E.N. 7228847 Working Memory: A Multi-Component Model E. N. 7228847 Msc Human Cognitive Neuropsychology The School of Philosophy, Psychology and Language Sciences, University of Edinburgh 1 E.N. 7228847 2010 2 E.N. 7228847 Abstract Previous studies have reported that combining two memory tasks or varying the level of cognitive demand in a task does not cause disruption in healthy adults’ performance. To examine whether a dual memory task would be more sensitive to the manipulation of cognitive load, a experiment was set in which recall of digits sequences was combined with recall of visual patterns. The results suggested that different levels of cognitive load do not affect the task being performed simultaneously, when retaining sequences of digits in memory while encoding and retrieving visual patterns and vice-eversa. These data challenge the assumption of a single limited attentional capacity, which prevents a trade-off of capacity between two concurrent tasks. However, this data can be easily explained by a multi-component model of working memory. 3 E.N. 7228847 Table of Contents 1. Introduction .......................................................................................................................... 5 1.1 Working memory and its different approaches ................................................................ 5 1.2 The multi-component working memory model ................................................................ 8 1.3 Coordination operation in dual tasks .............................................................................. 16 1.4 Aims and predictions of this study ................................................................................. 18 2 Method ................................................................................................................................. 22 2 .1 Participants .................................................................................................................... 22 2.2 Stimuli ............................................................................................................................ 22 2.2.2 Serial Digits recall.................................................................................................... 22 2.2.3 Visual pattern recall ................................................................................................. 22 2.3 Procedure ........................................................................................................................ 23 3 Results .................................................................................................................................. 28 3.1 Digit delayed recall x visual pattern immediate recall…………………………… ….. 27 3.2 Visual pattern delayed recall x digit sequence immediate recall…………………………..27 4. Discussion ............................................................................................................................ 33 References ............................................................................................................................... 39 4 E.N. 7228847 1. Introduction 1.1 Working Memory and Its Different Approaches Working memory refers to on-line cognition- the monitoring, processing and maintenance of information (Shah and Miyake, 1999). It is comprised of the functional components of cognition responsible for comprehending and forming mental representations about immediate environment, retaining information about immediate past experience, supporting acquisition of the new knowledge, problem-solving, and to formulating and acting upon specific goals (Hitch & Baddeley, 2007). A current debate in working memory research is about the nature of working memory. In order to determine the nature of working memory it is a key point to understand how cognitive functions and their impact on other forms of information are managed in the human brain. One focus of discussion is whether working memory can be used as a general unitary concept (WMG) or as a specific model (WMS). These different approaches have resulted in two research traditions, each one with different perspectives on the nature, structure and functions of working memory. According to the WMG (e.g. Cowan, 2001), working memory capacity is limited by a general flexible attentional resource that can be shared, whereas in WMS working memory is comprised of multiple independent domains and cognitive resources can operate in parallel (Baddeley & Logie, 1999). A way to tackle how working memory resources are allocated is through dual task methodology. The ability of individuals to perform more than one task at the same time has been one of the ongoing debates within the memory and attention literature. It is a very ecologically valid test as in real life situations people find themselves holding information in memory while performing a different task (e.g. talking while walking, cooking breakfast or thinking about directions while driving). Different theorists diverge in opinion about dual task ability. 5 E.N. 7228847 Within the theories in agreement with WMG model are the Time-Based ResourceSharing (TBRS) model (Barrouillet, Bernardin and Camos, 2004; Barrouilet & Camos, 2009) which proposes a general-purpose pool of limited resources that is shared between distinct cognitive activities. To avoid the decay, the information needs to be refreshed by attentional focus, otherwise it will be lost over a certain period of time. This pool of resources is usually interpreted as attention and it serves as a ‘bottleneck’ that switches between one activity to another in order to maintain the information during the dual task. Cognitive load is defined as the proportion of time during which attention is being captured by an activity, thereby impeding memory traces to decay (Barrouillet & Camos, 2009). The central attentional resource is thought to be shared between mental processes regardless of the nature of the information involved. In this view, verbal and visuospatial activities are assumed to compete for a common pool of domain-general limited resources, resulting in interference between such activities when they are performed together. Vergauwe, Barrouillet and Camos (2010) designed an experiment to examine the dual task effects between different domains. The storage tasks were the letter span- a series of consonants to be recalled (verbal storage) and the location span - a red square that appeared in different locations on matrices (visuo-spatial storage). For the processing tasks, two choice-reaction time tasks were used. For the verbal task, the participants were presented a range of words and asked to judge whether or not they were animal names (i.e., ‘elephant’). For the visual task, boxes were presented together with a horizontal line (varying in length) and two squares dots (differently positioned in relation to the horizontal line) and between the dots there was a gap. Participants were required to judge whether or not the line could fit into the gap. The increasing cognitive load of both verbal and visuospatial processing disrupted verbal storage and verbal recall was consistently 6 E.N. 7228847 poorer when it was combined with verbal processing than when it was combined with visuo-spatial processing (Vergauwe, Barrouillet & Camos, 2010). Visual recall performance (i.e. memory for coloured discs) has been shown to be disrupted by concurrent visual activities such as a tone-pitch discrimination task. Increasing the attentional demands on the discrimination task resulted in a larger dual task decrement, indicating an existence of a common pool of domain-general resources. Although increasing the cognitive load of either verbal or visuospatial processing disrupted verbal storage, verbal recall performance was poorer when it was combined with verbal processing than when it was combined with visual processing. It is assumed by the authors (Vergauwe, Barrouillet & Camos, 2010) of the study that a domain-specific interference is explained by the fact that verbal information is maintained by two independent mechanisms: attentional refresher and articulatory rehearsal. In this case, while visuospatial information interfered only with the attentional refresher, verbal processing interfered with both mechanisms. Another model that accounts for the unitary working