Long Term Memory – Reinforcement Learning Task Name Description Cognitive Construct Validity Probabilistic Reversal Learning Subjects select one stimulus from an array of concurrently presented stimuli (preferably 3); typically, more than one set of discriminanda are presented, in a randomized order across trials. Each choice is followed by positive or negative feedback (correct choice or incorrect choice). The relationship between the stimulus presented and the feedback given can be deterministic or probabilistic. Subjects must, based upon feedback alone, learn which cue in each discriminanda set are associated with positive feedback and then maintain optimized performance of the rule (rule maintenance). Once the subject meets pre-set performance criteria, feedback is adjusted without warning. In other words, the rules are "reversed" in one or more of the discriminanda sets. Subjects must update their behavior based upon the change in rules. Optimal reversal performance is often considered to involve cognitive control over pre-potent responding. Acquisition of a multiple choice visual discrimination is believed to reflect implicit learning processes. Optimal performance of a visual discrimination is thought to reflect rule maintenance. Reversal of a learned visual discrimination is thought to measure cognitive control over pre-potent responding (response inhibition). (Waltz & Gold, 2007) (Lee, Groman, London, & Jentsch, 2007) (Frank & Claus, 2006) MANUSCRIPTS ON THE WEBSITE: Cools, R., Altamirano, L., & D'Esposito, M. (2006). Reversal learning in parkinson's disease depends on medication status and outcome valence. Neuropsychologia, 44(10), 1663-1673. Waltz, J. A., & Gold, J. M. (2007). Probabilistic reversal learning Neural Construct Validity Interactions between declarative systems (hippocampal and prefrontal) and implicit systems (striatum) are predicted across initial learning of a discrimination, such that increasing activation in the striatum, as a function of trials performed, occurs along with learning. The ventrolateral and ventromedial frontal cortex are particularly involved in the effective inhibition of responses at reversal. (Dias, Robbins, & Roberts, 1996, 1997) (Fellows & Farah, 2005) (Fellows & Farah, 2003) Reliability As this is a "learning" task, test-retest reliability is unavailable. Psychometric Characteristics Practice effects are unknown, but are the subject of active investigation. Depending upon how the test is conducted, there can be ceiling effects for discrimination learning. The use of multiple discriminada sets, at least 3 stimuli in each set and probabilistic feedback can avoid this. Animal Model Stage of Research Analogous tests for mice, rats and monkeys exist. There is evidence that this specific task elicits deficits in schizophrenia. (Lee et al., 2007) (Boulougouris, Dalley, & Robbins, 2007) (Chudasama & Robbins, 2006) We need to assess psychometric characteristics such as test-retest reliability, practice effects, and ceiling/floor effects for this task. We need to study whether or not performance on this task changes in response to psychological or pharmacological intervention BUT: (Cools, Altamirano, & D'Esposito, 2006; Cools, Lewis, Clark, Barker, & Robbins, 2007) impairments in schizophrenia: further evidence of orbitofrontal dysfunction. Schizophrenia Research, 93(1-3), 296303. Pizzagalli Reward Task Recently, we described a probabilistic reward task based on a differential reinforcement schedule that allowed us to objectively assess participants’ propensity to modulate behavior as a function of reward history. In this 15-20min computerized task, participants are confronted with a choice between two responses that are linked to different probabilities of reward. Due to this probabilistic nature, participants cannot infer which stimulus is more advantageous based on the outcome of a single trial and so need to integrate reinforcement history over time in order to optimize their choices. Behavioral performance is analyzed using signaldetection theory by calculating both response bias toward the more frequently rewarded stimulus and overall discriminability (in addition to reaction time and hit rates). Unix scripts are available for automatic data quality check, outlier detection, and computation of behavioral variables. In prior studies in non-clinical samples, subjects reporting elevated depressive symptoms