Antipredator aspects of fallow deer bebaviour during calving season at Doñana National Park (Spain) C. SAN JOSÉ and F. BRAZA Estación Bioldgica de Dotiana, CSIC Apdo 1056, 41080 Sevilla, Spain Received 26 June 1990, accepted 1 March 1991 This study evaluates some morphological, ecological and behavioural aspects of fallow deer mothers and fawns during the calving season in order to know the antipredator tactics selected in this species living in a particularly open hahitat at Doñana National Park. In this area (allow deer give birth in synchrony during the first fortnight of June. At this time they move away from their matriarchal groups. A concentration of births in the early afternoon was detected, coinciding with the minimal activity of predators in the area. Mothers groom their offspring and ingest the birth remains to prevent the attraction of predators. During the first days of life fawns remain hidden keeping motionless. KEY WORDS: Dama dama, antipredator strategy, carving time, maternal behaviour. Introduction Materials and methods 139 140 Resulrs 143 Discussion 145 146 147 Acknowledgements References iNTRODUCTION The fawns in many species of ungulates are specially liable to suffer from predation during their first days of life. However, only a few works exist about the mechanisms of developing an antipredator strategy at calving time, probably due to the difficulty to observing mothers and their young at birth and during the days immediately after birth. Mothers and newborn young in those species use a ehiden> strategy (elk, ALTMANN 1932; fallow deer, GILBERT 1968; Coke’s bartebeest, GosLiNG 1969; bontebok, DAvID 1973, 1975; pronghorn, KITCHEN 1974; red deer, GIJINNESS et al. 1978; red buck, IRBY 1979). The species of these groups are basically classified as *cbidero or ((followers> depending on whether the newborn lie concealed for their first few clays or actively follow their mothers (LENT 1974, RAI.i.s et al. 1986). While <<following>> has been viewed as a strategy for avoiding predators in open habitats, hidinga is thought to reduce predation risk in closed habitats (LENT 1974, ESTES & ESTES 1979). As to the behaviour of fallow deer during the calving season, on the basis of the studies we have conducted to date (ALvA.aEz et a!. 1975, BitAzA et al. 1988, SAN Jose 1988, BItizA & SAN Jose 1989), we may classify fallow deer in principle as ehider>’ since the young remain hidden the first days after birth and are only visited periodically by their mothers, mainly to feed. Nevertheless, morphological, ecological and behavioural aspects, which decide this assignment, have not been evaluated quantitatively up to now. The aim of our study was to evaluate some of these aspects in mothers and fawns during the calving season and to discuss the results in relation to the antipredator strategies developed by this species and the physical characteristics of the habitat. MATERiALS AND METHODS Observations included in this paper were carried out at Doñana National Park from 1982 to 1987. In this National Park of 73,000 ha (Fig. I) situated at the mouth of the river Guadatquivir, the wild fallow deer population occupies an ecotone zone which corresponds to the transition between the xerophyric schrub and the marshland (BRAZA 1975). In this ecorone we distinguish between the following biotopes (according to ALLIER er al. 1974, and AGuIr.AR AMAT et al. 1979) (Fig.1): a) marshes: characterized by a strong seasonal dependence and made up of species like Salicornia ramosissima, Arthrocnemum sp. and Scispus maritimus; bI rush areas: form a mosaic wits pastures at the marshland borders and present a dominant vegetation of macus marit&nus; c) wet meadows: areas of pastures which are occasionally