memory is the model proposed by Cowan (1999, 2001). In this view, working memory is an activation of items from long-term memory. Different codes are processed according to the same principles. Not only does the capacity-limited focus of attention contribute to recall, but also the time or interference-limited sources of activation of long-term memory. Cowan (2001) suggested that some information can also be held in the focus of attention while other is stored passively. The information that is focus of attention can be adversely affected by a capacity limit, although the information that is passively stored is constrained by temporal limits and interference factor. The focus of attention shifts between different sources from time to time and such an operation prevents the capacity of central storage to be overloaded. Then, in the dual task it is possible to recall contents from one task and then 7 E.N. 7228847 shift attention towards another task, consequently activating memory representations of this latter task and recalling the content of it. Morey and Cowan (2005) combined visual and verbal stimuli. The visual task consisted of arrays of coloured-squares, then a second array with one of the squares circled was presented and the participant judged if the circled square was the same colour as it was in the first array presented. If the response was positive, they should type the S key, if was not, they should type the D key. In one of the trials participants rehearse the numbers in silence and in another they rehearsed aloud. The addition of the spoken rehearsal produced dramatic interference; it was though that attention was ‘called away’ from the visual task to engage in recalling the digits preventing the encoding of the visual array. This interpretation was confirmed by a second experiment (Morey and Cowan, 2005). In this second experiment they repeated the same task but used articulatory suppression of unrelated items instead of rehearsal numbers and found no effects on the visual task. 1.2 The Multi-Component Working Memory Model The concept of Working Memory was developed and empirically tested by Hitch and Baddeley (1974). This model comprises three temporary storage systems, namely, the phonological loop, the visual sketch pad and the episodic buffer (with the latter being a more general integrated storage system) and an attentional controller the central executive. According to Baddeley and Hitch (2007) the phonological loop is involved in verbal processes and is assumed to have a limited capacity available, which depends on the amount of memory activation available. This subsystem has been fractionated into a passive phonological store and an active rehearsal process. “The phonological store maintains items through repetition or registers visually presented material within the store by a process of articulatory naming” (Baddeley & Hitch, 2007, p.4). The rehearsal process 8 E.N. 7228847 is responsible for refreshing the decaying representations in the phonological store (Baddeley, 2007). The visual sketchpad stores and processes visuospatial information and is comprised of a passive visual cache and an active spatially based system - the inner scribe (Logie, 1995). The visual cache retains visual patterns while the inner scribe retains sequences of movements. The episodic buffer forms an interface between the three working memory subsystems and long-term memory. It binds perceptual information from the subsystems and from long-term memory. The term ‘buffer’ has to do with how it provides an interface between a number of different codes - visual, verbal, perceptual and from long-term memory both episodic and semantic (Baddeley, 2007). The central executive itself is not unified and is assumed to oversee a range of control processes such as controlling the subsidiary systems, which allow for the selection and implementation of strategies (Logie, Osaka & D’Esposito, 2007). It helps coordinate combined tasks as well as dealing with processing (Duff and Logie, 2001). Furthermore, the central executive is involved with some aspects of attention such as shifting attention between tasks as well as distributing and focusing attention in a dual task. Other processes that this system is involved with are monitoring and updating the representations of working memory, inhibiting dominant responses and building information from the subsidiary systems and from long-term memory (Baddeley, Baddeley, Bucks and Wilcock, 2001). The non-unitary nature of the model proposed by hitch and Baddeley (1974) is supported by the dual task paradigm (Baddeley, Logie, Bressi, Della Sala & Spinnler, 1986). Experimental paradigms used by Baddeley and colleagues compared individual’s performance in a single task condition to individual’s performance in a dual task condition. This comparison only is possible because each participant score is defined 9 E.N. 7228847 before dual task being performed. Then dual task is set according to individual span (Della Sala, Foley, Beschin, Allerhand & Logie, in press). The use of a span procedure guarantees that the individual is exhausting his or her maximum capacity for a specific task (Duff & Logie, 2001). In these dual task paradigms each one of the concurrent task is thought to tap one of the two subsidiary systems which means that the tasks pertain each one to one of each different domains. The nature of the two tasks performed must be taken into consideration since both tasks need to rely on different resources in order to avoid disruption in activity in dual tasking. Following this requirements, the dual task paradigm can be performed by healthy adults with little decrement in dual task (Baddeley et al., 1986, Cocchini, Logie, Della Sala, MacPherson & Baddeley, 2002; Logie, Cocchini, Della Sala & Baddeley, 2004; Macpherson, Della Sala, Logie & wilcock, 2007, Sebastian, Menor & Elousa, 2006; Duff & Logie, 2001). The fact that the dual tasks do not disrupt the performance in the main task has led working memory to be associated with multiple domain-specific systems. The codes used in the dual task are different and is suggested to employ different form of resources. More specifically, digit recall is thought to involve the phonological loop whereas tracking is thought to tap the visuo-spatial sketchpad (Baddeley, 2007). Also, the use of selective interference paradigms have reinforced the idea of a working memory system characterized by separated pool of resources, in that tasks pertaining at the same domain are combined. The results showed that two tasks pertaining to the same domain can not be supported, causing disruption on the tasks. (Baddeley, Zuco & Baddeley, 1990). In a study conducted by Baddeley et al. (1986) digit sequences recall was presented simultaneously with perceptuomotor tracking of a randomly moving target. This test was devised to compare dual task performance within healthy young adults, healthy aging adults, and Alzheimer’s disease (AD) patients. The dual task requirement 10 E.N. 7228847 resulted in about 15% drop in the performance of each task for young and healthy aging; a decrement considered to be small if participants are under a very demanding task because they are using their maximum capacity for each task. Only AD patients were penalised and this was associated with a dysfunction in attentional control within the central executive of working memory. From this point on, much attention has been paid to research in how patients with Alzheimer’s disease perform under dual task conditions which led to the idea for a separation in the attentional controller of working memory (Baddley & Della Sala, 1996). AD patients presented a specific dual task coordination function which reinforced the view that executive control in working memory cannot be characterized by a unified system; in other words it is not general-purpose control mechanism but it reflects more than one process in the healthy brain (Baddeley, 1996; Baddeley, Emslie, Kolodny & Duncan, 1998; Baddeley et al., 2001). A possible caveat in the paradigm tracking and digit recall was that a tracking task was supported by a perceptuomotor system which could indicate a perceptuomotor system separated from working memory and not a distinct system within working memory. To examine this issue, Cocchini et al. (2002) compared the