The Back Story of Psychology An Examination of the Personal and Social Contexts of the Discovery Process Submitted by Barbara Lusk blusk@ccccd.edu 972-548-6809 1 The Back Story of Psychology An Examination of the Personal and Social Contexts of the Discovery Process “Yet it is good to study the discoveries of others in a way that discloses to us the source of the discoveries and renders them in a sort our own. And I wish that authors would give us the history of their discoveries and the steps by which they have arrived at them. When they neglect to do so, we must try to divine these steps in order to profit the more from their works.” Leibniz to Louis Bourquet, 22 March 1714 Research in the history and sociology of science clearly indicates that scientists do not have great flashes of insight, weigh the evidence they gather, construct new theories and evaluate the ideas of others in a vacuum, untouched by their own very personal experiences and the broader social contexts in which they are embedded. Scientific inquiry, it is argued, does not typically proceed in an orderly fashion, with all concerned remaining objective, fair-minded, and untouched by jealousy, political expediency, and petty concerns. These historians and sociologists place tremendous emphasis on social and cultural factors affecting the development of science. Recently, a new wave of scholars, self-proclaimed “psychobiographers,” has added their distinctive perspective. Here, researchers delve more deeply into the personal lives of their subjects – early memories, traumatic experiences, the parent-child relationship, sibling rivalry, love-life, professional controversies, public and private battles with colleagues, foes, and their own demons – linking these (and more) to the development of the ideas, the theories they construct, and the manner in which their ideas are received by the relevant community of psychologists. As I consider my academic training as a clinical psychologist and my years of teaching introductory psychology courses, I am sorely aware of my own lack of knowledge about the history of science, and about the details of the lives of the individuals who shaped the field of psychology. At best, the standard texts relegate biographical information to a portion of a first chapter, a “boxed” entry located in the margins of later chapters, or it is buried in an appendix. Worse, the information that is provided is rarely more than a caricature. Mythic episodes stand in place of a systematic analysis of the unique personality of individual scholars, and a sociohistorical account of the development of scientific thought is never mentioned. These abbreviated accounts are, practically speaking, worthless. Humans are, by nature, curious and nosy. Interest in the “back story” of psychology seems a very natural response to the sanitized version of the “science” that is typically presented for review. But more important, there is great value for me and for students in knowing both the personal and historical/social context of the development of psychological knowledge. This information provides flesh and bones to the more abstract theories and processes of science; it makes the materials come alive and become more memorable. Should I receive this grant, I will study the autobiographies and biographies of some of the most significant figures in the development of psychological theory and practice. Major 2 figures drawn from the areas of psychoanalytic theory, behaviorism, humanism, animal behavior, social psychology, personality psychology, and cognitive/social development will be included. Included, too, will be women who were pioneers in the discipline. The purpose of this study grant is multi-layered. I will be reading these materials with an eye toward examining the following set of questions: First, what precisely is the new field of psychobiography? What are its main tenets and modus operandi? How does it differ from more mainstream work in the history of psychology? How does the new field of psychobiography fit within mainstream psychology? Is psychobiography considered a viable method of inquiry in the discipline? Is the current climate in psychology open to the study of individual lives? Second, how are the personal lives of the creators of the work I have chosen to examine connected to their finished products? For example, there is a well-known story of how Gordon Allport’s famous meeting with Freud soured him of psychoanalytic theory, but also ultimately influenced the development of his personality theory. Jung, in an autobiographical account, describes dreams he had as a child that ultimately shaped his ideas of consciousness. Who nurtured – and impeded - their creative