showed reduced responsiveness to the more frequently rewarded stimulus (Pizzagalli et al., 2005). Moreover, reward responsiveness negatively correlated with self-reported anhedonic symptoms (Bogdan and Pizzagalli, 2006; Pizzagalli et al., 2005), and predicted these symptoms one month later (Pizzagalli et al., 2005). These findings have recently been extended to two clinical groups: euthymic patients with bipolar disorder (Pizzagalli et al., 2008) and unmedicated subjects with unipolar depression (Pizzagalli et al., under review) were characterized by reduced reward learning and reward responsiveness, respectively. (Pizzagalli, Jahn, & O'Shea, 2005) MANUSCRIPTS ON THE WEBSITE: Pizzagalli, D. A., Goetz, E., Ostacher, M., Iosifescu, D. V., & Perlis, R. H. (2008). Euthymic Patients with Bipolar Disorder Show Decreased Reward Learning in a Probabilistic Reward Task. Biol Psychiatry. Pizzagalli, D. A., Jahn, A. L., & O'Shea, J. P. (2005). Toward an objective characterization of an anhedonic (Pizzagalli, Jahn, & O'Shea, 2005) (Pizzagalli, Bogdan, Ratner, & Jahn, 2007) (Pizzagalli, Goetz, Ostacher, Iosifescu, & Perlis, 2008) (Bogdan & Pizzagalli, 2006) In healthy controls, we have also shown that response bias was reduced under an acute stress condition (Bogdan & Pizzagalli, 2006) and by a pharmacological manipulation affecting the dopaminergic system (Pizzagalli et al., 2008) Conversely, response bias was increased when a nicotine patch was applied to healthy nonsmokers (Barr et al., 2007). Finally, ongoing ERP and fMRI studies have provided preliminary evidence that response bias is associated with feedback-related negativity (FRN) and activation in the dorsal anterior cingulate and basal ganglia. (Pizzagalli, Bogdan, Ratner, & Jahn, 2007) (Frank, Seeberger, & O'Reilly R, 2004) (Bogdan & Pizzagalli, 2006) (Barr, Pizzagalli, Culhane, Goff, & Evins, 2007) Information about test-retest reliability can be found in Pizzagalli et al. (2005). After an average of 38 days, the testretest correlation between bias scores was 0.57. Practice effects might emerged with repeated administration. This can be circumvented by asking participants to respond to different stimuli in different sessions. For an example, see: Bogdan & Pizzagalli, 2006 A federal grant is currently under review to develop a rodent model of our probabilistic reward task. This specific task needs to be studied in individuals with schizophrenia. Data already exists on psychometric characteristics of this task, such as testretest reliability, practice effects, ceiling/floor effects. There is evidence that performance on this task can improve in response to psychological or pharmacological interventions. phenotype: a signal-detection approach. Biological Psychiatry, 57(4), 319-327. Probabilisiti c Selection Task The probabilistic selection (PS) task measures participants' ability to learn from positive and negative feedback, both on a trial-to-trial basis, and in integrating reinforcement probabilities over many trials. It also probes performance in a novel 'test' phase which permits evaluation of whether one implicitly learned more from the positive or negative outcomes of their decisions. This "Go" or "NoGo" learning bias is very sensitive to dopaminergic manipulation and dopamine-related genetics. (Frank, Seeberger, & O'Reilly R, 2004) (Waltz et al., 2007) (Frank, Moustafa, Haughey, Curran, & Hutchison, 2007) MANUSCRIPTS ON THE WEBSITE: Frank, M. J., Seeberger, L. C., & O'Reilly R, C. (2004). By carrot or by stick: cognitive reinforcement learning in parkinsonism. Science, 306(5703), 1940-1943. Waltz, J. A., Frank, M. J., Robinson, B. M., & Gold, J. M. (2007). Selective reinforcement learning deficits in schizophrenia support predictions from computational models of striatal-cortical dysfunction. Biological Psychiatry. Performance in this task is defined by the ability to choose the probabilistically most optimal stimulus. Relative positive and negative feedback learning is similarly modulated by dopamine manipulation in this task and others that are meant to measure similar constructs using different stimuli, motor responses, and task rules. Probabilistic positive and negative feedback learning are sensitive to dopaminergic manipulation. Increases in dopaminergic stimulation, likely in the striatum, lead to better positive learning but cause impairments in negative feedback learning. Dopamine