performance of young adults on two different memory tasks, namely, digit recall (immediate verbal memory) and visual pattern recall (immediate visuo-spatial memory). The results did not suggest consistent reductions in performance in the verbal or in the visual when performed concurrently. Immediate recall of numbers dropped around 8% when a visual pattern preload was retained and an immediate recall of a visual pattern dropped by around 5% when a digit preload was retained. Those effects were significant and were not predicted by a multicomponent model of working memory, although the decrements were considered small when considered that both tasks were performed under a very demanding cognitive load; 11 E.N. 7228847 the large drop in dual-task performance that would be predicted by a model that assumes a single general purpose memory system was not observed. In this study, a preload procedure was utilised with the participant being required to encode material (e.g. digit sequences) and after 15 seconds recall it. In the interval of 15 seconds, the participant should encode material, but for immediate recall. For this reason, the immediate recall of content was compared to delayed recall of the same content. One interesting result was observed in this experiment: when comparing performance on delayed and immediate condition in dual task digit recall in the delayed condition was poorer than at immediate recall - even if the participants were free to rehearse the digits during the delay. To ensure that the preload task was not being encoded in the long term memory, while participants were performing the interpolated task, articulatory suppression was employed as a secondary task. In the articulatory suppression task participants were asked to repeat aloud irrelevant content such as the word “go” for 15 seconds. Articulatory suppression has proved to disrupt immediate verbal memory and due to this fact, it is suggested that long-term memory does not support the retention of the verbal material. Instead, it is assumed that the retention of digit sequences rely on subvocal rehearsal within the phonological loop component of working memory, and repeating aloud a random word affects the rehearsal which in turn prevents information decay. It is also important to compare the size of disruption caused by the articulatory suppression to the size of disruption caused by the dual task conducted in this experiment. The size of the disruption caused by ariculatory suppression reflected how the disruption resulted from the dual task cannot be considered robust (Cocchini et al., 2002). If it is expected that two different codes are not being processed by different domains the disruption caused by a dual task should be the same or similar regardless of the nature of the tasks being used in the dual task. Indeed, the single resource model would require additional assumptions 12 E.N. 7228847 about how the current state of one task was maintained while attention was focused on the other task and vice versa. The same pattern of results was found in a study combining two memory tasks comparing the performance from three groups: healthy young adults, older young adults and Alzheimer’s disease patients (Macpherson, et al., 2007). Healthy older adults and young adults were not reliably impaired in the dual task; participants recalled significantly more digits during delayed digit recall than digit recall combined with articulatory suppression and visual patterns. Also, participants recalled fewer digits during digit recall combined with visual pattern compared to digit recall combined with tracking, indicating that a combination of two memories tasks is more demanding than combining a memory task with a perceptuomotor task. Arguably, if a single attentional resource is divided between two tasks then varying the demand of one task should produce a significant effect on another secondary task. In a single attentional working memory model if one task overloads memory resources then there is less spare capacity to process the other task which could influence an individual’s performance. Logie, et al. (2004) used the dual task paradigm in which the demand of the one task was fixed while the demand of the other task was varied. The dual task was consisted of digits recall and tracking. In this study they compared 3 groups: Alzheimer’s patients, healthy adults and normal aging. The dual task performance in both conditions (i.e., tracking fixed demand concurrently with varied demand of digits length and fixed length of digits sequence concurrently with varied demand of tracking) was very similar to that found for performance of each respective single task in the healthy groups. This translates to the non effect of the second task upon the first task. Participants could not be performing one task at ceiling; leaving extra capacity to support the other task since the experiment was designed using the individual span for the fixed level which assured that the individual was using his or her maximum capacity. Many levels of load were tested at 13 E.N. 7228847 the same level both below and above the level of maximum capacity. Even when the demands of the concomitant tasks were much lower than individual ability level, a dual task decrement was not found. These results were replicated with another combination of tasks by Logie, Della Sala, MacPherson and Cooper (2007). In this study, the differences between younger and older participants were examined in a dual task combining digit recall with different speed levels on a response time (RT) task. In this task participants were asked to press a response key when an asterisk presented in the middle of the computer screen; the levels of demand were defined according to the time between the response and the next stimulus. Although dual task during encoding produced significant interference in memory performance, digit recall performance did not change according to the different levels of demand on the second task, which confirmed that increasing the level of one task does not leave spare capacity or that a high demand second task disrupts the first task. The young participants dual task performance was not influenced by the level of complexity of the tasks - tasks being performed at very low demand or at very high demand for each participant. The effect in encoding was consistent with previous experiments – a perceptuomotor task can undermine the verbal storage task during encoding (e.g. NavehBenjamim, Guez & Marom, 2003). Duff and Logie (2001) also tackled the evidence of a multi-component model in an experiment using a processing and storage dual task combination. A multi-component model supposes that in a task that involves processing and storage with the former being dealt with by the phonological loop or sketchpad, while processing is dealt by the central executive. In this experiment, the ‘working memory span’ was the measure, whereby the participant read lists of sentences and decided if the sentence was coherent or not and memorised the last word for each sentence (Daneman & Carpenter, 1980). As in this task 14 E.N. 7228847 the level of complexity increases in complexity and the final words of each sentence need to be remembered in serial ordered recall, this task is assumed to involve different levels of on-line processing and different levels of temporary memory load. A single attentional model of working memory interprets this task as a trade off between memory and processing tasks. That is, more complex sentences demand more working memory resources leaving less spare capacity for retention of the final words. However, for a multi-component model of working memory, the system responsible for processing and the systems responsible for storing are considered as entirely separate entities. In this particular experiment, before of performing the dual task, the participants performed both tasks (checking the sentences meaning and recalling of list s of words) in a single condition to determine an individual span for each task. Next, in a dual task condition, participants were asked to perform the sentence checking and final sentence word recalling. There was an increase in the overall performance on dual task which was not above 40%. This decrement was associated with an overload on the central executive. In