process? Were colleagues close at hand and at a distance more of a help or a hindrance? Did family obligations and the day-to-day vicissitudes of life intrude? How, precisely, did these scholars arrive at their theories? What false starts, serious blunders, and happy accidents accompanied the discovery process? Third, what was the cognitive/intellectual setting that existed and influenced the discovery process and, ultimately, the reception of new ideas? As stated earlier, no ideas emerge in a vacuum. Most theories are constructed as a response to “accepted” ideas that may be considered outdated, limiting, inaccurate, or useless. How does this process of unseating a previously accepted set of ideas or principals unfold? Competition, petty rivalries, and institutional cronyism surely affect these battles. Moreover, competition is not limited to psychologists with divergent perspectives. Often, two or more psychologists are involved in a “race” to be the first to “discover” important theories and win the recognition that it confers. Freud, for example, was involved in many “priority disputes” during his most creative years. Fourth, apart from intellectual concerns, what broader social factors affected the response of the scientific community to these ideas? Theories may be accepted or rejected because of the right “timing.” Issues of status, money, and political climate all influence the ultimate acceptance or rejection of theory. This type of information needed to answer the foregoing questions is rarely included in either journal publications of primary work or in any textbook overview of theoretical findings. The answer to these questions, I believe, is critical to an understanding of the process of the development of knowledge – the main objective of this study. 3 Plan of Study The sources I shall be examining are of five types: (1) full biographic treatments by noted historians in the field, (2) briefer biographical sketches that have appeared in the six volume Portraits of Pioneers in Psychology series, (3) autobiographies that have appeared in the widely acclaimed A History of Psychology in Autobiography, (4) general historical accounts of the development of psychology, and (5) a series of articles published by psychobiographers in both the recently published Handbook of Psychobiography edited by William Todd Schultz and standard journals in psychology. Week 1 To provide a frame for the biographies and autobiographies I have chosen to examine, I shall begin with an overview of the field of psychobiography. In his introduction to the handbook, William Todd Schultz provides a description of the specialty, its place in the field of psychology, and information regarding what is considered by the experts to be “good” versus “bad” psychobiographies. William M. Runyan, one of the founders of the field, provides a counterpoint in his chapter, “Evolving Conceptions of Psychobiography and the Study of Lives: Encounters With Psychoanalysis, Personality Psychology, and Historical Science.” Next, I shall read James William Anderson’s “The Psychobiographical Study of Psychologists,” which surveys a number of cases that have recently appeared. Last, to get some sense of how a practicing psychobiographer goes about his/her work, I shall read Nicole Barenbaum’s case study of Gordon Allport, “Four, Two, or One? Gordon Allport and the Unique Personality.” Weeks 2 - 4 Next, I shall turn to two full length biographies of the giants in psychodynamic theory. Peter Gay’s FREUD: A Life for Our Time is considered to be the authoritative account of Freud’s remarkable career. In this massive 650 page volume, Gay, the Sterling Professor of History at Yale University, also builds a case for history informed by psychoanalysis. I will follow Freud’s biography with Deirdre Bair’s Jung. A previous National Book Award recipient for her biography of Samuel Beckett, Bair has been a literary journalist and university professor of comparative literature. Her text on Jung includes restricted interviews, analytic diaries, and early drafts of Jung’s writings, in addition to private archival information she was allowed to access. Weeks 5 - 7 Watsonian behaviorism developed, in part, in opposition to both functionalism and psychodynamic theory’s emphasis on internal processes. Kerry W. Buckley’s 4 Mechanical Man: John Broadus Watson and the Beginnings of Behaviorism, addresses these issues in detail. Buckley received his Ph.D. in American Social and Intellectual History from the University of Massachusetts at Amherst. His work on Watson is considered to be somewhat of a hybrid—half biography and half history of behavioral psychology. Therefore the book serves to provide details of Watson’s