flooded and basically characterized by the association of Amzena gaditana and 44sphodelus aestivus; d) dry meadows: have a lower level of the subterranean water and present a characteristic association of the species Urinea maritima and Anthemis cotula; e) bracken areas: areas with a dense plant cover where the species Pseridium aquilinum dominates; f) schrubland: is made up of vegetation which colonized stabilized sands and is basically characterized by species like Halirnium halimifolium, Cistus libanotis, Erica scoparia, Calluna vulgaris and Stauracanthus genistoides, among which some isolated cork oaks (Quercus suber) are found. At present, the Iberian lynx (Lynx pardina) is the only predator able to kill half-grown deer in Doñana and, as we know, the lynx has very little influence on the early mortaliry of fallow deer fawns (BELTRAN et al. 1985). Of other potential predators of f allow deer calves (fox, genet, ichneumon, wildboar, Imperial eagle) rhe analyses do not reveal significant deer presence in their diet (VENERO 1982, PAL0MARE5 1986, RAu 1988, FERRER 1989). During 4 years (1983-1986), from the 25th of May to the 20th of June we conducted a census by car at least once a week a fraction of the ecotone area which measured 5.5 km long and 1.5 km wide (Fig.1). Generally two observers record the number of solitary females (animals char were ar least 50 away from any other fallow deer). The proportion of solitary females was calculated with respect ro the rotal number of females present in the area the same day. The mean population size during the period 1983-1986 was 264.44 (* 7.89) deer wirh a mean total number of females of 123.92 (±6.45). The mean calf-ratio was 60.37 (±2.05) (from BRAZA et al. 1990). The rutting season takes place during the first fortnight of October (BRAZA er al. 1986). intipredator behaviour in fallow deer ri 141 Area of Census Fig. 1. Doñana National Park and study area (above). The main biotopes considered in the study arc roportion ol the ecorone zone (below). — C. San José and F. Braza 142 198a > Ct 1984 0 U) 0 1985 30 - 20 10 L I 25 May Fig. 2 5 15 June P 1986 I 25 5 July Variations in the percentage of solitary females Females give birth to a single fawn at the end of spring (BRAZA er al. 1988), after a gestation period o 236 days (SAN Jos 1988), During the calving season, in the area of census we searched daily to localize newborn fawns. This was done on foot and sometimes counting on the assistance of a vehicle or a guard on Antipredator behaviour in fallow deer 143 horseback. When we found a fawn we recorded all the environmental characteristics of the place where it was hidden as well as the behaviour shown by the mother (if nearby) and the fawn at our approach. Once captured the fawn was marked and measured (BRAZA et aI. 1988, SAN josI et al. 1989). When it was not possible to detect the moment of birth we used various criteria to estimate the condition vnewbornn, e.g. the presence of remains of the placenta, the still moist fur of the fawn, the umbilical cord’s degree of healing, the hardening of the hooves and the presence of cartilage on their points. These criteria have frequently been used to determine the age of newborn fawns of other deer species (i.e. HAUGEN & SnAKE 1958). Finally, in order to make more detailed observations, we followed in a ‘main observation area’ (aprox. 