addition, the speed allowed for encoding also increased when the number of words to be remembered increased. The speed increasing for encode was not a feature of the single task which makes it more likely that that a substantial dual task decrement in memory span will be observed. There was no evidence of a trade off between resources. If both tasks were being performed at an individual’s maximum capacity, than each task should be utilizing 100% of the supply of resources available for the task. In the case of a single attentional capacity, when combining the two very demanding tasks, one of the tasks would be prioritise and as a consequence performance in the other task would be undermined. The fact that participants can perform two tasks in a dual task condition and achieve almost the same result as when performing these tasks in a single task condition is difficult to 15 E.N. 7228847 conciliate with a single pool of resources. Showing that processing task and memory task can be performed at the same time was interpreted as a temporary storage independent of processing system. (Duff & Logie, 2001). To explore a task switching mechanism operating in the dual task a second experiment articulated a dual task in which processing task included items that were more unrelated to the items included in the storage task. More specifically, the dual task combined arithmetic verification and recalling presented words. If a task switching mechanisms was operating in this dual task, then it would cost less for a single limited capacity system if the items included in these tasks were more similar between each other in comparison to each task including tasks less related between each other. The fact that such an experiment resulted in a smaller decrement than in the previous experiment, in which the material used on the tasks were related to each other, serves as evidence that the dual task performance is not necessarily supported by a task switching account. Conversely, more unrelated tasks are an advantage for the multi-component model as there would a whole pool of resources for supporting each different task contents. In this case, the purer the task (e.g. a task that involves just visual information rather than a task that uses visual information but can also involve verbal coding) the better the dual task performance will be. 1.3 Coordination Operation in Dual Tasks One reasonable explanation for the modest decrement in performance on the dual task can be offered by the possible different strategies used by participants for recalling the items in the task. A study conducted by Logie, Della Sala, Laiacona, Chalmers, & Wynn, (1996) showed that despite the fact that subjects were expected to use verbal rehearsal and phonological coding in order to retain the items, they may use different strategies to recall material which can interfere with the results of an experiment. For 16 E.N. 7228847 example, if subjects used a semantic strategy such as a visual mnemonic or a lexical strategy to retain material this would undermine the performance as this subject is using cognitive resources other than the phonological loop. As such, when performing this task concurrently with a visual task there will be disruption in performance since the task is relying on the same resource and consequently overusing its capacity. Also, the drop in performance observed may be a part of a working memory system that is needed to coordinate two systems operating simultaneously. One of the functions that the central executive is responsible for is the dual task coordination of so called ‘slave systems’ (Baddeley et al., 1986, Cocchini et al., 2004). The minimal effect found in the dual task could be thought as a result of the central executive being required for processing and for some storage function as well, leading this system to be overloaded. However, how the operation of theses independent resources might be coordinated is still is not clear yet. An additional cost, observed in the dual memory tasks, relates to a coordination function implemented when a person is encoding and retrieving material that is held within specialized memory resources (Duff & Logie, 2001). “In the single-task condition, the encoding and retrieval processes have to deal only with one kind of material (e.g. verbal) and one type of memory resource (e.g. phonological loop). In the dual task condition, the verbal material would be held in a verbal buffer and the visual material in a visual buffer” (Cocchini et al., p.1093-1094). For instance, take the case of a preload procedure with two memory tasks in which the preload task is A and intervening task is B. The coordinating mechanism is involved with the encoding from buffer in task A, then encoding and retrieval from the buffer from the task B, followed by the retrieval from the task A buffer. In a dual task, performance depends on the efficiency of both the storage mechanism and the coordination mechanism, which would require extra to the 17 E.N. 7228847 coordination device (Cocchini et al., 2002 ). Such a cost is due this swapping between buffers. In agreement, Baddeley, Emmslie, Kolodny & Duncan (1998) suggested that memory span tasks need encoding, retention and retrieval on a constant repetition over the performance of the dual task. The memory task costs because is not just the phonological loop that is involved in retention, but also executive resources are required for operating the repeated encoding and retrieval processes. Barnard (1999) contributed with an explanation for the coordination task saying that working memory involves specific connections and flow between components resources and multiple stores preserving information in different codes. In the model proposed by Barnard (1999) the equivalent to a central executive is responsible for fulfilling their executive functions in complex tasks as well as generating specific codes (i.e., acoustic or visuo-spatial) according to the nature of the stimuli. This means seriating or organizing the underlying content of thought into linguistic or spatial codes before sending the information to the correspondent systems. 1.4 Aims and Predictions of this Study Previous studies (Baddeley et al., 1986; Baddeley et al., 2001; Cocchini et al., 2002; Logie et al., 2004; MacPherson et al., 2007) have showed that two tasks can be performed with no decrement which is entirely compatible with a multi-component working memory view. These results were found when two memory tasks were combined or when levels of demand are manipulated. A remaining issue is whether the dual memory task in addition to the manipulation of the cognitive level will reproduce the same pattern of results as previous studies. Thus, the present study included the same paradigm in which the dual memory task paradigms and were used such as the studies conducted by Cocchini et al. (2002) and Macpherson et al. (2007). This paradigm was extended to the 18 E.N. 7228847 manipulation of cognitive levels of demand such as in the study conducted by Logie et al. (2004). According to a multi-component model, a trade-off should be observed only when storage or processing of one activity involves the same domain information and different cognitive loads in one of the tasks should not disrupt the other task. However, if a domaingeneral resource is shared between verbal and visuo-spatial activities recall performance should be affected by changes in cognitive load. The prediction made for this study was that increasing or decreasing of cognitive load would not interfere with recall of material fixed at participant span (delayed recall). To examine this prediction, an experiment was designed in which a dual memory task was performed when one of the tasks was maintained at span while the other task has its demand manipulated through five different levels of complexity, ranging from two levels below the individual span to two levels above the individual span. However, differently of the study conducted by Logie et al. (2004), the comparison made was among young adults rather than among Alzheimer’s patients with healthy older adults and young adults. The demand level on the fixed-demand task was set at each individual’s maximum capacity before the dual task condition. The use of individual span indicates that the individual is performing his