life, as well as information about the impact of behaviorism on society. Today, the name most associated with the behaviorist approach is B.F. Skinner. Daniel W. Bjork’s book, B.F. Skinner: A Life is based on intensive interviews with Skinner and his family and information from the Skinner collection in the Harvard archives. Described as objective, well-documented, and insightful, this book is the only complete biography of one of the most controversial and influential learning theorists. Bjork is a professor of history at St. Mary’s University in San Antonio. Week 8 I shall turn next to two scholars instrumental in developing personality theory. In their autobiographical accounts of their work, Gordon Allport and Henry A. Murray outline the many factors that affected the development of their thought and the reception of their ideas. These will be supplemented by Barenbaum’s two excellent psychobiographical case studies: one on Allport (mentioned above), the second, “Henry A. Murray: Personology as Biography, Science and Art,” which appears in the Portraits series. Week 9-10 Abraham Maslow is considered to be a leader in the “third wave” of psychology. This movement, a reaction to the determinism of psychoanalysis and behaviorism, is addressed in Edward Hoffman’s biography Abraham Maslow—The Right to Be Human. Hoffman, a clinical psychologist in private practice in New York City, presents a compelling account of Maslow’s life and work. Equal in stature to Maslow in the development of humanism is Carl Rogers. To explore his role in shaping the humanist movement, I shall read Lakin’s “Carl Rogers and the Culture of Psychotherapy” published in the Portraits series. Week 10-12 Erik Erikson, trained by Sigmund and Anna Freud, departed from psychoanalytic doctrine by insisting that social circumstances were no less important than inner psyche in determining human personality. His theory of psychosocial development over the life span is considered to be one of the most influential developmental theories in psychology. Lawrence J. Friedman’s comprehensive biography, Identity’s Architect, a Biography of Erik H. Erikson, explores the relationship between Erikson’s personal life and his groundbreaking ideas. Friedman is a noted historian and professor at Indiana University, as well as a four-time National Endowment of the Humanities recipient. 5 Jean Piaget, best known for reorganizing cognitive development theory into a series of stages, is considered to be a pioneer in the constructivist theory of knowing. To explore the life and work of this leader in cognitive theory, I shall read Zigler's and Gilman’s account, “The Legacy of Jean Piaget,” in the Portraits series. Weeks 12-13 Much important work in the study of psychology is derived from animal studies. I shall focus on two of the towering figures in animal/comparative psychology, Harry Harlow, best known for his research on the importance love and affection in primates, and Robert Yerkes, namesake and director of the first US lab for the study of non-human primates. To gain a better understanding of the professional and biographical aspects of Harlow’s groundbreaking work, I shall rely on Deborah Blum, a Pulitzer Prize-winning author and professor of journalism at the University of Wisconsin. Her book, Love at Goon Park: Harry Harlow and the Science of Affection, has received high praise from reviewers for her deft handling and intermingling of biographical and professional matters. For the story of Yerkes’ life, research, institution-building efforts, and the reception accorded to his efforts, I shall read noted historian of psychology Donald A. Dewsbury’s recently published Monkey Farm: A History of the Yerkes Laboratories of Primate Biology, Orange Park, Florida, 1930-1965. Week 14 Social Psychology has always been a vibrant sub-specialty in the discipline of psychology and always seems to engage the attention of students enrolled in general psychology classes. The lives and work of Leon Festinger, Stanley Milgram, Floyd Allport, and Soloman Asch, all leaders in social psychology, are explored in brief chapters in the Portraits series. J.W. Brehm writes a detailed account of Festinger in the article, “Leon Festinger: Beyond the Obvious.” T. Blass discusses Milgram in “Stanley Milgram: A Life of Inventiveness and Controversy.” Katz, Johnson, and Nichols provide a comprehensive account of Floyd Allport in “Floyd Henry Allport: Founder of Social Psychology As a Behavioral Science.” Last, Clark McCauley and Paul Rozin write about the work of Soloman Asch in their report: “Solomon Asch: Scientist and Humanist.” Week 15 Most histories of science have had the tendency to ignore the important roles that women