depletions, as in Parkinson's disease and older seniors (greater than 70 years of age) are associated with relatively better negative feedback learning. Genes that control dopamine function in the striatum are predictive of probabilistic positive and negative learning, whereas genes that control dopamine function in prefrontal cortex are predictive of rapid trial-to-trial learning from negative feedback. Negative feedback learning is also associated with enhanced errorrelated negativity (brain potentials originating from anterior-cingulate cortex) and activation of this same region in fmri. In healthy populations, there is not a very large test-retest reliability (some participants can show a positive learning bias in one session and a negative bias in another). On the other hand, there are robust genetic effects in this task, with large effect sizes, so on average the task must measure a reliable trait characteristic. Thus individual variability is likely due to either (i) state-dependent changes; or (ii) intrinsic noise in the measure (for example a participant might be labeled a 'positive' learner after choosing stimulus A, but any one subject might happen to prefer stimulus A from the outset (before receiving any reinforcement) simply due to its surface features). These problems can be mitigated Practice effects have been assessed in Frank & O'Reilly (2006). On average participants are faster to learn the task after multiple sessions, but this practice does affect relative positive versus negative feedback learning. (Frank & O'Reilly R, 2006) Currently being used for rat testing in collaboration with Claudio DaCunha's lab in Brazil. There is evidence that this specific task elicits deficits in schizophrenia. Data already exists on psychometric characteristics of this task, such as testretest reliability, practice effects, ceiling/floor effects. There is evidence that performance on this task can improve in response to psychological or pharmacological interventions. (Frank et al., 2007) (Frank, Woroch, & Curran, 2005) (Klein et al., 2007) (Frank & O'Reilly R, 2006) Weather Prediction Task This is a 200 trial learning task. The stimuli for this task consist of four “tarot” cards that each have a unique appearance that consists of an arrangement of circles, squares, triangles or diamonds. On any given trial, between 0 and 3 of these cards are presented, and there are 14 possible card combinations. Each tarot card is associated with one of two outcomes (rain or sun) with a fixed probability. The probability of each outcome (rain or sun) on any given trial is computed as the conditional probability of each outcome and card occurring together. After the set of cards is presented for that trial, participants have up to 5 seconds to respond. Subjects are then presented with their prediction accuracy and the actual weather for that trial (rain or sun). A running score on the side of the screen increases or decreases as a function of the accuracy of the subject’s predictions. (Knowlton, Mangels, & Squire, 1996; Knowlton, Squire, & Gluck, 1994) MANUSCRIPTS ON THE WEBSITE: Gluck, M. A., Shohamy, D., & Myers, C. (2002). How do people solve the "weather prediction" task?: individual variability in strategies for probabilistic category learning. Learn Mem, 9(6), 408-418. Keri, S., Juhasz, A., Rimanoczy, A., Szekeres, G., Kelemen, O., Cimmer, C., Research on how subjects solve this task suggests three different possible learning strategies (Gluck, Shohamy, & Myers, 2002): 1) multi-cue – respond to each pattern on the basis of associated of all four cues with each outcome; 2) one-cue – respond on the basis of the presence or absence of a single cue, ignoring the other cues; or 3) singleton – learn only about patterns (4 total) that have only one cue present an all other absent. Participants who adopt a multi-cue strategy do better than participants who adult either of the other two strategy, for which performance does not differ (Gluck et al., 2002). It is also thought that early learning on the WPT is dependent on implicit, striatally governed processes, while later learning can engage explicit classification learning processes (Price, 2005). Parkinson’s patients do poorly on versions of the task involving feedback based learning, but are not impaired on versions requiring observational learning Evidence that this task measures striatally based learning comes from