or her maximum capacity and this prevent the possibility that the scopes of the experiment were undermined by the fact that individual had spared capacity to be spent on the second task. Therefore, the task is considerate at a perfect level, since it is not too difficult which would lead to a floor performance and it is not too easy, in this case leading to a ceiling performance. For this study a preload procedure was used; participants were required to encode and hold in memory the material for one task (preload task), followed by the presentation of a different memory stimulus to be encoded and recalled immediately. Next participant 19 E.N. 7228847 was asked to recall the preload task. A preload procedure was used with the intent to prevent that any adverse effect found is due to a dual task product and not caused by competition for sensory input. In combining two memory tasks in the present experiment, it was essential to prevent that competition for sensory input – for example, having o listen to digits and watch a visual pattern simultaneously, could cause the encoding to fail. According to Cocchini et al. (2002), two stimuli being encoded at the same time can disrupt the task, only to be competing for the same input channels. Then a task performance could be affected by this competition and not due to a lack of sufficient resources to deal with the tasks. A preload procedure voids this competition. The activity to tap into the visual domain was visual patterns. Tose were designed based on the Visual Patterns Test –VPT (Della Sala, Gray, Baddeley, Allamano & Wilson, 1999). This test is assumed to measure the non-verbal aspect of working memory. The non-verbal-memory aspects of working memory were divided into visual and spatial components (Logie, 1995). Therefore, the VPT was designed to obtain a more focused measure of the visual component of visuospatial memory, rather than measuring visual and spatial memory in working memory. A study carried out by (Della Sala, et al., 1999) showed that The correlation between the span of visual pattern and Corsi Block Test -test that is thought to measure the spatial component of visuo-spatial memory, since it requires memory for position of blocks (Corsi, 1972) - is low, suggesting that the tests were measuring different functions. In the same study, the use of selective interference paradigm showed that Corsi Block test and visual pattern test were disrupted when spatial and visual secondary tasks, respectively, were performed during a delay interval. This suggests that the tests were measuring different functions. In the same study, the use of selective interference paradigm showed that Corsi Block test and visual pattern test were disrupted when spatial and visual secondary tasks were performed during a delay interval, 20 E.N. 7228847 respectivally. This confirms that the visual pattern task is involved with the visual domain and Corsi Block test was involved with the spatial doamin. The other task utilised in this study was a digit span task; according to Baddeley (2007) digit span is a task that requires working memory for its recall. For Baddley (2007) digit span is assumed to be dealt by the verbal domain of working memory and substantially confirmed by empirical evidence. It is thought to be heavily supported by working memory due to the lack of effective strategies to support encoding (Logie et al., 2007). Besides, sequences of digits do not involve any semantic or lexical information which make it unlike to involve long-term memory (Baddeley, 2007; Logie et al, 2007). 21 E.N. 7228847 2 Method 2 .1 Participants Participants were originally 32 native English-speakers university students and they signed a consent form. This offered the explanation about the procedure and about the rights of the participant. 2.2 Stimuli 2.2.2 Serial digits recall. Participants listened to a list of digits recorded by a female native English speaker at a rate of one digit per second. Immediately after presentation participants were asked to recall the digits orally in the order of presentation. Following correct recall of two out of three sequences, the sequence length was increased by one digit and the procedure continued. No restrictions were imposed in time to recall. Participants were presented with three sequences at each length, and testing ceased when the participant failed to recall at least two out of the three sequences. Individual span was taken as the longest length of sequence for which the participant recalled at least two out of three sequences correctly. The dependent variable was the percentage of correctly recalled digits in the correct position. 2.2.3 Visual pattern recall. The visual pattern test consisted of matrices containing varying arrangements of opened and filled squares. The matrices patterns were designed to be difficult to encode verbally. The vast majority of the patterns contained squares of 2.5 x 2.5 cm in size with 22 E.N. 7228847 the exception of the 2 x 11-squared matrix that contained 2x2 cm squares due to space limitations. These patterns were randomly-generated in squares filling (in black) half of the squares. The participants were presented in a card containing a matrix for three seconds. Next, the pattern was removed and participants were given a same size blank matrix in which they had to mark with a cross the previously filled cells. Following the correct recall of one out of three patterns at any given level; complexity was increased by adding two squares at a time. The grids steadily progressed in size, from the smallest, a 2X2 matrix (with two filled cells) to the largest, 5X6 matrix (with 15 filled cells). The individual visual pattern span was taken as the mean of the last three patterns for which filled squares were correctly recalled. 2.23 Procedure The testing session took approximately 30 minutes. As soon as the participant arrived they were asked to read the information sheet about the study and to sign the consent form. The procedure was explained and the session finished after they completed all the phases of the experiment. Participants of this study were specifically asked to use subvocal rehearsal in the retention of the digit sequences. For the visual pattern recall the instructions highlighted the use of visual encoding. In the digits sequence recall task participants were encouraged to recall the sequences in the right order but to recall the digits even if they could not remember the exact sequence. This was made in order to preserve the right order of the sequence. For example, if the participants were presented sequence containing the numbers “three-four-five” but could not remember the number 5 they should say “three, I do not remember, five”. 23 E.N. 7228847 The material was comprised of three different sets for visual pattern test: one for the span checking, one for the fixed level and one for the varied level. The same was devised for the digit span. None of the patterns or the sequences of numbers were repeated. Digit span and visual pattern test were assessed in three conditions: 1) single-task, 2) dual-task in a fixed level of difficulty and 3) dual-task in a varied level of difficulty. For the dual task condition, participants were exposed to one task for delayed recall and the second task for immediate recall. The number of patterns or sequences in the interpolated task was relative to how many patterns could be fitted within 15 seconds. The number of lists for each participant varied depending on the length of their digit span, and the performance measure was the proportion of digits recalled correctly. Most of the time , participants had only one visual pattern or digit sequence as intervening task, as most of participants had too long sequences length or too high complexity level of patterns to be recalled within the 15 seconds available for the immediate task The experiment comprised four phases. In the first phase of the experiment, digit recall in a standard condition was assessed to determine their individual digit span. In the second phase visual pattern recall in a standard condition was assessed to determine their individual visual span. In the third phase the level of demand for the first task (e.g. digit span) was fixed at individual span, whereas the demand of the second task (e.g. visual pattern) was varied the five levels of demand. The fixed condition was for delayed recall and the varied condition was for immediate recall for both of the tasks. There were five different sequences at span to be combined different levels of the second task. For the varied demand condition, five different levels of demand patterns comprising levels very low level (i.e. Span-2), low level ( i.e. Span-1), standard (at Span), high (i.e. Span +1), and very high (Span+2) – span refers to individual span for each person calculated in each 24 E.N. 7228847 case. For example: digit sequence at span was presented. While the sequence was being held in memory a series of visual pattern at a given level were showed for immediate recall during 15 seconds. In most of the cases only one visual pattern could be displayed for encoding and immediate recall during the 15 seconds period. After that, the participant was asked to recall the sequence of numbers. In the fourth phase, the level of demand for the first task (visual pattern) was fixed at span for each participant whereas the demand of the second task (digit span) was varied through the five levels of demand in the same way as was done in the third phase. The design of dual task is shown in detail in Figure 1. 