150 ha) the movements of the pregnant females. This control was carried out by means of a binocular telescope (20(40) x (80(500) from a 30 m high observation tower continually from dawn to nightfall. We used non parametric statistics (SIEGEL 1956) to evaluate the level of significance of the results. RESULTS A sudden increase in the proportion of solitary females was observed each year at the beginning of June (Fig. 2). The distributions of solitary females in different years are coincident (83 X 84, 84 X 85, 85 X 86, t = 1; P< 0.01; KendalL Coefficience of Concordance). Only 108 birth dates of the 190 fawns captured could be known with precision. The peak of solitary females coincides with the average date of birth estimated. With the exception of 1982, with a mean date of birth the 5th of June, for the rest of the years (1983-1987) most of births occurred within the 3 first days of June: 1=1 (±3.7), isiS (±3.8), *=3 (±2.2), Yc=2 (±3.8) and i=2.5 (±4.3) respectively. In 41 cases it was possible to record exactly the time of birth. From these cases we could calculate the average hour which is 14:16 (± 3.16). Most births (x = 25.54; P’cO.OOi) were detected between 15:00 hr and 17:00 hr (Universal time) (Fig.3). In all the births observed in their entirity (n = 20), the mother turns towards the fawn, which Ties on the ground immediately after birth, and starts to lick it actively, removing and ingesting the amniotic membranes which still cover the young. Finally, D 0 C? cy CC C C) C.) a) 3 — Distribution of births over the day’ight hourt (Universal time) C. San José and F. Braza 144 rush marsh dry meadow ‘wet meadow Ei schrub bracken Habitat preference shown by femaLes for Fig. 4. hiding their fawn (n = 190). the female ingests the newborn’s faeces. The newborn fawn has a dark brown coat with white speckles on its back, the belly being entirely white. Its colour gets progressively lighter in the course of the 1st week of life. During the first days of life the fawn lies hidden. Its mother lies down some metres off or goes away to join a group or to graze alone. The biotope of the hidding place was not selected randomly (xi = 511.45; P<0.001). Most of the fawns captured were found in a rush area Cx? = 467.46; P<O.0O1) (Fig.4). The fawn remains motionless and adopts the typical flat-on-the-ground posture, frequently curled up totally concealing its extremities and head, although the tendency to run away when a person is approaching rapidly is correlated with the fawn’s age (r I; P<0.001) (Fig. 5), increasing and surpassing a probability of 50% from the 3rd day of life. 01190 fawns captured, for 89 it was possible to observe the mother’s behaviour when we approached the fawn. In 76% of all cases the female went away from the place where the young was hidden. In 28 cases the female stayed near the place where the young was being marked at an average distance of 72.5 m (±45.2). / no flight Itighi lawn Eig. 5. age (days) TemporaL variation in the fawn’s reaction at the approach of a person on foot Antipredator behaviour in fallow deer 145 When analysing the fawns’ and mothers’ behaviour in the main biotopes (marshes, rush areas and meadows) the tendency was similar; so, the flight probability by fawns younger than 4 days was significantly low (*=0.12; SD=0.12; x 5.44; P<0.05, yJ = 14.74; P<O.O1 and Ic = 12; Pc 0.01 repectively); the mother’s flight tendency was high in the three biotopes (* = 0.74; SD = 0.36) but due to the small 11; P<o.01). sample it was significant only for rush areas (x? Finally, counting on 13 cases of fawns ohserved daily, the average date of integration into the group was calculated. The criterion being the age of the young when seen in a group for the second time. This value was of 13.61 days (±4.31). DISCUSSION Most of fallow deer births take place in Doñana during the first fortnight of June. In this characteristic the population coincides with most deer populations in temperate areas presenting a reproductive period which is markedly seasonal and extraordinarily synchronized (Vos 1960, MITCHELL & LINCOLN 1973, BERGERUD 