1 The Whys and Whens of Personal Uncertainty David De Cremer Rotterdam School of Management, Erasmus University Constantine Sedikides University of Southampton Address correspondence to David De Cremer, Rotterdam School of Management, Erasmus Centre of Behavioural Ethics (www.erim.nl/behaviouralethics), Erasmus University Rotterdam, Burgemeester Oudlaan 50, 3062 PA Rotterdam, PO Box 1738, 3000 DR Rotterdam, The Netherlands; Email: ddecremer@rsm.nl; Phone: + 31 10 4081953. 2 The Whys and Whens of Personal Uncertainty Van den Bos (in press) argued for the regulatory importance of personal uncertainty. He defined personal uncertainty as “a subjective sense of doubt or instability in self-views, world-views, or the interrelation between the two” and as involving “the implicit and explicit feelings and other subjective reactions people experience as a result of being uncertain about themselves” (p. ###). Given that personal uncertainty is uncomfortable or aversive, people are motivated to reduce it. They do so in a variety of ways, most notably by adhering to cultural worldviews. Personal uncertainty, then, or its reduction, is a self-regulatory process through which people assign value to their daily practices and socio-cultural norms. We concur that personal uncertainty reduction is a critical regulator of a range of social behaviors, and we welcome the contribution to the study of these phenomena by the uncertainty management model (Van den Bos, in press). In our commentary, we will focus on the link between uncertainty reduction and procedural fairness (i.e., the enactment of perceived fair vs. unfair organizational decision-making rules; De Cremer & Sedikides, 2005), and we will highlight two issues that have implications for the model. First, why do people want to reduce their uncertainty? Second, when do people want to maintain or even increase their uncertainty? The Whys of Personal Uncertainty Reduction What do people want to achieve by reducing personal uncertainty? Alternatively, what are the goals people want to promote when attempting to reduce personal uncertainty? And are different types of goals associated with different types of personal 3 uncertainty (Sedikides, De Cremer, Hart, & Brebels, 2010; Sedikides, Hart, & De Cremer, 2008)? Van den Bos (in press) has induced personal uncertainty by instructing participants to describe “the emotions that the thought of your being uncertain arouses in you” and “what you think physically will happen to you as you feel uncertain” (p. ###). This manipulation was originally designed, in part, to disentangle the relative contribution of personal uncertainty versus existential terror on adherence to sociocultural norms, values, or worldviews. As such, this operationalization of personal uncertainty is suited to generic concerns about the social world, and that is why we (De Cremer, Brebels, & Sedikides, 2008) labeled it in our research as general uncertainty. We superimposed then a manipulation of procedural rules in terms of granting or denying organizational members voice (i.e., opinion or input in the decision-making process; Folger, 1977). We reasoned that, in this context, generic uncertainty would activate organizational fairness concerns. Members would adopt the implicit goal of finding out whether their organization is fair or unfair. Being granted voice would be a diagnostic sign of organizational fairness, whereas being denied voice would be a diagnostic sign of organizational unfairness. Personal uncertainty, though, could also refer to one’s social belongingness, what we labeled belongingness uncertainty. We operationalized this type of uncertainty accordingly. We instructed participants to describe “the emotions that the thought of you being uncertain about whether you belong to a group of other people arouses in you” and “what you think physically will happen to you as you feel uncertain about whether you belong to a group of other people” (De Cremer et al., 2008, p. 121). We then 4 superimposed a manipulation of procedural rules in terms of granting or denying members voice. We reasoned that, in this context, belongingness uncertainty would likely activate concerns about one’s inclusionary status in the organization or about the strength of one’s relationships with others. Members would adopt the implicit goal of finding out the extent to which they are embraced by their organization or the extent to which they belong to it. Being granted voice would be a diagnostic sign of belongingness prompting stronger organizational identification, whereas being denied voice would be a diagnostic sign of exclusion prompting organizational disidentification. We obtained support for these hypotheses (De Cremer et al., 2008). When general uncertainty was activated, participants deemed the organizational decision-making procedures as fairer in the case in which they were granted than denied voice. However, their strength of organizational identification was unaffected by procedural rules. Conversely, when belongingness uncertainty was activated, participants identified more strongly with the organization in the case in which they were granted than denied voice. However, their judgments of organizational fairness were unaffected by procedural rules Needless to say, our findings (De Cremer et al., 2008) leave several questions unanswered. Our research, for example, is suggestive rather than conclusive about the mechanisms that drove the obtained results. In particular, goals would need to be directly manipulated either at the implicit or explicit level, and they would need to be so independently of the type of uncertainty (i.e., general vs. belongingness) involved. Yet, these findings call for a more refined specification of many of the effects that Van den Bos (in press) reported. Such an empirical tack can clarify which type of personal uncertainty produces what kinds of effects and why. 5 The Whens of Personal Uncertainty Maintenance or Increase Van den Bos (in press) acknowledged that people do not always attempt to reduce their personal uncertainty. For examples, he states, and cites supportive work for his statements, that “experiencing uncertainty may sometimes be sought out and occasionally may instigate contemplation” (p. ###) and that “I do not want to suggest that people want to reduce personal uncertainty all the time” (p. ###). We submit, however, that more than lip service needs to be paid to this issue, and we discuss how people may maintain or even search for uncertainty. Theorists and researchers from a humanistic perspective (Aron, Aron, & Smollan, 1992; Erikson, 1963; Maslow, 1970) or an achievement