25 E.N. 7228847 Figure 1. Dual task structure. Encoding at span Span -2 Recall recall 15 seconds Encoding at span Span -1 Recall recall 15 seconds Encoding at span At Span Recall recall 15 seconds Encoding at span Span+1 Recall recall 15 seconds Encoding at span Span+2 Recall recall 15 seconds Note: Five different structures for the task in the dual task. 26 E.N. 7228847 The order of the experiment phases were counterbalanced across subjects. Four groups of participants were formed each group containing one quarter of the total number of participants. Each group followed a different order of phases: Group 1) phase 1, phase 2, phase 3 and phase 4, Group 2) phase 2, phase 1, phase 3, and phase 4, Group 3) phase 1, phase 2, phase 4, phase 3, Group 4) phase 2, phase 1, phase 3, phase 4. Recall for the digits task was recorded on an audio cassette for subsequent scoring. In order to facilitate the process of correction of the visual pattern tasks each pattern had a reference number and on the answer sheet the blanked patterns; each square was marked with a number. Each participant had an answer sheet filled with the numbers corresponded to the squares marked by the participant which were copied for subsequent correction. The example of correction method is shown in Figure 2. Figure 2. 1 2 3 4 Right answer: 2-3 27 E.N. 7228847 3 Results The data of two participants could not be used, remaining then 30 participants, comprised of 14 females and 16 males. Their mean age was 22.9 years old (SD = 2.6), lowest age 20 and higher 29. They were paid a modest honorarium for taking part As the order that participants was counterbalanced (the experiment could be taken in four different orders). An one-way ANOVA was conducted to observe whether the order that participants accomplished interfered on their performance on each condition. The was no significant interaction between the four conditions and order: Digit delayed recall [F (12, 104) =1.49 MSE = 606.55, p>.001]; digit immediate recall [F (12,104) =.97, MSE = 328.85, p>.001]; visual pattern delayed recall [F (12,104) = 1.01, MSE = 470.75, p>.001]; and visual pattern immediate recall [F(12,104) =.61, MSE = 460.75, p>.001], indicating that participants were not beneficiating of a possible training effect. The Table 1 shows the mean scores for digit span checking and for visual pattern span checking. Table 1. Digit Span 6.86 Visual Pattern 10.6 Overall scores for each participant in each condition were analysed and differences among conditions were evaluated via analyses of variance (ANOVA). 28 E.N. 7228847 3.1 Digit delayed recall x visual pattern immediate recall There was no significant effect of dual task demand for digits delayed recall [F (4,116) =1.05, MSE = 637.6 p>.001] which means that maintaining digits in memory was not disrupted by encoding and recalling visual patterns (see Figure 1). The effect size was .035. Relation between Time and Participants Scores 120 110 100 90 85.28 77.5 78.02 80 76.97 72.1 70 digit delayed 60 50 40 30 20 1 2 3 4 5 Figure 1. Digit sequence means scores for delayed condition, interpolated by visual pattern immediate recall. The trend for delayed digit recall is reliably constant, while the visual pattern varied through the five different levels of demand. Figure 2 shows a significant decrement on the performance of visual pattern as the levels of demand increased [F (4,116) = 1.38, MSE = 308.82, p <.001]. Participants showed a total drop in performance of 9.7% between the lowest (81.6%) demand and the highest (91.3%) demand conditions. The effect size was 0.367, indicating that this results can be extended to a bigger size of population. The effect size was .046. 29 E.N. 7228847 Relation between Time and Participants Scores 120 110 100 87.51 91.35 85.83 83.43 90 80 81.59 70 visual pattern immediate recall 60 50 40 30 20 1 2 3 4 5 Figure 2. Means scores for visual pattern immediate recall. Increasing the the level of visual pattern produced a decrement in participants perfornce in this task. 3.2 Visual pattern delayed recall x digit sequence immediate recall There was no significant effect of dual task for visual pattern delayed recall [F (4,116) =.94, MSE = 461.4, p>.001] which means that maintaining visual patterns in memory was not disrupted by encoding and recalling sequences of digits. It can be observed in Figure 3 that the trend for visual pattern delayed recall is reliably constant, whereas the digit immediate recall trend varied through the five different levels of demand. The effect size was 0.32. 30 E.N. 7228847 Relation between Time and Participants Scores 120 110 100 90 80.36 80 80.32 79.67 73.59 72.73 70 visual pattern delayed recall 60 50 40 30 20 1 2 3 4 5 Figure 3. Visual pattern means scores for delayed condition, interpolated by visual digits sequences immediate recall. The trend for delayed visual patter recall is reliably constant, while the visual patterns varied through the five different levels of demand. There was a significant decrement on the performance as the level of demand on digit sequences increased [F (4116) = 16.8, MSE = 327.79, p <.001]. Participants showed a total drop in performance of 32.5% between the lowest (66.2%) demand and the highest (98.7%) demand conditions (see Figure 4). 31 E.N. 7228847 Relation between Time and Participants Scores 120 110 100 98.88 98.66 90 89.03 83.03 80 70 66.16 digit sequences immediate recall 60 50 40 30 20 1 2 3 4 5 Figure 4. Means scores for digit sequences immediate recall. Increasing the the level of digit sequences produced a decrement in participants perfornce in this task. It can be observed from the trends of Figure 1 and Figure 2 that the trends are not showing opposite directions, showing no evidence for a trade off between the two tasks presented here. The same can be verified from Figure 3 and Figure 4. 32 E.N. 7228847 4. Discussion Previous studies have shown that two concurrent memory tasks can be performed by healthy adults with minimal impact on the performance on either task (Cocchini, 2002; MacPherson, 2004). Also, previous studies have shown that varying the demand of one of the tasks involved in the dual task doesn’t affect the performance in those tasks (Logie, 2004). These findings seem to indicate the operation of distinct, specialized cognitive resources for each task. However, there is still an ongoing debate about whether or not working memory is characterized by multiple specialized systems or by a non-specialized indistinct system. The dual task is a reliable way to tackle this issue and varying the demand of the cognitive load on a dual task experiment allows exploring in detail how resources interact with the demand from cognitive tasks (Logie et al., 2004; Logie et al., 2007). The present study aimed to examine how resources are allocated during the dual task when the demands of one of the tasks are manipulated. The population chosen to be part of the experiment was healthy adults since most of literature concerning the dual task is related to Alzheimer’s disease. It is equally as important to conduct more studies to explore dual