1975, CLUTTON-BROCK & GUINNESS 1975). Two general hypothesis are invoked to explain birth synchrony: (i) optimal timing with respect to season may enhance the survival and growth of offspring as well as the survival and future reproductive success of the mother (DAUPHINE & MCCLURE 1974, BUNNELL 1980, DUNBAR 1980, CLUTTON-BROCIC et al. 1982, RUTBERG 1987) and (ii) females may synchronize births in order to reduce predation on newborns either by satiating or confusing predators (DAUPHINE & MCCLURE 1974, EsTEs & ESTES 1979, LOTT 1981, BERGERUD et a!. 1984). With respect to predators, as already mentioned in the Introduction, at present there are no large predators in the area and there is only the lynx (Lynx pardina) to be mentioned, which attacks young individuals in autumn and winter, affecting mortality rates to a minimum degree (DELIBES 1980, BELTRAN et al. 1985). However, until the forties there were wolves (Canis lupus) living in Doñana; so, predation was an important force selecting for breeding synchrony in fallow deer. Furthermore, the cycle of forage availability, in an area like Donana with a very short spring and a very dry summer, is also probably an important factor selecting the time of the calving season. The fact that none of the offspring of late breeding survived the summer seems to confirm this hypothesis. As in other ungulate species (i.e. FESTA-BIANCHET 1988) inadequate nutrition is suggested as the cause of mortality of late newborn calves. At Doñana a peak of births has been detected coinciding with the period of minimal diurnal activity of predators in the area (DELIBES 1980, ALVAREZ et al. 1983, PALOMARES 1986, BELTRAN 1988, RAU 1988). A similar diurnal birth distribution has been described for other fallow deer populations (CHAPMAN & CHAPMAN 1975, STERBA & KLOSAK 1984) and for other ungulate species (E5TEs 1966, GOSLING 1969). ESTES (1966) suggests that the morning peak allows the calves to gain coordination before night fall, when predators are more active, but, since we have no data from nocturnal observations, in our case it is not possible hypothesize. The rapid removal of the placenta by placentophagia also lessens the possibility that predators will be attracted to the slowly-developing calf. In some species a diurnal distribution of breeding has been interpreted as a possible thermal regulation C. San José and F. Braza 146 (Len 1981) but we think that this could be less adaptative in Doñana where the mean minimal temperature itt May-June never falls bellow 10 OC in June (i=z12.51*1.72 °C during the period 1983-1986). The efficiency of *clying-outn as an antipredator behaviour is increased by the initial cleaning of the fawn and by the mother consuming his faeces. These factors reduce the chance of predator detecting a calf by its smell. This behaviour has been reported for many ungulate species (GOSLING 1969, ESPMARK 1971, JACKSON et al. 1972, KITCHEN 1974, AUTENRIETH & FICHTER 1975, GUBERNIK 1980, TRILLMICH 1981, SADLEIR 1984). On the basis of behavioural aspects of antipredator tactics of fallow deer during the calving season, these species could be classified as <<hider since the newborn lie concealed for their first few days of life. Accompanied by the concealment of young calves, the isolation of the female reduces the chance of predation by making detection by predators more difficult. This behaviour is usually shown by species that calve on shrublands which vegetation provides a good concealment (ALTMANN 1963, HAWKINS & KLIMSTRA 1970, JACKSoN et al. 1972, Wmm et al. 1972, HEIDEMANN 1973, MEIER 1973, CHAPMAN & CHAPMAN 1975, CLUTTON-BROCK & GUINNESS 1975, NELSON & MEal 1981, OZOGA & VERME 1986). Females of species such as wildebeest that calve on short-grass