framework (Csikszentmihalyi, 1980; Elliot & Thrash, 2001; Meece, Anderman, & Anderman, 2006) have long advocated or demonstrated the benefits of psychological growth and self-expansion as well as of furthering one’s skills and abilities. The notions of growth and improvement are also reflected in cultural discourse (e.g., overcoming personal adversity or rising from humble roots; McAdams, 2006). Self-improvement is indeed a potent human motive (Sedikides & Hepper, in press). It is reflected in preferences for upward feedback trajectories, upward comparison feedback, and feedback that may be self-threatening in the present but useful in the future. Moreover, improvement-oriented feedback increases satisfaction or positive affect and leads to better performance. The self-improvement motive may underlie, in part, such life-style decisions as relocation, occupational change, relationship dissolution, friendship formation, and initiation of hobbies or interests. People can be consciously aware of their need or desire to improve (Neiss, Sedikides, Shahinfar, & Kupersmidt, 2006; Tuckey, Brewer, & 6 Williamson, 2002). Importantly, they are often also consciously aware of antecedents of their self-improvement efforts such as failure (Taylor, Neter, & Wayment, 1995) or a traumatic event (McFarland & Alvaro, 2000). Given the desirability and cultural relevance of self-improvement, how do people self-regulate in order to initiate or sustain self-improvement strivings? We propose that one important self-regulatory tactic involves personal uncertainty. We assume that people have a rudimentary insight into the relation between uncertainty and improvement: they somehow know that uncertainty, albeit a temporarily uncomfortable state, can be harnessed for long-term improvement purposes. People may then put this insight to use by maintaining their personal uncertainty at current level or even by purposefully increasing their personal uncertainty to a maximum tolerable level. They will endure, if not nourish, high personal uncertainty to the point where their self-improvement efforts have been initiated, are fully operative, or have achieved the desired outcomes (Sedikides & Gregg, 2003; Sedikides & Strube, 1997). This self-regulatory process may be qualified by individual differences: It will likely be more pronounced for persons high in need for cognition, low in need for structure or closure, high in need for achievement, and high in self-esteem or self-compassion. Closing Remarks We built on existing research on the uncertainty management model (Van Den Bos, in press) to offer two suggestions. First, differentiating further the construct of personal uncertainty can pave the way toward profitable empirical pursuits. In a preliminary investigation (De Cremer et al., 2008), we demonstrated that general uncertainty influences different judgmental domains than belongingness uncertainty 7 perhaps by activating different goals. Second, incorporating work on the selfimprovement motive (Sedikides & Hepper, in press) raises questions about the circumstances that result in the maintenance or increase (rather than reduction) of personal uncertainty. We hope that the two suggestions can further strengthen the impact and broaden the scope of the uncertainty management theory. 8 References Aron, A., Aron, E. N., & Smollan, D. (1992). Inclusion of other in the self scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 596-612. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1980). Love and the dynamics of personal growth. In K. S. Pope (Ed.), On love and loving (pp. 306-326). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. De Cremer, D., Brebels, L., & Sedikides, C. (2008). Being uncertain about what? Procedural fairness effects as a function of general uncertainty and belongingness uncertainty. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 1520-1525. De Cremer, D., & Sedikides, C. (2005). Self-uncertainty and responsiveness to procedural justice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 157-173. Elliot, A. J., & Thrash, T. M. (2001). Achievement goals and the hierarchical model of achievement motivation. Educational Psychology Review, 12, 139-156. Erikson, E. H. (1963). Childhood and society (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Norton.Folger, R. (1977). Distributive and procedural justice: Combined impact of “voice” and improvement of experienced inequity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 108-119. McFarland, C., & Alvaro, C. (2000). The impact of motivation on temporal comparisons: Coping with traumatic events by perceiving personal growth. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 327-343. Maslow, A. (1970). Motivation and personality (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Harper & Row. McAdams, D. P. (2006). The redemptive self: Stories Americans live by. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. 9 Meece, J. L., Anderman, E. M., & Anderman, L. H. (2006). Classroom goal structure, student motivation, and academic achievement. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 487-503. Neiss, M. B., Sedikides, C., Shahinfar, A., & Kupersmidt, J. (2006). Self-evaluation in naturalistic context: The case of juvenile offenders. British Journal of Social Psychology, 45, 499-518. Sedikides, C., De Cremer, D., Hart, C. M., & Brebels, L. (2010). Procedural fairness responses in the context of self-uncertainty. In R. M. Arkin, K. C. Oleson, & P. J. Carroll (Eds.), The uncertain self: A handbook of perspectives from social and personality psychology (pp. 142-159). New York, NY: Psychology Press. Sedikides, C., & Gregg, A. P. (2003). Portraits of the self. In M. A. Hogg & J. Cooper (Eds.), Sage handbook of social psychology (pp. 110-138). London, England: Sage Publications. Sedikides, C., Hart, C., M., & De Cremer, D. (2008). The self in procedural fairness. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2, 2107-2124. Sedikides, C., & Hepper, E. G. D. (in press). Self-improvement. Social and Personality Psychology Compass. Sedikides, C., & Strube, M. J. (1997). Self-evaluation: To thine own self be good, to thine own self be sure, to thine own self be true, and to thine own self be better. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 29, 209-269. New York, NY: Academic Press. Taylor, S. E., Neter, E., & Wayment, H. A. (1995). Self-evaluation processes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 1278-1287. 10 Tuckey, M., Brewer, N., & Williamson, P. (2002). The influence of motives and goal orientation on feedback seeking. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 75, 195-216.