task capacity in the healthy brain. The results reported in the present study show that varying the level of complexity of one task did not affect the performance in the concurrent task. The experiment showed an overall reduction in performance for participants with increasing task demand. However, no evidence was found that manipulating the load of one task affected performance on the other task. These findings are difficult to conciliate with a single general-limited capacity, since this would be very sensitive to a manipulation of the cognitive demand. This is in accordance with a multiple-component model of working memory (Baddeley, 2007) and this model can offer a very reasonable interpretation of the data 33 E.N. 7228847 found here. Digit span task is processed by the phonological loop and the visual patterns by the visual sketchpad. As both systems have distinct resources available for each task, the loads used for processing one activity did not affect the processing for the other activity. More specifically, the prediction for the operation of the tasks presented in this experiment is that when participants were holding digit sequences in their memory as well as Vergauwe al. (2010) encoding and recalling visual patterns bellow they span, they had the capacity for verbal material being exhausted while the capacity for visual material processing was not being totally used. This is explained by the fact that performance is below span and there was still residual capacity spared, which made that participants performed quite well in these tasks having very high scores. When the level of demand was set at span a non significant small decrement was observed in their performance, which can be explained by two very demanding tasks being performed at the same time and the additional cost needed for a coordination operation between those tasks (Cocchini et al, 2002; Duff & Logie, 2001; Baddeley et al, 1998). Then, the levels of visual performance immediate recall were increased. This made the scores decrease, as the load was higher than visual sketch pad could support. The resources were not enough to deal with the demanding task but there were no sign that this affected the performance in the verbal task, suggesting that each system conserve their own pool of resources. There was no sign of a trade off between those verbal and visual processing Vergauwe al. (2010) suggesting that. This pattern of results is very similar to that found in previous studies (Baddeley et al., 1986, Cocchini, et al., 2002; Logie, et al., 2004; Macpherson, et al., 2007, Sebastian et al., 2006; Duff & Logie, 2001). Cochini et al and Macpherson et al. (2004) proposed that people can perform two memory tasks without decrement in the tasks is possible. In addition, when Logie et al. (2004) and Logie et al. (2007) manipulated the 34 E.N. 7228847 demand of tasks in the dual task no decrement on the coupled task was found; this seems to reflect the fact that two different activities utilised independent resource pools. An interesting factor that appeared was regarding the trend found on the visual patterns immediate recall task. Those did not show the sharp decrease showed by digit sequences immediate recall. This could be explained by some of the more complex patterns in the immediate condition recall being more difficult to recall than the other pattern across the task, making participants achieving same proportion in the scores than they achieved in the less complex patterns. It could be argued that participants were not performing at their maximum span when their individual span was checked and they were putting more effort when performing the dual task. However, the means for digit sequence was similar to Cocchini et al (2002) and the means for visual pattern were higher than the ones found Cocchini et al., 2004. The results cannot be explained by participants not performing at their maximum capacity (e.g. the span checking procedure were not sensitive due to participants putting more effort on dual ask), since the results showed that performance was constant and the increase of the task demand disrupted as the task demand increased. Vergauwe al. (2010) also varied the demands of two dual tasks combining visual storage and processing tasks and two verbal storage and processing tasks, although contrary to other studies and the experiment reported here, it was observed decrement in participants performance. These divergent findings may be regarded to differences between the procedures that were used by different authors. The tasks included in the dual need to draw upon different domains of working memory. The tasks used by Vergauwe et al. (2010) are said to be supported by different domains. However semantic judgement, for example, is prone to involve visual content (some participants use metal imagery to remember the elements for semantic judgement and use verbal coding to support visual 35 E.N. 7228847 task, when retaining the name of the colours for example ain colours). Those tests are not empirically proved to use the codes they are intending to. Conversely, the Digit Span Task is considered to measure verbal capacity and empirical bases support it. This task was validated as a technique that measures short working memory and was exhausted tested and confirmed as depending on the phonological loop component of working memory (for a review see Baddeley, 2007). Besides, visual patterns were proven to be an important tool to measure visual memory (Della Sala et al., 1999; Logie et al., 1990). Morey and Cowan (2005) further do not use tasks that seem to be purely measuring they domain intent to (a task is said to measure visual capacity, but it involves other stimuli, other codes, such as verbal). For instance, the task used for Cowan and Morey (2005) involved colours and the some participants could easily be using verbal coding to support this visual task. The current experiment procedure was designed to avoid any kind of contamination from stimuli that would require cognitive processes that not the ones being required by the tasks. This is what did not happen in the Cowan and Morey (2005), they presented a cross pattern in between two tasks to signalise the start of another task and participants had to pres keys with the letters experiment. Yet, these authors do not titrate the task for individuals’ performance. The disruption caused by the dual task condition could mean that that level of difficulty in one of the tasks is above individual level and the disruption is being caused simply due to overload of the individual limited capacity for each specific domain. Cowan’s (2001) theory may interpret the results reported here in terms of a single pool of resources, with the preload task causing temporary activation that gradually decayed the memory preload involves temporary activation that gradually decays while the attentional system is focused on the interpolated task. During delayed recall, attention 36 E.N. 7228847 is then focused on retrieving items from decaying traces. So participants held a preload content (verbal or visual) in the passively held store while the contents in the intervening task were being focused by attention. However Cowan’s theory would find some difficulty to explain how individual performing a task in a very low level (two below their span) did not left extra capacity to be used on the task that was demanding more capacity to be dealt with the second task In this case, the task one or two levels above individuals span. If a trade off between two tasks within a single limited capacity model was to be considered the results pattern should show opposite trends which means that as performance in one task increased performance in the other task would decrease. On the contrary, the data suggest a multi-component working memory model. The multi-component working memory model is also supported by studies related to individual differences. In a study examining individual ability in verbal and spatial processing and storage complex span tasks, Bayliss, Jarrold, Gunn and Baddeley (2003) found that individual differences in processing efficiency and storage capacity each account for unique variance in complex span tasks performance. This means that processing and storage are independent, since they offer independent constraints on complex