areas which offer less conceal, do not seek isolation for parturition and there is no lying-out behaviour (ESTES 1966). In Doñana, fallow deer have adapted a <<hiden> strategy to an open habitat, selecting the rush areas as the biotope that offers the highest cover, and then displaying a typical thiders. behaviour as shown by other fallow deer living in forests (GanERT 1968, CHAPMAN & CHAPMAN 1973). On the other hand, the cryptic colour of the fallow deer newborn calves and the particular position that they adopt lying-out could be interpreted as a complete strategy of camouflage (TINBERGEN et al. 1967) and permits an understanding of the motionless behaviour displayed in the open biotopes (marshes and meadows). Furthermore, the success of the <hider, strategy in ungulates depends in part on the mother’s habitity to minimize the information she transmits about her young’s hiding place while remaining close enough to distract or drive away a predator (ALTMANN 1963, LANGMAN 1977, MACCONNELL-YOUNT & SMITH 1978, PRATT & ANDERSON 1979, TRUE’rr 1979, STEIGERS & FLINDERS 1981, BYERS & BYERS 1983). At the approach of a potential predator (a person on foot) most fallow deer mothers run away from the place where the young is hidden, attracting the attention of the predator by their flight. Finally, the effectiveness of lying-out in a very young calf depends on a flight distance of almost zero (TINBERGEN et al, 1967, GOSLKNG 1969). A predator may then pass close by without the calf revealing its presence by flight. In contrast, a few days later, fallow deer calves jump and run away when approached to within a few metres. This is probably correlated with improved chance of escape from predators by flight. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This study was supported by the Comislén Ioterininisterial de Ciencias and the Junta de Andalucia. y TecnologIa (C1CYT) Antipredator behaviour in fallow deer 147 REFERENCES AGIJILAR AMATJ., MONTES C., RAMIREZ L. & TORRES A. 1979. Pargue Nacionat de Doñana. Mapa ecoidgico. Madrid: Instituto para Ia Conservacio’n de La Naturaleza. ALLIER C., GONZALEZ BERNALDEZ F. & RAMIREZ L. 1974. Donana. Mapa ecologico. Sevill.a: Estacidn Biolc5gica de Doiiana (Consejo Superior de Investigaciones CientIficas). M. 1952. Social behavior of elk, Cervus canadensis nelsoni, in the Jackson Hole area of Wyoming. Behaviour 4: 111-143. ALTMANN M. 1963. Naturalistic studies of maternal care in moose and elk, pp. 233-253. In: Rheinfold ML., Edit. Maternal behavior in mammals. London: Rheinf old H.L. ALVAREZ F.) BEAm F., AZCARATE T., AGUILERA E. & MAaTIN R. 1983. Circadian rhythms in a vertebrate community of Doñana National Park. Antis XV Congreso Internacional Fauna Cinegdtica Silvestre 15: 379-387. ALVAREZ F., BRA7.A F. & NORZAGARAY A. 1975. Etograma cuantificado dcl game (Dama dama) en libertad. Doilana Acres Vertebrata 2: 93-142. AUTENRIETSI R.E. &. FJCHTER E. 1975. On the behavior and socialization of pronghorn fawns. Wildlife Monographs 42: 1-11. BELTRAN j.F. 1988. Ecologia y conducta espacio-temporal dcl lince ibérico (Lynx pardina) en ci Parque Nacional de Doñana. Tesis Doctora4 tlnivenidad de Sevilla. BELTRAN J.F., SAN Josii C., DELIBES M. & BRAZA F. 1985. An analysis of Iberian lynx predation upon fallow deer in the Coto Doñana, SW Spain. Proceedings of the XVII International Congress of Game Biologists 17: 173-181. BERGERUn A.T. 1975. The reproductive season of Newfoundland caribou. Canadian Journal of Zoology 53: 1213-1221. BERGERUn A.T., BUTLER 11.E. & MILLER DR. 1984. Antipredator tactics of calving caribou: dispersion in mountains. Canadian Journal of Zoology 62: 1566-1575. BRAZA F. 1975. Censo del gamo Dama dama) en DoEana. Naturalia Hispdnica 3: 1-27. BRAZA