11(14), 3187-3191. Holmes, V. M. (1973). Order of main and subordinate clauses in sentence perception. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 12(3), 285-293. Holmes, V. M. (1979). Some hypotheses about syntactic processing in sentence comprehension. In W. E. Cooper & E. C. T. Walker (Eds.), Sentence processing (pp. 227-247). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Holmes, V. M. (1984). Parsing strategies and discourse context. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 13, 237-257. Holmes, V. M., & Forster, K. I. (1972). Perceptual complexity and underlying sentence structure. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 11(2), 148-156. Holmes, V. M., Kennedy, A., & Murray, W. S. (1987). Syntactic structure and the garden path. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 39A, 277-293. Holmes, V. M., Stowe, L., & Cupples, L. (1989). Lexical expectations in parsing complement-verb sentences. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 668-689. Homae, F., Hashimoto, R., Nakajima, K., Miyashita, Y., & Sakai, K. L. (2002). From perception to sentence comprehension: The convergence auditory and visual information of language in the left inferior frontal cortex. Neuroimage, 16(4), 883-900. Homae, F., Yahata, N., & Sakai, K. L. (2003). Selective enhancement of functional connectivity in the left prefrontal cortex during sentence processing. Neuroimage, 20(1), 578-586. Hoover, M. L. (1992). Sentence processing strategies in Spanish and English. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 21, 275-300. Hopf, J. M., Bader, M., Meng, M., & Bayer, J. (2003). Is human sentence parsing serial or parallel? Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Cognitive Brain Research, 15(2), 165-177. Hopf, J.-M., Bayer, J., Bader, M., & Meng, M. (1998). Event-related brain potentials and case information in syntactic ambiguities. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10(2), Mar 1998, 1264-1280. Hsiao, F., & Gibson, E. (2003). Processing relative clauses in Chinese. Cognition, 90(1), 3-27. Humphries, C., Love, T., Swinney, D., & Hickok, G. (2005). Response of anterior temporal cortex to syntactic and prosodic manipulations during sentence processing. Human Brain Mapping, 26(2), 128138. Humphries, C., Willard, K., Buchsbaum, B., & Hickok, G. (2001). Role of anterior temporal cortex in auditory sentence comprehension: An fMRI study. Neuroreport, 12(8), 1749-1752. Hyona, J., & Hujanen, H. (1997). Effects of case marking and word order on sentence parsing in Finnish: An eye fixation analysis. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50A, 841-858. 11 Igoa, J. M., Carreiras, M., & Meseguer, E. (1998). A study on late closure in Spanish: Principle-grounded vs. frequency-based accounts of attachment preferences. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51A, 561-592. Indefrey, P., Hagoort, P., Herzog, H., Seitz, R. J., & Brown, C. M. (2001). Syntactic processing in left prefrontal cortex is independent of lexical meaning. Neuroimage, 14(3), 546-555. Inui, T., Otsu, Y., Tanaka, S., Okada, T., Nishizawa, S., & Konishi, J. (1998). A functional MRI analysis of comprehension processes of Japanese sentences. Neuroreport, 9, 3325-3328. Isel, F., Alter, K., & Friederici, A. D. (2005). Influence of prosodic information on the processing of split particles: ERP evidence from spoken German. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(1), 154-167. Jennings, F., Randall, B., & Tyler, L. K. (1997). Graded effects of verb subcategory preferences on parsing: Support for constraint-satisfaction models. Language & Cognitive Processes, 12(4), 485-504. Jescheniak, J. D., Hahne, A., & Friederici, A. D. (1998). Brain activity patterns suggest prosodic influences on syntactic parsing in the comprehension of spoken sentences. Music Perception, 16, 55-62. Johnson, B. W., & Hamm, J. P. (2000). High-density mapping in an N400 paradigm: evidence for bilateral temporal lobe generators. Clinical Neurophysiology, 111(3), 532-545. Juffs, A. (1998). Some effects of first language argument structure and morphosyntax on second language sentence processing. Second Language Research, 14(4), 406-424. Juffs, A., & Harrington, M. (1995). Parsing effects in second language sentence processing: Subject and object asymmetries in "Wh"-extraction. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 17(4), 483-516. Juffs, A., & Harrington, M. (1996). Garden path sentences and error data in second language sentence processing. Language Learning, 46(2), 283-326. Jun, S. A. (2003). Prosodic phrasing and attachment preferences. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 32(2), 219-249. Jurafsky, D. (1996). A probabilistic model of lexical and syntactic access and disambiguation. Cognitive Science, 20, 137-194. Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1992). A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review, 99, 122-149. Just, M. A., Carpenter, P. A., & Keller, T. A. (1996). The capacity theory of comprehension: New frontiers of evidence and arguments. 103, 773-780. Just, M. A., Carpenter, P. A., Keller, T. A., Eddy, W. F., & Thulborn, K. R. (1996). Brain activation modulated by sentence comprehension. Science, 274, 114-116. Just, M. A., Cherkassky, V. L., Keller, T. A., & Minshew, N. J. (2004). Cortical activation and synchronization during sentence comprehension in high-functioning autism: evidence of underconnectivity. Brain, 127(Part 8), 1811-1821. Just, M. A., Newman, S. D., Keller, T. A., McEleney, A., & Carpenter, P. A. (2004). Imagery in sentence comprehension: an fMRI study. Neuroimage, 21(1), 112-124. Kaan, E. (2001). Effects of NP type on the resolution of word-order ambiguities. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30(5), 529-547. Kaan, E. (2002). Investigating the effects of distance and number interference in processing subject-verb dependencies: An ERP study. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 31(2), 165-193. Kaan, E., Harris, A., Gibson, E., & Holcomb, P. (2000). The P600 as an index of syntactic integration difficulty. Language & Cognitive Processes, 15(2), 159-201. Kaan, E., & Swaab, T. Y. (2003a). Electrophysiological evidence for serial sentence processing: a comparison between non-preferred and ungrammatical continuations. Cognitive Brain Research, 17(3), 621-635. Kaan, E., & Swaab, T. Y. (2003b). Repair, revision, and complexity in syntactic analysis: An electrophysiological differentiation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(1), 98-110. Kaan, E., Wijnen, F., & Swaab, T. Y. (2004). Gapping: Electrophysiological evidence for immediate processing of "missing" verbs in sentence comprehension. Brain & Language, 89(3), 584-592. Kaiser, E., & Trueswell, J. C. (2004). The role of discourse context in the processing of a flexible word-order language. Cognition, 94(2), 113-147. 12 Kamide, Y., Altmann, G. T. M., & Haywood, S. L. (2003). The time-course of prediction in incremental sentence processing: Evidence from anticipatory eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 49(1), 133-156. Kamide, Y., & Mitchell, D. C. (1997). Relative clause attachment: Non-determinism in Japanese parsing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 26, 247-254. Kamide, Y., & Mitchell, D. C. (1999). Incremental pre-head attachment in Japanese parsing. Language & Cognitive Processes, 14, 631-662. Kamide, Y., Scheepers, C., & Altmann, G. T. M. (2003). Integration of syntactic and semantic information in predictive processing: Cross-linguistic evidence from German and English. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 32(1), 37-55. Kang, A. M., Constable, R. T., Gore, J. C., & Avrutin, S. (1999). An event-related fMRI study of implicit phrase-level syntactic and semantic processing. Neuroimage, 10(5), 555-561. Kaschak, M. P., & Glenberg, A. M. (2000). Constructing meaning: The role of affordances and grammatical constructions in sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory & Language, 43(3), 508-529. Kaschak, M. P., Madden, C. J., Therriault, D. J., Yaxley, R. H., Aveyard, M., Blanchard, A. A., et al. (2005). Perception of motion affects language processing. Cognition, 94(3), B79-B89. Katayama, J., Miyata, Y., & Yagi, A. (1987). Sentence verification and event-related brain potentials. Biological Psychology, 25, 173-185. Keller, T. A., Carpenter, P. A., & Just, M. A. (2001). The neural bases of sentence comprehension: A fMRI examination of syntactic and lexical processing. Cerebral Cortex, 11, 223-237. Kemmer, L., Coulson, S., De Ochoa, E., & Kutas, M. (2004). Syntactic processing with aging: An eventrelated potential study. Psychophysiology, 41(3), 372-384. Kennedy, A., & Murray, W. S. (1984). Inspection times for words in syntactically ambiguous sentence under three presentation conditions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 833-849. Kennedy, A., Murray, W. S., Jennings, F., & Reid, C. (1989). Parsing complements: Comments on the generality of the principle of minimal attachment. Language & Cognitive Processes, 4(SI51-SI76). Kennison, S. M. (1999). American English usage frequencies for noun phrase and tensed sentence complement-taking verbs. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 28(2), 165-177. Kennison, S. M. (2000). Two assessments of American usage frequencies for ninety-seven sentence complement-taking nouns. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29(3), 249-264. Kennison, S. M. (2001). Limitations on the use of verb information during sentence comprehension. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8(1), 132-138. Kennison, S. M. (2002). Comprehending noun phrase arguments and adjuncts. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 31(1), 65-81. Kennison, S. M. (2003). Comprehending the pronouns her, him, and his: Implications for theories of referential processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 49(3), 335-352. Kennison, S. M. (2005). Different time courses of integrative semantic processing for plural and singular nouns: implications for theories of sentence processing. Cognition, 97(3), 269-294. Kiehl, K. A., Laurens, K. R., & Liddle, P. F. (2002). Reading anomalous sentences: An event-related fMRI study of semantic processing. Neuroimage, 17(2), 842-850. Kim, A., & Osterhout, L. (2005). The independence of combinatory semantic processing: Evidence from event-related potentials. Journal of Memory and Language, 52(2), 205-225. Kimball, J. P. (1973). Seven principles of surface structure parsing in natural language. Cognition, 2, 15-47. King, J. W., & Kutas, M. (1995). Who did what and when? Using word- and clause-level ERPs to monitor working memory usage in reading. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 376-395. Kjelgaard, M. M., & Speer, S. R. (1999). Prosodic facilitation and interference in the resolution of temporary syntactic closure ambiguity. Journal of Memory & Language, 40(2), 153-194. Klin, C. M., Guzman, A. E., Weingartner, K. M., & Ralano, A. S. (2006). When anaphor resolution fails: Partial encoding of anaphoric inferences. Journal of Memory and Language, 54(1), 131-143. 13 Kluender, R., & Kutas, M. (1993a). Bridging the gap: Evidence from ERPs on the processing of unbounded dependencies. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 196-214. Kluender, R., & Kutas, M. (1993b). Subjacency as a processing phenomenon. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8, 573-633. Knoeferle, P., Crocker, M. W., Scheepers, C., & Pickering, M. J. (2005). The influence of the immediate visual context on incremental thematic role-assignment: evidence from eye-movements in depicted events. Cognition, 95(1), 95-127. Knosche, T. R., Maess, B., & Friederici, A. D. (1999). Processing of syntactic information monitored by brain surface current density mapping based on MEG. Brain Topography, 12(2), 75-87. Koenig, J.-P., Mauner, G., & Bienvenue, B. (2003). Arguments for adjuncts. Cognition, 89(2), 67-103. Koh, S. (1997). The resolution of the dative NP ambiguity in Korean. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 26, 265-273. Kolk, H. H. J., Chwilla, D. J., van Herten, M., & Oor, P. J. W. (2003). Structure and limited capacity in verbal working memory: A study with event-related potentials. Brain & Language, 85(1), 1-36. Konieczny, L. (2000). Locality and parsing complexity. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29(6), 627645. Konieczny, L., Hemforth, B., Scheepers, C., & Strube, G. (1997). The role of lexical heads in parsing: Evidence from German. Language & Cognitive Processes, 12(2-3), 307-348. Koornneef, A. W., & Van Berkum, J. J. A. (2006). On the use of verb-based implicit causality in sentence comprehension: Evidence from self-paced reading and eye tracking. Journal of Memory and Language, In Press, Corrected Proof. Kraljic, T., & Brennan, S. E. (2005). Prosodic disambiguation of syntactic structure: For the speaker or for the addressee? Cognitive Psychology, 50(2), 194-231. Kuperberg, G. R., McGuire, P. K., Bullmore, E. T., Brammer, M. J., Rabe-Hesketh, S., Wright, I. C., et al. (2000). Common and distinct neural substrates for pragmatic, semantic, and syntactic processing of spoken sentences: An fMRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 321-341. Kuperberg, G. R., Sitnikova, T., Caplan, D., & Holcomb, P. J. (2003). Electrophysiological distinctions in processing conceptual relationships within simple sentences. Cognitive Brain Research, 17(1), 117129. Kurtzman, H. S., & MacDonald, M. C. (1993). Resolution of quantifier scope ambiguities. Cognition, 48, 243-279. Kutas, M. (1997). Views on how the electrical activity that the brain generates reflects the functions of different language systems. Psychophysiology, 34, 383-398. Kutas, M., & Kluender, R. (1991). What is who violating? A reconsideration of linguistic violations in light of event-related potentials. CRL Newsletter, 6, 3-19. Langford, J., & Holmes, V. M. (1979). Syntactic presupposition in sentence comprehension. Cognition, 7, 363-383. Lapata, M., Keller, F., & Walde, S. S. I. (2001). Verb frame frequency as a predictor of verb bias. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30(4), 419-435. Lau, E. F., & Ferreira, F. (2005). Lingering effects of disfluent material on comprehension of garden path sentences. Language & Cognitive Processes, 20(5), 633-666. Lee, M. W. (2004). Another look at the role of empty categories in sentence processing (and grammar). Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 33(1), 51-73. Lehiste, I. (1973). Phonetic disambiguation of syntactic ambiguity. Glossa, 7, 107-122. Lelekov, T., Dominey, P. F., & Garcia-Larrea, L. (2000). Dissociable ERP profiles for processing rules vs instances in a cognitive sequencing task. Neuroreport, 11(5), 1129-1132. Levelt, W. J. M. (1978). A survey of studies in sentence perception: 1970-1976. In W. J. M. Levelt & G. B. Flores_d'Arcais (Eds.), Studies in the perception of language (pp. 1-74). New York: John Wiley & Sons. Lewis, R. L., & Vasishth, S. (2005). An activation-based model of sentence processing as skilled memory retrieval [Review]. Cognitive Science, 29(3), 375-419. 14 Li, P., Bates, E., & Liu, H. (1992). Cues as functional constraints on sentence processing in Chinese. In H. C. Chen & O. J. L. Tzeng (Eds.), Language processing in Chinese (pp. 207-234): Elsevier. Li, P., Bates, E., & MacWhinney, B. (1993). Processing a language without inflections: A reaction time study of sentence interpretation in Chinese. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 169-192. Limber, J. (1976). Syntax and sentence interpretation. In R. J. Wales & E. Walker (Eds.), New approaches to language mechanisms (pp. 151-181). Amsterdam: North Holland. Lipka, S. (2002). Reading sentences with a late closure ambiguity: Does semantic information help? Language & Cognitive Processes, 17(3), 271-298. Liversedge, S. P., Paterson, K. B., & Clayes, E. L. (2002). The influence of only on syntactic processing of "long" relative clause sentences. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, A, 55, 225-240. Liversedge, S. P., Pickering, M. J., Branigan, H. P., & van Gompel, R. P. G. (1998). Processing arguments and adjuncts in isolation and context: The case of by-phrase ambiguities in passives. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 24(2), Mar 1998, 1461-1475. Liversedge, S. P., Pickering, M. J., Clayes, E. L., & Branigan, H. P. (2003). Thematic processing of adjuncts: Evidence from an eye-tracking experiment. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10(3), 667-675. Lonzi, L., & Zanobio, M. E. (1983). Syntactic component in language responsible cognitive structure: Neurological evidence. Brain and Language, 18, 177-191. Love, T., & Swinney, D. (1996). Co-reference processing and levels of analysis in object-relative constructions: Demonstration of antecedent reactivation with the cross-modal priming paradigm. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 25, 5-24. Luka, B. J., & Barsalou, L. W. (2005). Structural facilitation: Mere exposure effects for grammatical acceptability as evidence for syntactic priming in comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 52(3), 436-459. Luke, K.-K., Liu, H.-L., Wai, Y.-Y., Wan, Y.-L., & Tan, L. H. (2002). Functional anatomy of syntactic and semantic processing in language comprehension. Human Brain Mapping, 16, 133-145. MacDonald, M. C. (1993). The interaction of lexical and semantic ambiguity. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 692-715. MacDonald, M. C. (1997). Lexical representations and sentence processing: An introduction. Language & Cognitive Processes, 12(2-3), 121-136. MacDonald, M. C., & Christiansen, M. H. (2002). Reassessing working memory: Comment on Just and Carpenter (1992) and Waters and Caplan (1996) [Review]. Psychological Review, 109(1), 35-54. MacDonald, M. C., Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1992). Working memory constraints on the processing syntactic ambiguity. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 56-98. MacDonald, M. C., Pearlmutter, N. J., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1994a). The lexical nature of syntactic ambiguity resolution. Psychological Review, 101, 676-703. MacDonald, M. C., Pearlmutter, N. J., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1994b). Syntactic ambiguity resolution as lexical ambiguity resolution. In J. C. Clifton, L. Frazier & K. Rayner (Eds.), Perspectives on sentence processing (pp. 123-153). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. MacWhinney, B. (1988). The processing of restrictive relative clauses in Hungarian. Cognition, 29, 95-141. Mak, W. M., Vonk, W., & Schriefers, H. (2002). The influence of animacy on relative clause processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 47(1), 50-68. Marslen-Wilson, W. D., Tyler, L. K., Warren, P., Grenier, P., & Lee, C. S. (1992). Prosodic effects in minimal attachment. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45A, 73-88. Martin-Loeches, M., Casado, P., Hinojosa, J. A., Carretie, L., Munoz, F., & Pozo, M. A. (2005). Higher-order activity beyond the word level: Cortical dynamics of simple transitive sentence comprehension. Brain & Language, 92(3), 332-348. Martin-Loeches, M., Munoz, F., Casado, P., Melcon, A., & Ferstl, E. C. (2004). Are the anterior negativities to grammatical violations indexing working memory? Psychophysiology, 42, 508-519. Mason, R. A., Just, M. A., Keller, T. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (2003). Ambiguity in the brain: What brain imaging reveals about the processing of syntactically ambiguous sentences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 29(6), 1319-1338. 15 Matzke, M., Mai, H., Nager, W., Russeler, J., & Munte, T. (2002). The costs of freedom: an ERP - study of non-canonical sentences. Clinical Neurophysiology, 113(6), 844-852. Mauner, G., & Koenig, J. P. (2000). Linguistic vs. conceptual sources of implicit agents in sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory & Language, 43(1), 110-134. Mauner, G., Melinger, A., Koenig, J.-P., & Bienvenue, B. (2002). When is schematic participant information encoded? Evidence from eye-monitoring. Journal of Memory and Language, 47(3), 386-406. Mauner, G., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Carlson, G. N. (1995). Implicit arguments in sentence processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 357-382. Mazuka, R. (1991). Processing of empty categories in Japanese. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 20, 215-232. Mazuka, R., & Itoh, K. (1995). Can Japanese speakers be led down the garden path? In R. M. N. Nagai (Ed.), Japanese sentence processing (pp. 295-329). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, Assoc. Mazuka, R., Itoh, K., & Kondo, T. (1997). Processing down the garden path in Japanese: Processing of sentences with lexical homonyms. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 26, 207-228. McClelland, J. L. (1987). The case for interactionism in language processing. In M. Coltheart (Ed.), Attention and performance XII: The psychology of reading (pp. 3-36). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. McDonald, J. L. (1987). Assigning linguistic roles: The influence of conflicting cues. Journal of Memory and Language, 26, 100-117. McElree, B. (1993). The locus of lexical preference effects in sentence comprehension: A time-course analysis. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 536-? McElree, B. (2000). Sentence comprehension is mediated by content-addressable memory structures. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29(2), 111-123. McElree, B., Foraker, S., & Dyer, L. (2003). Memory structures that subserve sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 48(1), 67-91. McElree, B., & Griffith, T. (1995). Syntactic and thematic processing in sentence comprehension: Evidence for a temporal dissociation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 21, 134-157. McElree, B., & Griffith, T. (1998). Structural and lexical constraints on filling gaps during sentence comprehension: A time-course analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 24(2), Mar 1998, 1432-1460. McElree, B., Traxler, M. J., Pickering, M. J., Seely, R. E., & Jackendoff, R. (2000). Reading time evidence for enriched composition. Cognition, 78, B17-B25. McKinnon, R., & Osterhout, L. (1996). Constraints on movement phenomena in sentence processing: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Language & Cognitive Processes, 11(5), 495-523. McKoon, G., & Macfarland, T. (2002). Event templates in the lexical representations of verbs. Cognitive Psychology, 45(1), 1-44. McKoon, G., & Ratcliff, R. (1994). Sentential context and on-line lexical decision. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 1239-1243. McKoon, G., & Ratcliff, R. (1998). Memory-based language processing: Psycholinguistic research in the 1990s. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 25-42. McKoon, G., & Ratcliff, R. (2003). Meaning through syntax: Language comprehension and the reduced relative clause construction. Psychological Review, 110(3), 490-525. McKoon, G., & Ratcliff, R. (2005). "Meaning through syntax" in sentence production and comprehension: Reply to McRae et al. (2005). Psychological Review, 112(4), 1032-1039. McRae, K., Ferretti, T. R., & Amyote, L. (1997). Thematic roles as verb-specific concepts. Language & Cognitive Processes, 12(2-3), 137-176. McRae, K., Hare, M., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2005). Meaning through syntax is insufficient to explain comprehension of sentences with reduced relative clauses: Comment on McKoon and Ratcliff (2003). Psychological Review, 112(4), 1022-1031. 16 McRae, K., Spivey-Knowlton, M. J., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1998). Modeling the influence of thematic fit (and other constraints) in on-line sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory & Language, 38(3), 283312. Mecklinger, A., Schriefers, H., Steinhauer, K., & Friederich, A. D. (1995). Processing relative clauses varying on syntactic and semantic dimensions: An analysis with event-related potentials. Memory and Cognition, 23, 477-494. Mehler, J., & Carey, P. (1967). Role of surface and base structure in the perception of sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 6, 335-338. Mehler, J., Segui, J., Pittet, M., & Barriere, M. (1978). Strategies for sentence perception. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 7, 3-16. Meng, M., & Bader, M. (2000a). Mode of disambiguation and garden-path strength: An investigation of subject-object ambiguities in German. Language & Speech, 43(Part 1), 43-74. Meng, M., & Bader, M. (2000b). Ungrammaticality detection and garden path strength: Evidence for serial parsing. Language & Cognitive Processes, 15(6), 615-666. Merlo, P. (1994). A corpus-based analysis of verb continuation frequencies for syntactic processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 23, 435-457. Meseguer, E., Carreiras, M., & Jr, C. C. (2002). Overt reanalysis strategies and eye movements during the reading of mild garden path sentences. Memory & Cognition, 30, 551-561. Metzing, C., & Brennan, S. E. (2003). When conceptual pacts are broken: Partner-specific effects on the comprehension of referring expressions. Journal of Memory and Language, 49(2), 201-213. Meyer, M., Alter, K., Friederici, A. D., Lohmann, G., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2002). FMRI reveals brain regions mediating slow prosodic modulations in spoken sentences. Human Brain Mapping, 17(2), 7388. Meyer, M., Alter, K., Friederici, A. D., & von_Cramen, D. Y. (2000). Different hemodynamic responses to sentence-level syntactic and prosodic processing. Neuroimage, 11, 281. Meyer, M., Friederici, A. D., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2000). Neurocognition of auditory sentence comprehension: Event related fMRI reveals sensitivity to syntactic violations and task demands. Cognitive Brain Research, 9(1), 19-33. Meyer, M., Steinhauer, K., Alter, K., Friederici, A. D., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2004). Brain activity varies with modulation of dynamic pitch variance in sentence melody. Brain & Language, 89(2), 277-289. Michael, E. B., Keller, T. A., Carpenter, P. A., & Just, M. A. (2001). fMRI investigation of sentence comprehension by eye and by ear: Modality fingerprints on cognitive processes. Human Brain Mapping, 13(4), 239-252. Miller, G. A. (1962). Some psychological studies of grammar. American Psychologist, 17, 748-762. Miller, J. R. (1981). Effects of semantic and syntactic focus on text production and comprehension. Millis, K. K., & Just, M. A. (1994). The influence of connectives on sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 128-147. Milsark, G. L. (1983). On length and structure in sentence parsing. Cognition, 13, 129-134. Misono, Y., Mazuka, R., Kondo, T., & Kiritani, S. (1997). Effects and limitations of prosodic and semantic biases on syntactic disambiguation. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 26(2), 229-245. Mitchell, D. C. (1984). An evaluation of subject-paced reading tasks and other methods for investigating immediate processes in reading. In D. E. Kieras & M. Just (Eds.), New methods in reading comprehension research (pp. 69-89). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Mitchell, D. C. (1987). Reading and syntactic analysis. In J. R. B. A. M. Colley (Ed.), Cognitive approaches to reading (pp. 87-112). New York: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Mitchell, D. C., & Corley, M. M. B. (1994). Immediate biases in parsing: Discourse effects of experimental artifacts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 20, 217-222. Mitchell, D. C., Corley, M. M. B., & Garnham, A. (1992). Effects of context in human sentence parsing: Evidence against a discourse-based proposal mechanism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 69-88. 17 Mitchell, D. C., Cuetos, F., Corley, M. M. B., & Brysbaert, M. (1995). Exposure-based models of human parsing: Evidence for the use of coarse-grained (nonlexical) statistical records. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 24, 469-488. Mitchell, D. C., & Holmes, V. M. (1985). The role of specific information about the verb in parsing sentences with local structural ambiguity. Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 542-559. Miyake, A., Carpenter, P. A., & Just, M. A. (1994). A capacity approach to syntactic comprehension disorders: Making normal adults perform like aphasic patients. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 11, 671717. Miyamoto, E. T. (2002). Case markers as clause boundary inducers in Japanese. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 31, 307-347. Miyamoto, E. T., Gibson, E., Pearlmutter, N. J., Aikawa, T., & Miyagawa, S. (1999). A U-shaped relative clause attachment preference in Japanese. Miyamoto, E. T., & Takahashi, S. (2001). Sources of difficulty in processing scrambling in Japanese. In M. Nakayama (Ed.), Sentence processing in East-Asian languages (pp. in press?). Palo Alto, CA: CSLI Publications. Mo, L., Liu, H. L., Jin, H., & Yang, Y. L. (2005). Brain activation during semantic judgment of Chinese sentences: A functional MRI study. Human Brain Mapping, 24(4), 305-312. Moreno, E. M., & Kutas, M. (2005). Processing semantic anomalies in two languages: an electrophysiological exploration in both languages of Spanish-English bilinguals. Cognitive Brain Research, 22(2), 205220. Moro, A., Tettamanti, M., Perani, D., Donati, C., Cappa, S. F., & Fazio, F. (2001). Syntax and the brain: Disentangling grammar by selective anomalies. Neuroimage, 13(1), 110-118. Morris, J., & Holcomb, P. J. (2005). Event-related potentials to violations of inflectional verb morphology in English. Cognitive Brain Research, 25(3), 963-981. Muller, H. M., King, J. W., & Kutas, M. (1997). Event-related potentials elicited by spoken relative clauses. Cognitive Brain Research, 5(3), 193-203. Muller, J. L., Hahne, A., Fujjj, Y., & Friederici, A. D. (2005). Native and nonnative speakers' processing of a miniature version of Japanese as revealed by ERPs. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(8), 12291244. Muller, R.-A., Rothermel, R. D., Behen, M. E., Muzik, O., Mangner, T. J., & Chugani, H. T. (1997). Receptive and expressive language activations for sentences: A PET study. NeuroReport, 8, 37673770. Munte, T. F., Anvari, E., Matzke, M., & Johannes, S. (1995). Brain potential correlates of number errors in the Turkish language. Neuroscience Letters, 199(1), 57-60. Munte, T. F., Heinze, H.-J., & Mangun, G. R. (1993). Dissociation of brain activity related to syntactic and semantic aspects of language. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 335-344. Munte, T. F., Heinze, H.-J., Matzke, M., Wieringa, B. M., & Johannes, S. (1998). Brain potentials and syntactic violations revisited: No evidence for specificity of the syntactic positive shift. Neuropsychologia, 36(3), 217-226. Munte, T. F., Matzke, M., & Johannes, S. (1997). Brain activity associated with syntactic incongruities in words and pseudo-words. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 318-329. Munte, T. F., Schiltz, K., & Kutas, M. (1998). When temporal terms belie conceptual order. Nature, 395, 7173. Munte, T. F., Szentkuti, A., Wieringa, B. M., Matzke, M., & Johannes, S. (1997). Human brain potentials to reading syntactic errors in sentences of different complexity. Neuroscience Letters, 235(3), 105-108. Nagata, H. (1990). Speaker's sensitivity to rule violations in sentences. Psychologia: an International Journal of Psychology in the Orient, 33(3), Sep 1990, 1179-1184. Nagata, H. (1991). On-line judgments of grammaticality of sentences involving rule violations. Psychologia: an International Journal of Psychology in the Orient, 34(3), Sep 1991, 1171-1176. Nagata, H. (1991). Temporal course of activation of the antecedent by the reflexive in syntactically ambiguous sentences in Japanese. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 20, 501-520. 18 Nagata, H. (1993). Unimmediate construction of syntactic structure for garden path sentences in Japanese. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 22, 365-382. Nagata, H. (1997). Anchoring effects in judging grammaticality of sentences violating the subjacency condition. Psychologia, 40(3), 163-171. Nagata, H. (2003). Judgments of grammaticality of Japanese sentences violating the principle of full interpretation. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 32(6), 693-709. Nagel, H. N., Shapiro, L. P., & Nawy, R. (1994). Prosody and the processing of filler-gap sentences. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 23, 473-485. Nagel, H. N., Shapiro, L. P., Tuller, B., & Nawy, R. (1996). Prosodic influences on the resolution of temporary ambiguity during on-line sentence processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 25(2), 319-344. Nakai, T., Matsuo, K., Kato, C., Matsuzawa, M., Okada, T., Glover, G. H., et al. (1999). A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of listening comprehension of languages in human at 3 tesla: Comprehension level and activation of the language areas. Neuroscience Letters, 263(1), 33-36. Nakano, Y., Felser, C., & Clahsen, H. (2002). Antecedent priming at trace positions in Japanese long-distance scrambling. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 31(5), 531-571. Nakayama, M. (1999). Sentence processing. In N. Tsujimura (Ed.), The handbook of Japanese linguistics (pp. 398-424). Oxford: Blackwell Publishers. Neville, H., Nicol, J. L., Barss, A., Forster, K. I., & Garrett, M. F. (1991). Syntactically based sentence processing classes: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 151-165. Newman, A. J., Pancheva, R., Ozawa, K., Neville, H. J., & Ullman, M. T. (2001). An event-related fMRI study of syntactic and semantic violations. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30(3), 339-364. Ni, W., Constable, R. T., Mencl, W. E., Pugh, K. R., Fulbright, R. K., Shaywitz, S. E., et al. (2000). An eventrelated neuroimaging study distinguishing form and content in sentence processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(1), 120-133. Ni, W., Fodor, J. D., Crain, S., & Shankweiler, D. (1998). Anomaly detection: Eye movement patterns. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 27(5), Sep 1998, 1515-1539. Nicol, J. L. (1996). What can prosody tell a parser? Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 25(2), 179-192. Nicol, J. L., Fodor, J. D., & Swinney, D. (1994). Using cross-modal lexical decision tasks to investigate sentence processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 1229-1238. Noguchi, Y., Takeuchi, T., & Sakai, K. L. (2002). Lateralized activation in the inferior frontal cortex during syntactic processing: Event-related optical topography study. Human Brain Mapping, 17(2), 89-99. Noppeney, U., & Price, C. J. (2004). An fMRI study of syntactic adaptation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(4), 702-713. Nordlie, J., Dopkins, S., & Johnson, M. (2001). Words in a sentence become less accessible when an anaphor is resolved. Memory & Cognition, 29(2), 355-362. Oberecker, R., Friedrich, M., & Friederici, A. D. (2005). Neural correlates of syntactic processing in twoyear-olds. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(10), 1667-1678. Osterhout, L. (1997). On the brain response to syntactic anomalies: Manipulations of word position and word class reveal individual differences. Brain & Language, 59(3), 494-522. Osterhout, L., & Hagoort, P. (1999). A superficial resemblance does not necessarily mean you are part of the family: Counterarguments to Coulson, King and Kutas (1998) in the P600/SPS-P300 debate. Language & Cognitive Processes, 14(1), 1-14. Osterhout, L., & Holcomb, P. J. (1992). Event-related brain potentials elicited by syntactic anomaly. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 785-806. Osterhout, L., Holcomb, P. J., & Swinney, D. A. (1994). Brain potentials elicited by garden-path sentences: Evidence of the application of verb information during parsing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 786-803. 19 Osterhout, L., McKinnon, R., Bersick, M., & Corey, V. (1996). On the language specificity of the brain response to syntactic anomalies: Is the syntactic positive shift a member of the P300 family. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8(6), 507-526. Osterhout, L., & Mobley, L. A. (1995). Event-related brain potentials elicited by failure to agree. Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 739-773. Osterhout, L., & Nicol, J. (1999). On the distinctiveness, independence, and time course of the brain responses to syntactic and semantic anomalies. Language & Cognitive Processes, 14(3), 283-317. Ostrosky-Solis, F., Rigalt, C., Perez, M., & Marcos, J. (1996). Brain potentials (ERPs) and syntactic comprehension: Effects of thematic role order. Brain and Cognition, 30, 297-300. Palladino, P., Cornoldi, C., De Beni, R., & Pazzaglia, F. (2001). Working memory and updating processes in reading comprehension. Memory & Cognition, 29(2), 344-354. Pannekamp, A., Toepel, U., Alter, K., Hahne, A., & Friederici, A. D. (2005). Prosody-driven sentence processing: An event-related brain potential study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(3), 407-421. Patel, A. D. (1998). Syntactic processing in language and music: Different cognitive operations, similar neural resources? Music Perception, 16(1), 27-42. Patel, A. D., Gibson, E., Ratner, J., Besson, M., & Holcomb, P. J. (1998). Processing syntactic relations in language and music: An event-related potential study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10(6), 717733. Paterson, K. B., Liversedge, S. P., & Underwood, G. (1999). The influence of focus operators on syntactic processing of short relative clause sentences. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 52A(Human Experimental Psychology), 717-737. Pearlmutter, N. J. (2000). Linear versus hierarchical agreement feature processing in comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29(1), 89-98. Pearlmutter, N. J., Garnsey, S. M., & Bock, K. (1999). Agreement processes in sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory & Language, 41(3), 427-456. Pearlmutter, N. J., & Gibson, E. (2001). Recency in verb phrase attachment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 27(2), 574-590. Pearlmutter, N. J., & Macdonald, M. C. (1995). Individual differences and probabilistic constraints in syntactic ambiguity resolution. 34, 521-542. Schafer, A., Carter, J., Clifton, C., & Frazier, L. (1996). Focus in relative clause construal. Language & Cognitive Processes, 11(1-2), 135-163. Schafer, A. J., Speer, S. R., Warren, P., & White, S. D. (2000). Intonational disambiguation in sentence production and comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29(2), 169-182. Schiller, N. O., Munte, T. F., Horemans, I., & Jansma, B. M. (2003). The influence of semantic and phonological factors on syntactic decisions: An event-related brain potential study. Psychophysiology, 40(6), 869-877. Schlesewsky, M., & Bornkessel, I. (2003). Ungrammaticality detection and garden path strength: A commentary on Meng and Bader's (2000) evidence for serial parsing. Language & Cognitive Processes, 18(3), 299-311. Schlesewsky, M., Bornkessel, I., & Meyer, M. (2002). Why a "word order difference" is not always a "word order" difference: A reply to Weyerts, Penke, Munte, Heinze, and Clahsen. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 31(5), 437-445. Schlosser, M. J., Aoyagi, N., Fulbright, R. K., Gore, J. C., & McCarthy, G. (1998). Functional MRI studies of auditory comprehension. Human Brain Mapping, 6(1), 1-13. Schmauder, A. R., & Egan, M. C. (1998). The influence of semantic fit on on-line sentence processing. Memory & Cognition, 26(6), 1304-1312. Schmauder, A. R., Kennision, S. M., & C. Clifton, J. (1991). On the conditions necessary for obtaining argument structure complexity effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 1188-1192. Schneider, D., & Phillips, C. (2001). Grammatical search and reanalysis. Journal of Memory & Language, 45(2), 308-336. 20 Scholes, R. J. (1971). On the spoken disambiguation of superficially ambiguous sentences. Language and Speech, 14, 1-11. Schriefers, H., Friederici, A. D., & Kuhn, K. (1995). The processing of locally ambiguous relative clauses in German. Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 499-520. Schutze, C. T., & Gibson, E. (1999). Argumenthood and English prepositional phrase attachment. Journal of Memory & Language, 40(3), 409-431. Sedivy, J. C. (2002). Invoking discourse-based contrast sets and resolving syntactic ambiguities. Journal of Memory & Language, 46(2), 341-370. Sedivy, J. C. (2003). Pragmatic versus form-based accounts of referential contrast: Evidence for effects of informativity expectations. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 32(1), 3-23. Sedivy, J. C., Tanenhaus, M. K., Chambers, C. G., & Carlson, G. N. (1999). Achieving incremental semantic interpretation through contextual representation. Cognition, 71(2), 109-147. Seidenberg, M. S., & MacDonald, M. C. (1999). A probabilistic constraints approach to language acquisition and processing. Cognitive Science, 23(4), 569-588. Shapiro, L. P. (2000). Charting the time-course of language processing. Brain & Language, 71(1), 224-226. Shapiro, L. P., Brookins, B., Gordon, B., & Nagel, N. (1991). Verb effects during sentence processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 17, 983-996. Shapiro, L. P., & Hestvik, A. (1995). On-line comprehension of VP-ellipsis: Syntactic reconstruction and semantic influence. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 24, 517-532. Shapiro, L. P., Hestvik, A., Lesan, L., & Rachel Garcia, A. (2003). Charting the time-course of VP-ellipsis sentence comprehension: Evidence for an initial and independent structural analysis. Journal of Memory and Language, 49(1), 1-19. Shapiro, L. P., Nagel, H. N., & Levine, B. A. (1993). Preferences for a verb's complements and their use in sentence processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 96-114. Shapiro, L. P., Zurif, E., & Grimshaw, J. (1987). Sentence processing and the mental representation of verbs. Cognition, 27, 219-246. Shapiro, L. P., Zurif, E. B., & Grimshaw, J. (1989). Verb processing during sentence comprehension: Contextual impenetrability. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 18, 223-243. Shattuck-Hufnagel, S., & Turk, A. E. (1996). A prosody tutorial for investigators of auditory sentence processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 25(2), 193-247. Shriberg, E., Bates, R., Stolcke, A., Taylor, P., Jurafsky, D., Ries, K., et al. (1998). Can prosody aid the automatic classification of dialog acts in conversational speech? Language & Speech, 41(Part 3-4), 443-492. Snedeker, J., & Trueswell, J. (2003). Using prosody to avoid ambiguity: Effects of speaker awareness and referential context. Journal of Memory and Language, 48(1), 103-130. Snedeker, J., & Trueswell, J. C. (2004). The developing constraints on parsing decisions: The role of lexicalbiases and referential scenes in child and adult sentence processing. Cognitive Psychology, 49(3), 238299. Speer, N. K., & Zacks, J. M. (2005). Temporal changes as event boundaries: Processing and memory consequences of narrative time shifts. Journal of Memory and Language, 53(1), 125-140. Speer, S. R., & Clifton, C. (1998). Plausibility and argument structure in sentence comprehension. Memory & Cognition, 26(5), 965-978. Speer, S. R., Kjelgaard, M. M., & Dobroth, K. M. (1996). The influence of prosodic structure on the resolution of temporary syntactic closure ambiguities. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 25, 249271. Spivey, M. J., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1998). Syntactic ambiguity resolution in discourse: Modeling the effects of referential context and lexical frequency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 24(6), 1521-1543. Spivey, M. J., Tanenhaus, M. K., Eberhard, K. M., & Sedivy, J. C. (2002). Eye movements and spoken language comprehension: Effects of visual context on syntactic ambiguity resolution. Cognitive Psychology, 45(4), 447-481. 21 Spivey-Knowlton, M., & Sedivy, J. (1995). Resolving attachment ambiguities with multiple constraints. Cognition, 55, 227-267. Spivey-Knowlton, M. J., Trueswell, J. C., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1993). Context effects in syntactic ambiguity resolution: Discourse and semantic influences in parsing reduced relative clauses. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 47, 276-309. St. John, M. F., & McClelland, J. L. (1990). Learning and applying contextual constraints in sentence comprehension. Artificial Intelligence, 46, 217-257. Steedman, M. (1999). Connectionist sentence processing in perspective. Cognitive Science, 23(4), 615-634. Steinhauer, K. (2003). Electrophysiological correlates of prosody and punctuation. Brain & Language, 86(1), 142-164. Steinhauer, K., Alter, K., & Friederici, A. D. (1999). Brain potentials indicate immediate use of prosodic cues in natural speech processing. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 191-195. Steinhauer, K., & Friederici, A. D. (2001). Prosodic boundaries, comma rules, and brain responses: The closure positive shift in ERPs as a universal marker for prosodic phrasing in listeners and readers. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30(3), 267-295. Steinhauer, K., Mecklinger, A., Friederici, A. D., & Meyer, M. (1997). Probability and strategy: An ERP study on the processing of syntactic anomalies [in German]. Zeitschrift fur Experimentelle Psychologie, 44(2), 305-331. Stevenson, S., & Merlo, P. (1997). Lexical structure and parsing complexity. Language & Cognitive Processes, 12(2-3), 349-399. Stewart, A. J., Pickering, M. J., & Sanford, A. J. (2000). The time course of the influence of implicit causality information: Focusing versus integration accounts. Journal of Memory & Language, 42(3), 423-443. Stirling, L., & Wales, R. (1996). Does prosody support or direct sentence processing? Language & Cognitive Processes, 11(1-2), 193-212. Stowe, L. A. (1986). Parsing WH-constructions: Evidence for on-line gap location. Language and Cognitive Processes, 1, 227-245. Stowe, L. A. (1988). Thematic structures and sentence comprehension. In G. N. Carlson & M. K. Tanenhaus (Eds.), Linguistic structure in language processing (pp. 319-354). Dordrecht: Reidel. Stowe, L. A., Broere, C. A. J., Paans, A. M. J., Wijers, A. A., Mulder, G., Vaalburg, W., et al. (1998). Localizing components of a complex task: Sentence processing and working memory. Neuroreport, 9(13), 2995-2999. Stowe, L. A., Paans, A. M. J., Wijers, A. A., & Zwarts, F. (2004). Activations of "motor" and other nonlanguage structures during sentence comprehension. Brain & Language, 89(2), 290-299. Stowe, L. A., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Carlson, G. N. (1991). Filling gaps on-line: Use of lexical and semantic information in sentence processing. Language and Speech, 34, 319-340. Straub, K., Wilson, C., McCollum, C., & Badecker, W. (2001). Prosodic structure and wh-questions. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30(4), 379-394. Streb, J., Hennighausen, E., & Rosler, F. (2004). Different anaphoric expressions are investigated by eventrelated brain potentials. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 33(3), 175-201. Streb, J., Rosler, F., & Hennighausen, E. (1999). Event-related responses to pronoun and proper name anaphors in parallel and nonparallel discourse structures. Brain & Language, 70(2), 273-286. Stromswold, K., Caplan, D., Alpert, N., & S.Rauch. (1996). Localization of syntactic comprehension by positron emission tomography. Brain & Language, 52, 452-473. Sturt, P. (2003). A new look at the syntax-discourse interface: The use of binding principles in sentence processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 32(2), 125-139. Sturt, P. (2003). The time-course of the application of binding constraints in reference resolution. Journal of Memory and Language, 48(3), 542-562. Sturt, P., Costa, F., Lombardo, V., & Frasconi, P. (2003). Learning first-pass structural attachment preferences with dynamic grammars and recursive neural networks. Cognition, 88(2), 133-169. Sturt, P., & Crocker, M. W. (1996). Monotonic syntactic processing: A cross-linguistic study of attachment and reanalysis. Language & Cognitive Processes, 11(5), 449-494. 22 Sturt, P., & Crocker, M. W. (1997). Thematic monotonicity. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 26(3), 297-322. Sturt, P., Pickering, M. J., & Crocker, M. W. (1999). Structural change and reanalysis difficulty in language comprehension. Journal of Memory & Language, 40(1), 136-150. Sturt, P., Pickering, M. J., & Crocker, M. W. (2000). Search strategies in syntactic reanalysis. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29(2), 183-194. Sturt, P., Pickering, M. J., Scheepers, C., & Crocker, M. W. (2001). The preservation of structure in language comprehension: Is reanalysis the last resort? Journal of Memory & Language, 45(2), 283-307. Sturt, P., Scheepers, C., & Pickering, M. (2002). Syntactic ambiguity resolution after initial misanalysis: The role of recency. Journal of Memory and Language, 46(2), 371-390. Sussman, R. S., & Sedivy, J. C. (2003). The time-course of processing syntactic dependencies: Evidence from eye movements. Language & Cognitive Processes, 18(2), 143-163. Tabor, W., Galantucci, B., & Richardson, D. (2004). Effects of merely local syntactic coherence on sentence processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 50(4), 355-370. Tabor, W., & Hutchins, S. (2004). Evidence for self-organized sentence processing: Digging-in effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 30(2), 431-450. Tabor, W., Juliano, C., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1997). Parsing in a dynamical system: An attractor-based account of the interaction of lexical and structural constraints in sentence processing. Language & Cognitive Processes, 12(2-3), 211-271. Tabor, W., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1999). Dynamical models of sentence processing. Cognitive Science, 23(4), 491-515. Takazawa, S., Takahashi, N., Nakagome, K., Kanno, O., Hagiwara, H., Nakajima, H., et al. (2002). Early components of event-related potentials related to semantic and syntactic processes in the Japanese language. Brain Topography, 14(3), 169-177. Tamaoka, K., Sakai, H., Kawahara, J., & Miyaoka, Y. (2003). The effects of phrase-length order and scrambling in the processing of visually presented Japanese sentences. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 32(4), 431-454. Tamaoka, K., Sakai, H., Kawahara, J., Miyaoka, Y., Lim, H., & Koizumi, M. (2005). Priority information used for the processing of Japanese sentences: Thematic roles, case particles or grammatical functions? Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 34(3), 281-332. Tanenhaus, M. K., Spivey-Knowlton, M. J., Eberhard, K. M., & Sedivy, J. C. (1995). Integration of visual and linguistic information in spoken language comprehension. Science, 268(5217), 1632-1634. Taraban, R., & McClelland, J. L. (1988). Constituent attachment and thematic role assignment in sentence processing: Influences of cont-based expectation. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 597-563. Thornton, R., MacDonald, M. C., & Arnold, J. E. (2000). The concomitant effects of phrase length and informational content in sentence comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29(2), 195203. Thornton, R., MacDonald, M. C., & Gil, M. (1999). Pragmatic constraint on the interpretation of complex noun phrases in Spanish and English. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 25(6), 1347-1365. Tong, Y. X., Gandour, J., Talavage, T., Wong, D., Dzemidzic, M., Xu, Y. S., et al. (2005). Neural circuitry underlying sentence-level linguistic prosody. Neuroimage, 28(2), 417-428. Traxler, M. J. (2002). Plausibility and subcategorization preference in children's processing of temporarily ambiguous sentences: Evidence from self-paced reading. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55A, 75-96. Traxler, M. J. (2005). Plausibility and verb subcategorization in temporarily ambiguous sentences: Evidence from self-paced reading. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 34(1), 1-30. Traxler, M. J., Bybee, M. D., & Pickering, M. J. (1997). Influence of connectives on language comprehension: Eye-tracking evidence for incremental interpretation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50A, 481-497. 23 Traxler, M. J., McElree, B., Williams, R. S., & Pickering, M. J. (2005). Context effects in coercion: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 53(1), 1-25. Traxler, M. J., Morris, R. K., & Seely, R. E. (2002). Processing subject and object relative clauses: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 47(1), 69-90. Traxler, M. J., & Pickering, M. J. (1996a). Case-marking in the parsing of complement sentences: Evidence from eye movements. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A(Human Experimental Psychology), 991-1004. Traxler, M. J., & Pickering, M. J. (1996b). Plausibility and the processing of unbounded dependencies: An eye-tracking study. Journal of Memory & Language, 35(3), 454-475. Traxler, M. J., Pickering, M. J., & Clifton, C. (1998). Adjunct attachment is not a form of lexical ambiguity resolution. Journal of Memory & Language, 39(4), 558-592. Traxler, M. J., Pickering, M. J., & McElree, B. (2002). Coercion in sentence processing: evidence from eyemovements and self-paced reading. Journal of Memory and Language, 47(4), 530-547. Traxler, M. J., Sanford, A. J., Aked, J. P., & Moxey, L. M. (1997). Processing causal and diagnostic statements in discourse. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 23(1), 88-101. Traxler, M. J., Williams, R. S., Blozis, S. A., & Morris, R. K. (2005). Working memory, animacy, and verb class in the processing of relative clauses. Journal of Memory and Language, 53(2), 204-224. Tree, J. E. F., & Meijer, P. J. A. (2000). Untrained speakers' use of prosody in syntactic disambiguation and listeners' interpretations. Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung, 63(1), 1-13. Trueswell, J. C. (1996). The role of lexical frequency in syntactic ambiguity resolution. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 566-585. Trueswell, J. C., & Kim, A. E. (1998). How to prune a garden path by nipping it in the bud: Fast priming of verb argument structure. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 102-123. Trueswell, J. C., Sekerina, I., Hill, N. M., & Logrip, M. L. (1999). The kindergarten-path effect: studying online sentence processing in young children. Cognition, 73(2), 89-134. Trueswell, J. C., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1994). Toward a lexicalist framework for constraint-based syntactic ambiguity resolution. In C. Clifton, K. Rayner & L. Frazier (Eds.), Perspectives on sentence processing (pp. 155-180). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Trueswell, J. C., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Kello, C. (1993). Verb-specific constraints in sentence processing: Separating effects of lexical preference from garden-paths. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 528-553. Trueswell, J. T., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Garnsey, S. M. (1994). Semantic influences on parsing: Use of thematic role information in syntactic ambiguity resolution. Journal of Memory & Language, 33, 285318. Tyler, L. K., & Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (1977). The on-line effects of semantic context on syntactic processing. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16, 683-692. Uehara, K. (1997). Judgments of processing load in Japanese: The effect of NP-ga sequences. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 26, 255-263. Ueno, M., & Kluender, R. (2003). Event-related brain indices of Japanese scrambling. Brain & Language, 86(2), 243-271. Ullman, M. T. (2001). The declarative/procedural model of lexicon and grammar. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30(1), 37-69. Ullman, M. T. (2004). Contributions of memory circuits to language: the declarative/procedural model [Review]. Cognition, 92(1-2), 231-270. van Berkum, J. J. A., Brown, C. M., & Hagoort, P. (1999a). Early referential context effects in sentence processing: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Journal of Memory & Language, 41(2), 147182. van Berkum, J. J. A., Brown, C. M., & Hagoort, P. (1999b). When does gender constrain parsing? Evidence from ERPs. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 28(5), 555-571. 