span tasks performance. Accordingly, Hitch and Hutton (2001) found that arithmetic and verbal tasks were explored in children and the results showed that reading span and operation span accounted for unique variance in reading attainment, reflecting a separation of resources for tow different domains A possible caveat for the current experiment is that the visual patterns used for the visual task were not validated before, therefore, the same experiment redesign taking into consideration these particularities in future experiment. Also, as the experiment was made on paper and the time was controlled by the experimenter, it does not offer the same time accuracy time as a computer-paced programme could offer. However, it does not 37 E.N. 7228847 invalidate the findings provided by this experiment. A more accurate measure of time, using a computer paced program would provide a more reliable study. Thus, a possibility for a future study is to reproduce this experiment, although in this new study using a validate pattern test (i.e. the visual Pattern test- reference) and a computer-paced program. In conclusion, the experiment reported that when two tasks are performed when one of the tasks is set at a fixed level and the other task has its demand manipulated, there was no trade off between two different activities (i.e., visual and verbal). The results found in this study have important implications for the theoretical matters of working memory, which is the definition about if working memory is better characterised as comprising a single limited capacity or independent pool of resources that can operate in parallel. The fact that the level of one task does not place a constraint on the other task reflects the operation of domain-specific memory systems with independent pools of resources - as predicted by the multi-component working memory model. Conversely, theories that assume a single general-limited capacity would find difficulty in explaining why the demand manipulation of one task did not produce any effect in the other task. The present study added more evidence about distinct systems containing independent pool of resources that can operate in parallel. 38 E.N. 7228847 References Baddeley, A. & Della Sala, S. (1996). Working memory and executive control. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences, 351, 1397-1403. Baddeley, A., Emslie, H., Kolodny, J. and Duncan, J. (1998). Random generation and the executive control in working memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51 A ,4, 819-852. Baddeley, A. & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In Bower, G. (Ed.), Recent Advances in Learning and Motivation (pp. 47-90). London: Academic Press. Baddeley, A., Baddeley, H., Bucks, R. and Wilcock, G. (2001). Attentional control in Alzheimers’s disease. Brain, 124, 1492-1508. Baddeley, A. (2007). Working Memory, thought, and action. Oxford University Press: Oxford, England. Baddeley, A. and Logie, R. (1999). Working memory: The multiple-component model. In: Miyaki, A. and Shah, P. (Eds.) Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control (pp. 28-61). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Baddeley, A. (1996). Exploring the central executive. The quarterly journal of experimental psychology, 49 A, 1, 5-28. Baddeley, A., Logie, R, Bressi, S, Della Sala, S. and Spinnler, H. (1986). Dementia and working memory. The quarterly journal of experimental psychology, 38A, 603-618. Barnard, P. (1999). Interacting Cognitive subsystems: modelling working memory. In: Miyaki, A. and Shah, P. (Eds.) Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control (pp- 298-339). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Barrouillet, P., Bernardin, S.& Camos, V. (2004). Time constraints and resource sharing in adult’s working memory spans. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 83100. Bayliss, D., Jarrold, C., Deborah, G. & Baddeley, A. (2003). The complexities of complex span: Explaining individual differences in working memory in children and adults. Journal of experimental psychology: General ,132, 71-92. 39 E.N. 7228847 Cochinni, G., Logie, R., Della Dala, S., Macpherson, S. and Baddeley, A. (2002). Concurrent performance of two memory tasks: evidence for domain-specific working memory systems. Memory and Cognition, 30, 7, 1086-1095. Corsi, P. (1972). Human memory and the medial temporal region of the brain. Dissertation Abstracts International, 34 (02), 891B. (University Microfilms No. AAI05–77717). Cowan, N. (1999). An embedded-processes model of working memory. InA. Myiake & P. Shah (Eds.), Models of Working Memory (pp. 62-101). New York: Cambrigde University Press. Cowan, N. (2001). The Magical Number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of the mental storage. Behavioural & Brain Sciences, 24, 87-114. Daneman,M. & CarpenterP. (1980). Individual differences in Working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 19, 450-466. Della Sala, S., Baddeley, A., Papagno, C. and Spinnler, H. (1995). Dual task paradigm: A means to examine the central executive. In: J. Grafman, K. Holyak and F. Boller (eds.), Structures and Functions of The Human Prefrontal Cortex (Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, Vol. 769, pp. 161-190). New York: New York Academy of Sciences. Della Sala, S., Gray., Baddely, A., Allamano, N. & Wilson, L. (1999). Pattern span: A tool for unwelding visuo-spatial memory. Neupsychologia, 37, 1189-1199. Della Salla, S., Foley, J., Beschin, N., Allerhand, M. & Logie, R. (in press) Assessing dual task performance using a paper-and-pencil test: Normative data. Duff, S and Logie, R. (2001). Processing and storage in working memory span. The quarterly journal of experimental psychology. 54A ,1, 31-48. Baddeley, A. & Hitch, G. (2007). Working memory: past, present...and future? In: Osaka, Naoyuki, O., Logie, R. & D’Esposito, M. (Eds.), The cognitive neuroscience of working memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp 1-20. Hitch G., Towse, J. & Hutton, U. (2001). What limits children’s working memory span? Theoretical accounts and applications for scholastic development. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General (130) 184-198. Logie, R. (1995). Visuo-Spatial Working Memory. Hove, UK: Erlbaum. Logie, R., Della Sala S., Cocchini, G., Baddeley, A. (2004). Is there a specific executive capacity for dual task coordination? Evidence from Alzheimer’s disease. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A (1), 31-48. Logie, R., Zucco, G. & Baddeley A. (1990). Interference With Visual Short-Term Memory. Acta Psychologica, 75, 55-74. 40 E.N. 7228847 Logie, R., Della Sala, S., Laiacona, M., Chalmers, P. & Wynn, V. (1996). Group Agregates and Individual Reability: The Case of Verbal Short-Term. Memory. Memory & Cognition, 24(3), 305-321. Logie, R., Della Sala, S., MacPherson, S. & Cooper, J. (2007). Dual Task Demands on Encoding and Retrieval Processes: Evidence from Healthy Adult Ageing. Cortex, 43, 159169. MacPherson, S., Della Salla, S., Logie, S. & Wilcock, G. (2007). Specific AD Impairment in Concurrent Performance of Two Memory Tasks. Cortex, 43, 858-865. Miyaki, A. and Shah, P. (1999). Models of working memory: An introduction. In: Miyaki, A. and Shah, P. Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 1-27. Morey, C. & Cowan, N. (2005). When Do Visual and Verbal Memories Conflict? The Importance of Working Memory Load and Retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 31 (4), 703-713. Naveh-Benjaminm, M., Guez, J., Craik, F. & Kreuger, S. (2005). Divided attention in younger and older adults: Effects of strategy on relatedness performance and secondary tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition (31), 520537. Sebastian, M., Menor, J.& Elousa, M. (2006). Attentional dysfunction of the central executive in AD: Evidence from dual task and perseveration errors. Cortex , 42, 1015-1020. Vergauwe, E., Barrouillet P. & Camos, V. (2009). Visual and spatial working memory are not that dissociated after all: a time-based resource-sharing account. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and cognition, 35(4), 1012-28. Vergawe, E., Barrouilet, P. & Camos, V. (2010). Do mental processes share a domain-general resource? Psychological Science, 21 ,3, 384-390. 41