F., GARCIAJ.E. & ALVAREZ F. 1986. Rutting behaviour of fallow deer. Acta Theriologica 31: 467-4 78. BRAn F. & SAN Jos C. 1989. An analysis of mother-young behaviour of fallow deer during lactation period. Behavioural Processes 17: 93-106. BRAZA F., SAN Jost C. & BLosa A. 1988. Birth measurements, parturirion dates and progeny sex ratio of Dama dama in Doñana. Journal of Mammalogy 69: 607-610. BRAZA F., SAN Josh C., BLOM A., CASES V. & GARCIA j.E. 1990. Population parameters of fallow deer at DoEana National Park (SW Spain). Acta Theriologica 35: 279-290. BUNNELL T.L. 1980. Factors controlling lambing period of Dali’s sheep. Canadian Journal of Zoology 58: 1027-1031. BIERS J.A. & BIERS K.Z. 1983. Do pronghorn morhers reveal the locations of their fawns? Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 13: 147-156. CHAPMAN D. & CHAPMANN N. 1975. Fallow deer (Dama dama). Lavenham, Suffolk; Dalton T. CLUnON-BROCK T.H. & GUINNESS F,E. 1975. Behaviour of red deer (Cervus elaphus) at calving Lime. Behaviour 55: 286-300. CLunoN-BR0CK T.H., GUINNESS F.E. & ALBON S.D. 1982. Red deer: behaviour and ecology of two sexes. Edinbuh: Edinburgh Vnivenity Press. DAUPHINE T.C. & MCCLURE R.L. 1974. Synchronous mating in Canadian barren-ground caribou. Journal of Wildlife Management 38: 54-66. DAwn J.H.M. 1973. The behaviour of bontebok, Damaliscus dorcas dorcas (Pallas 1766), with special reference to territorial behaviour. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie 33: 38-107. DAvmJ.H.M. 1975. Observations on mating behaviour, patturition, suckling and the mother-young bond in the bontebok (Damaliscus dorcas dorcas). Journal of Zoology, London 177: 203-223. DELIBES M. 1980. Feeding ecology of rhe Spanish lynx in the Coto Doftana. Acta Theriologica 25: 309-324. DUNBAR R.J.M. 1980. Demographics and life-history variables of a population of gelada baboons Theropithecus gelada. Journal of Animal Ecology 49: 485-506. ALTMANN C. San José and F. Braza 148 ESI’MkRK Y. 1971. Mother-young relationship and ontogeny of behaviour in reindeer (Rangifer tarandus L.). Zeitschrif I für Tierpsychologie 29: 42-81. ESTES RD. 1966. Behaviour and life history of the wildebeesr (Connochaetes taurinus Burchell). Nature 212: 999-1000. ESTES RD. & E5TF,s R.K. 1979. The birth and survival of wildebeest calves. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie 50: 45-95. FERRER M. 1989. El águila imperial. Base bibliografica de especics amenazadas. Sevilla: Junta de 1ndalucia, Agencia del Medio Ambiente. FEST&-BI&NCHET M. 1988. Birthdate and survival in bighorn lambs (Ovis canadensis). Journal of Zoology, London 214: 653-661. GILBERT B.K. 1968. Development of social behavior in the fallow deer(Dama dama). Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie 25: 867-876. L.M. 1969. Parturition and related behaviour in coke’s harrebeest (Alcelaphus buselaphus co/eel Gunther). Journal of Reproduction and Fertility 6: 265-286. GUBERNIK D.J. 1980. Maternal imprinting or maternal dabellingm in goats? Animal Behaviour 28: 124-129. GuINNESS FE., GIBSON R.M. & CLUTTON-BROCK Ti-I. 1978. Calving time of red deer (Cerr’us elaphus) on Rhum. Journal of Zoology, London 185: 105-114. FIAUGEN AD.