24 van Berkum, J. J. A., Brown, C. M., Hagoort, P., & Zwitserlood, P. (2003). Event-related brain potentials reflect discourse-referential ambiguity in spoken language comprehension. Psychophysiology, 40(2), 235-248. van Berkum, J. J. A., Brown, C. M., Zwitserlood, P., Kooijman, V., & Hagoort, P. (2005). Anticipating upcoming words in discourse: Evidence from ERPs and reading times [Review]. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 31(3), 443-467. van Berkum, J. J. A., Hagoort, P., & Brown, C. M. (1999). Semantic integration in sentences and discourse: Evidence from the N400. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11(6), 657-671. van Berkum, J. J. A., Zwisterlood, P., Hagoort, P., & Brown, C. M. (2003). When and how do listeners relate a sentence to the wider discourse? Evidence from the N400 effect. Cognitive Brain Research, 17(3), 701-718. van Dyke, J. A., & Lewis, R. L. (2003). Distinguishing effects of structure and decay on attachment and repair: A cue-based parsing account of recovery from misanalyzed ambiguities. Journal of Memory and Language, 49(3), 285-316. van Gompel, R. P. G., & Majid, A. (2004). Antecedent frequency effects during the processing of pronouns. Cognition, 90(3), 255-264. van Gompel, R. P. G., & Pickering, M. J. (2001). Reanalysis in sentence processing: Evidence against current constraint-based and two-stage models. Journal of Memory & Language, 45(2), 225-258. van Gompel, R. P. G., Pickering, M. J., Pearson, J., & Liversedge, S. P. (2005). Evidence against competition during syntactic ambiguity resolution. Journal of Memory and Language, 52(2), 284-307. van Herten, M., Kolk, H. H. J., & Chwilla, D. J. (2005). An ERP study of P600 effects elicited by semantic anomalies. Cognitive Brain Research, 22(2), 241-255. Van Petten, C., & Bloom, P. (1999). Speech boundaries, syntax and the brain. Nature Neuroscience, 2(2), 103-104. Vandenberghe, R., Nobre, A. C., & Price, C. J. (2002). The response of left temporal cortex to sentences. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(4), 550-560. Vos, S. H., & Friederici, A. D. (2003). Intersentential syntactic context effects on comprehension: the role of working memory. Cognitive Brain Research, 16(1), 111-122. Vos, S. H., Gunter, T. C., Kolk, H. H. J., & Mulder, G. (2001). Working memory constraints on syntactic processing: An electrophysiological investigation. Psychophysiology, 38(1), 41-63. Vos, S. H., Gunter, T. C., Schriefers, H., & Friederici, A. D. (2001). Syntactic parsing and working memory: The effects of syntactic complexity, reading span, and concurrent load. Language & Cognitive Processes, 16(1), 65-103. Vosse, T., & Kempen, G. (2000). Syntactic structure assembly in human parsing: A computational model based on competitive inhibition and a lexicalist grammar. Cognition, 75(2), 105-143. Wales, R., & Toner, H. (1979). Intonation and ambiguity. In W. E. Cooper & E. C. T. Walker (Eds.), Sentence processing (pp. 136-158). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Walker, E. (1976). Some grammatical relations among words. In R. J. Wales & E. Walker (Eds.), New approaches to language mechanisms (pp. 183-201). Amsterdam: North Holland. Walker, J. P., Fongemie, K., & Daigle, T. (2001). Prosodic facilitation in the resolution of syntactic ambiguities in subjects with left and right hemisphere damage. Brain & Language, 78(2), 169-196. Wanner, E. (1980). The ATN and the sausage machine: Which one is baloney? Cognition, 8, 209-225. Warner, J., & Glass, A. (1987). Context and distance-to-disambiguation effects in ambiguity resolution. Journal of Memory and Language, 26, 714-738. Warren, P. (1996). Prosody and parsing: An introduction. Language & Cognitive Processes, 11(1-2), 1-16. Warren, T., & Gibson, E. (2002). The influence of referential processing on sentence complexity. Cognition, 85(1), 79-112. Warren, T., & Gibson, E. (2005). Effects of NP type in reading cleft sentences in English. Language & Cognitive Processes, 20(6), 751-767. Wartenburger, I., Heekeren, H. R., Burchert, F., Heinemann, S., De Bleser, R., & Villringer, A. (2004). Neural correlates of syntactic transformations. Human Brain Mapping, 22(1), 72-81. 25 Waters, G., Caplan, D., Alpert, N., & Stanczak, L. (2003). Individual differences in rCBF correlates of syntactic processing in sentence comprehension: effects of working memory and speed of processing. Neuroimage, 19(1), 101-112. Waters, G. S., & Caplan, D. (1996a). The capacity theory of sentence comprehension - Critique of Just and Carpenter (1992). Psychological Review, 103(4), 761-772. Waters, G. S., & Caplan, D. (1996b). The measurement of verbal working memory capacity and its relation to reading comprehension. 49, 51-79. Waters, G. S., & Caplan, D. (1996c). Processing resource capacity and the comprehension of garden path sentences. 24, 342-355. Waters, G. S., Caplan, D., & Dufault, C. (2000). The relationship between working memory capacity and online sentence processing efficiency in patients with DAT. Brain & Language, 74(3), 332-334. Watson, D., & Gibson, E. (2005). Intonational phrasing and constituency in language production and comprehension. Studia Linguistica, 59(2-3), 279-300. Watt, S. M., & Murray, W. S. (1996). Prosodic form and parsing commitments. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 25, 291-318. Weber, A., Grice, M., & Crocker, M. W. (2006). The role of prosody in the interpretation of structural ambiguities: A study of anticipatory eye movements. Cognition, In Press, Corrected Proof. Weckerly, J., & Kutas, M. (1999). An electrophysiological analysis of animacy effects in the processing of object relative sentences. Psychophysiology, 36(5), 559-570. Weiss, S., & Mueller, H. M. (2003). The contribution of EEG coherence to the investigation of language. Brain & Language, 85(2), 325-343. Weyerts, H., Penke, M., Munte, T. F., Heinze, H. J., & Clahsen, H. (2002). Word order in sentence processing: An experimental study of verb placement in German. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 31(3), 211-268. Whaley, C. P. (1979). Predictive analysis vs. segmentational analysis in sentence perception. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 8, 523-542. Wicha, N. Y. Y., Moreno, E. M., & Kutas, M. (2004). Anticipating words and their gender: An event-related brain potential study of semantic integration, gender expectancy, and gender agreement in Spanish sentence reading. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(7), 1272-1288. Wingfield, A. (1975). The intonation-syntax interaction: Prosodic features in perceptual processing of sentences. In A. Cohen & S. G. Nooteboom (Eds.), Structure and processing in speech perception (pp. 146-156). Berlin: Springer. Wolf, F., Gibson, E., & Desmet, T. (2004). Discourse coherence and pronoun resolution. Language & Cognitive Processes, 19(6), 665-675. Xu, J., Kemeny, S., Park, G., Frattali, C., & Braun, A. (2005). Language in context: emergent features of word, sentence, and narrative comprehension. Neuroimage, 25(3), 1002-1015. Yamashita, H. (1995). The use of verb argument information used in a pro-drop language. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 24, 333-347. Yamashita, H. (1997). The effects of word-order and case marking information on the processing of Japanese. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 26, 163-188. Yamashita, H. (2000). Structural computation and the role of morphological markings in the processing of Japanese. Language & Speech, 43(Part 4), 429-459. Yang, C. L., Gordon, P. C., Hendrick, R., & Hue, C. W. (2003). Constraining the comprehension of pronominal expressions in Chinese. Cognition, 86(3), 283-315. Yang, C. L., Gordon, P. C., Hendrick, R., Wu, J. T., & Chou, T. L. (2001). The processing of coreference for reduced expressions in discourse integration. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30(1), 21-35. Yerkovich, F. R., Walker, C. H., & Blackman, H. S. (1979). The role of presupposed and focal information in integrating sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning, 18, 535-548. Zagar, D., Pynte, J., & Raviteau, S. (1997). Evidence for early-closure attachment on first-pass reading times in French. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50A, 421-438. Zurif, E. B. (2000). Syntactic and semantic composition. Brain & Language, 71(1), 261-263. 26