studies showing impaired WPT performance in patients with Parkinson’s disease (Knowlton, Mangels et al., 1996), Huntington’s Chorea (Knowlton, Squire et al., 1996), and Tourette’s Syndrome (Keri, Szlobodnyik, Benedek, Janka, & Gadoros, 2002). Neuroimaging studies in healthy individuals reveal activation of the striatum (Gluck, Poldrack, & Keri, 2008; Poldrack & Foerde, 2008; Poldrack & Gabrieli, 2001; Poldrack, Prabhakaran, Seger, & Gabrieli, 1999). There is evidence that individuals with schizophrenia are not impaired in performance on this task (Keri et al., 2005; Keri et al., 2000; Weickert et al., 2002), unless they are taking by testing participants in multiple blocks with different stimuli and averaging. Unknown Unknown Unknown This task does not seem to elicit deficit sin schizophrenia. We need to assess psychometric characteristics such as test-retest reliability, practice effects, and ceiling/floor effects for this task. There is evidence that performance on this task can improve in response to psychological or pharmacological interventions. et al. (2005). Habit learning and the genetics of the dopamine D3 receptor: evidence from patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls. Behav Neurosci, 119(3), 687-693. (Shohamy et al., 2004). Parkinson’s patients are less likely than control to use a multi-cue strategy and more likely to use a singleton strategy (Shohamy et al., 2004). antipsychotics that block dopamine receptors in the striatum (Beninger et al., 2003). REFERENCES: Barr, R. S., Pizzagalli, D. A., Culhane, M. A., Goff, D. C., & Evins, A. E. (2007). A Single Dose of Nicotine Enhances Reward Responsiveness in Nonsmokers: Implications for Development of Dependence. Biol Psychiatry. Beninger, R. J., Wasserman, J., Zanibbi, K., Charbonneau, D., Mangels, J., & Beninger, B. V. (2003). Typical and atypical antipsychotic medications differentially affect two nondeclarative memory tasks in schizophrenic patients: a double dissociation. Schizophr Res, 61(2-3), 281-292. Bogdan, R., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2006). Acute stress reduces reward responsiveness: implications for depression. Biol Psychiatry, 60(10), 1147-1154. Boulougouris, V., Dalley, J. W., & Robbins, T. W. (2007). Effects of orbitofrontal, infralimbic and prelimbic cortical lesions on serial spatial reversal learning in the rat. Behav Brain Res, 179(2), 219-228. Cools, R., Altamirano, L., & D'Esposito, M. (2006). Reversal learning in parkinson's disease depends on medication status and outcome valence. Neuropsychologia, 44(10), 1663-1673. Cools, R., Lewis, S. J., Clark, L., Barker, R. A., & Robbins, T. W. (2007). L-dopa disrupts activity in the nucleus accumbens during reversal learning in parkinson's disease. Neuropsychopharmacology, 32(1), 180189. Chudasama, Y., & Robbins, T. W. (2006). Functions of frontostriatal systems in cognition: comparative neuropsychopharmacological studies in rats, monkeys and humans. Biological Psychiatry, 73(1), 19-38. Dias, R., Robbins, T. W., & Roberts, A. C. (1996). Dissociation in prefrontal cortex of affective and attentional shifts. Nature, 380, 69-72. Dias, R., Robbins, T. W., & Roberts, A. C. (1997). Dissociable forms of inhibitory control within prefrontal cortex with an analog of the Wisconsin Card Sort Test: restrictions to novel situations and independence from "on-line" processing. The Journal of Neuroscience, 17(23), 9285-9297. Fellows, L. K., & Farah, M. J. (2003). Ventromedial frontal cortex mediates affective shifting in humans: evidence from a reversal learning paradigm. Brain, 126(Pt 8), 1830-1837. Fellows, L. K., & Farah, M. J. (2005). Different underlying impairments in decision-making following ventromedial and dorsolateral frontal lobe damage in humans. Cereb Cortex, 15(1), 58-63. Frank, M. J., & Claus, E. D. (2006). Anatomy of a decision: striato-orbitofrontal interactions in reinforcement learning, decision making, and reversal. Psychol Rev, 113(2), 300-326. Frank, M. J., Moustafa, A. A., Haughey, H. M., Curran, T., & Hutchison, K. E. (2007). Genetic triple dissociation reveals multiple roles for dopamine in reinforcement learning. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 104(41), 16311-16316. Frank, M. J., & O'Reilly R, C. (2006). A mechanistic account of striatal dopamine function in human cognition: psychopharmacological studies with cabergoline and haloperidol. Behav Neurosci, 120(3), 497-517. Frank, M. J., Seeberger, L. C., & O'Reilly R, C. (2004). By carrot or by stick: cognitive reinforcement learning in parkinsonism. Science, 306(5703), 1940-1943. Frank, M. J., Woroch, B. S., & Curran, T. (2005). Error-related negativity predicts reinforcement learning and conflict biases. Neuron, 47(4), 495-501. Gluck, M. A., Poldrack, R. A., & Keri, S. (2008). The cognitive neuroscience of category learning. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 32(2), 193-196. Gluck, M. A., Shohamy, D., & Myers, C. (2002). How do people solve the "weather prediction" task?: individual variability in strategies for probabilistic category learning. Learn Mem, 9(6), 408-418. Keri, S., Juhasz, A., Rimanoczy, A., Szekeres, G., Kelemen, O., Cimmer, C., et al. (2005). Habit learning and the genetics of the dopamine D3 receptor: evidence from patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls. Behav Neurosci, 119(3), 687-693. Keri, S., Kelemen, O., Szekeres, G., Bagoczky, N., Erdelyi, R., Antal, A., et al. (2000). Schizophrenics know more than they can tell: probabilistic classification learning in schizophrenia. Psychol Med, 30(1), 149155. Keri, S., Szlobodnyik, C., Benedek, G., Janka, Z., & Gadoros, J. (2002). Probabilistic classification learning in Tourette syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 40(8), 1356-1362. Klein, T. A., Neumann, J., Reuter, M., Hennig, J., von Cramon, D. Y., & Ullsperger, M. (2007). Genetically determined differences in learning from errors. Science, 318(5856), 1642-1645. Knowlton, B. J., Mangels, J. A., & Squire, L. R. (1996). A neostriatal habit learning system in humans. Science, 273(5280), 1399-1402. Knowlton, B. J., Squire, L. R., & Gluck, M. A. (1994). Probabilistic classification learning in amnesia. Learn Mem, 1(2), 106-120. Knowlton, B. J., Squire, L. R., Paulsen, J. S., Swerdlow, N. R., Swenson, M., & Butters, N. (1996). Dissociations within nondeclarative memory in Huntington's Disease. Neuropsychology, 10, 538-548. Lee, B., Groman, S., London, E. D., & Jentsch, J. D. (2007). Dopamine D2/D3 receptors play a specific role in the reversal of a learned visual discrimination in monkeys. Neuropsychopharmacology, 32(10), 21252134. Pizzagalli, D. A., Bogdan, R., Ratner, K. G., & Jahn, A. L. (2007). Increased perceived stress is associated with blunted hedonic capacity: potential implications for depression research. Behav Res Ther, 45(11), 2742-2753. Pizzagalli, D. A., Goetz, E., Ostacher, M., Iosifescu, D. V., & Perlis, R. H. (2008). Euthymic Patients with Bipolar Disorder Show Decreased Reward Learning in a Probabilistic Reward Task. Biol Psychiatry. Pizzagalli, D. A., Jahn, A. L., & O'Shea, J. P. (2005). Toward an objective characterization of an anhedonic phenotype: a signal-detection approach. Biological Psychiatry, 57(4), 319-327. Poldrack, R. A., & Foerde, K. (2008). Category learning and the memory systems debate. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 32(2), 197-205. Poldrack, R. A., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2001). Characterizing the neural mechanisms of skill learning and repetition priming: evidence from mirror reading. Brain, 124(Pt 1), 67-82. Poldrack, R. A., Prabhakaran, V., Seger, C. A., & Gabrieli, J. D. (1999). Striatal activation during acquisition of a cognitive skill. Neuropsychology, 13(4), 564-574. Price, A. L. (2005). Cortico-striatal contributions to category learning: dissociating the verbal and implicit systems. Behav Neurosci, 119(6), 1438-1447. Shohamy, D., Myers, C. E., Grossman, S., Sage, J., Gluck, M. A., & Poldrack, R. A. (2004). Cortico-striatal contributions to feedback-based learning: converging data from neuroimaging and neuropsychology. Brain, 127(Pt 4), 851-859. Waltz, J. A., Frank, M. J., Robinson, B. M., & Gold, J. M. (2007). Selective reinforcement learning deficits in schizophrenia support predictions from computational models of striatal-cortical dysfunction. Biological Psychiatry. Waltz, J. A., & Gold, J. M. (2007). Probabilistic reversal learning impairments in schizophrenia: further evidence of orbitofrontal dysfunction. Schizophrenia Research, 93(1-3), 296-303. Weickert, T. W., Terrazas, A., Bigelow, L. B., Malley, J. D., Hyde, T., Egan, M. F., et al. (2002). Habit and skill learning in schizophrenia: evidence of normal striatal processing with abnormal cortical input. Learn Mem, 9(6), 430-442.