& SPEATCE W.D. 1958. Determining age of young fawn white-railer deer. Journal of Wildlife Management 22: 3 19-321. HAwKINS RE. & KLIMSTRA W.D. 1970. A preliminary study of the social organization of whitetailed deer. Journal of Wildlife Management 34: 407-419. I-IEI0EMANN G. 1973. Zur Biologie des Damwildes (Cervus dama L 1758). Hambmg & Berlin: P. Parey. itaY L.R, 1979. Reproduction in mountain red-buck Redunca fulvoruf ala. Mammalia 43: 191-213. ACKSON R.M., WHITE M. & KNOWLTON F-F. 1972. Activiry patterns of young white-tailed deer lawns in South Texas. Ecology 53: 262-270. (ITCHEN D.W. 1974. Social behavior and ecology of the pronghorn. Wildlife Monographs 38: 1-96. ANGMAN V.A. 1977. Cow-call relationships in giraffe (Giraffa camelopardalis giraf-fa). Zeitschrift für Tie,psychologie 43: 264-286. £NT P.C. 1974. Mother-infant relationships in ungulates, pp. 14-55. In: Geist V. & Walther P.R., Edits. The behaviour of ungulates and its relation ro management. Moges, Switzeland: International Union for Conservation of Nature. OTT D.F. 1981. Sexual behavior and intersexual strategies in American bison. Zeitschrift für Tieqsychologie 56: 97-114. IACCONNELI.-VOUNT E. & SMITH C. 1978. Mule deer-coyote interactions in north central Colorado. Journal of Mammalogy 59: 422-423. IEIF.R E. VON 1973. Beitrage zur geburt des Damwildes (Cervus dama L.). Zeitschrift für Saugetierkunde 38: 348-373. IITCHEL B. & LINCOLN G.A. 1973. Conception dates in relation to age condition in two populations o1 red deer in Scotland. Journal of Zoology, London 171: 14 1-152. IELSON M.E. & ME.CII L.D. 1981. Deer socialization and wolf predation in Norrheastern Minnesota. Wildlife Monographs 77: 1-47. ‘ZOGA JJ. & VERME L.J. 1986. Relation of maternal age to fawn-rearing success in white-tailed deer. Journal of Wildlife Management 50: 480-486. SLOMARES F. 1986. Ecologia de Ia gineta y del meloncillo en el Parque Nacional de Doñana. Tesis de Licenciatura, Universidad de Granada. {ATT D.M. &ANDERSON Vii. 1979. Giraffe cow-calf relationships and social development of the calf in the Serengeti. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie 51: 233-251. &LLS K., KRANZ K. & LUNDRIGAN B. 1986. Mother-young relationships in captive ungulates: variability and clustering. Animal Behaviour 34: 134-145. tu J. 1988. Ecologia del zorro, Vulpes vulpes, en Ia Reserva Biolégica de Donana, Iluelva, SO España. Tesis Doctoral, Universidad de Sevilla. .JTBERG A.T. 1987. Adaptive hypotheses of birth synchrony in ruminants: as interspecific test. The American Naturalist 5: 693-710. GOSLING Antipredator behaviour in fallow deer 149 SADLF.IR R.M.F.S. 1984. Ecological consequence of lactation. Acta Zoologica Pennica 171: 179-182. SAN José C. 1988. Estrategia reproductiva de las hembras de gamo (Dama dama). Tesis Doctoral, Unive&dad Complutense de Madrid. SAN JOSé C., BRAZA F. & VARELA I. 1989. Captura y marcaje de crIas de gamo en ei Parque Nacional de Donana. Actas IX Bienal Real Sociedad Española Historia Natural 9 (1): 289-301. SIEGEL S. 1956. Nonparametric statistics for the behavioral sciences. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Co., 312 pp. STEIGERS W.D. JR & FuNonis J.T. 1981. Mortality and movements of mute deer fawns in Washington. Journal of Wildlife Management 44: 381-388. STERBA 0. & KLOSAK IC. 1984. Reproductive biology of fallow deer, Dama dama. B. Female reproduction. Acta Scientiarum Academiae Scientiarum Bohemoslovacae, Brno 18: 1-46. TINBERGEN N., IMPEKOvEN M. & FRANCE D. 1967. An experimeot on spacing-out as a defence against predation. Behaviour 28: 307-321. TRIu.MIcH F. 1981. Mutual mother-pup recognition in Galapagos fur seals and sea lions: cues used and functional significance. Behaviour 78: 21-42. TRIJETT J.C. 1979. Observations of coyote predation on mule deer fawns in Arizona. Journal of Wildlife Management 43: 956-958. VENERO J.L. 1982. Dicta de los grandes fitófagos silvestres dcl Parque Nacional de Doflana. Tesis Doctora4 tiniversidad de Sevilla. Vos A. nt 1960. Behavior of barren ground caribou on their calving grounds. Journal of Wildlife Management 24: 250-258. WIuTE M., KNOWLTON F.F. & BLAZF.NER W.C. 1972. Effects of dam-newborn fawn behavior on capture and mortality. Journal of Wildlife Management 36: 897-906.