have played in the development of their discipline. Happily, recent work has begun to correct this glaring defect. For an overview of the contributions of women in 6 psychology I shall read Untold Lives: The First Generation of American Women Psychologists, published by Scarborough, an associate professor of psychology at State University of New York, and her collaborator Furumoto, a professor of psychology at Wellesley. Next, I will explore the lives of two of the first women psychologists through Alexandra Rutherford’s, “Mother of Behavior Therapy and Beyond: Mary Cover Jones and the Study of the ‘Whole Child’”, and Furumoto’s “From Paired Associates to a Psychology of Self: The Intellectual Odyssey of Mary Whiton Calkins.” Each of these appears in the Portraits series. Conclusion The study of science should have something to do with the study of individuals— real people who reside in the real world. As Horace Judson states, “ . . . scientists understand very well [that] personality has always been an inseparable part of their styles of enquiry, a potent if unacknowledged factor in their results. Indeed, no art or popular entertainment is so carefully built as is science upon the individual talents, preferences, and habits of its leaders.” This study grant allows me to do the focused reading of biographical and psychobiographical accounts of many of the most important figures in the history of psychology. The knowledge gained will enhance every course I teach. I will use the material to develop a more comprehensive introduction to general psychology, life span psychology, and the psychology of personality. First impressions matter. The richer and more detailed the introduction, the more likely that students will be engaged and excited about pursuing knowledge, participating in learning. I also anticipate collaborating on the development of a learning community organized around a theme of the process of discovery. The readings for this study grant would certainly be excellent preparation for such an endeavor. Professionally, I will benefit by becoming a better teacher, more informed, more excited about psychology as an example of the broader topic of the history of science. I anticipate taking advantage of opportunities to present pieces of what I have learned both locally, at Psi Beta and at the Seniors Active in Learning (S.A.I.L.) visiting lecture series, and nationally at conferences specific to this field. This is an entirely new world of information for me—at this rather late date in my professional career. But, as Michelangelo said at 87, “I am still learning.” Thus, this study grant is not only about the history of psychology through biography, but also the history of the development of knowledge. As I did the initial work necessary to prepare this grant proposal, I recognized how much there is to gain from understanding something of the way scientific inquiry unfolds. This endeavor really is personal, not just business as usual. 7 Bibliography Allport, G. W. (1967). “Autobiography.” In E. G. Boring & G. Lindzey (Eds.), A History of Psychology in Autobiography (Vol. V, pp. 3-25). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Anderson, J. W. (2005). “The Psychobiographical Study of Psychologists.” In W. T. Schultz (Ed.), Handbook of Psychobiography (pp. 203-209). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Bair, D. (2003). Jung: A Biography. Boston: Little, Brown and Company. Barenbaum, N. B. (2005). “Four, Two, or One? Gordon Allport and the Unique Personality.” In W. T. Schultz (Ed.), Handbook of Psychobiography (pp. 223 – 239). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Barenbaum, N. B. (2006). “Henry A. Murray: Personology as Biography, Science, and Art.” In D. A. Dewsbury, L. T. Benjamin, Jr., & M. Wertheimer (Eds.), Portraits of Pioneers in Psychology (Vol. VI, chapter 11). Washington, D.C: American Psychological Association. Bjork, D. W. (1993). B. F. Skinner: A Life. New York: BasicBooks, a Division of HarperCollins. Blass, T. (1996). “Stanley Milgram: A Life of Inventiveness and Controversy.” In G. A. Kimble, C. A. Boneau, & M. Wertheimer, (Eds.), Portraits of Pioneers in Psychology (Vol. II, pp. 315-331). Washington, D.C: American Psychological Association. Blum, D. (2002). Love at Goon Park: Harry Harlow and the Science of Affection. New York: Berkley Publishing Group. Brehm, J. W. (1998). “Leon Festinger: Beyond the Obvious.” In G. A. Kimble & M. Wertheimer, (Eds.), Portraits of Pioneers in Psychology (Vol. III, pp. 329-344). Washington, D.C: American Psychological Association. Buckley, K. W. (1989). Mechanical Man: John Broadus Watson and the Beginnings of Behaviorism. New York: The Guilford Press. Dewsbury, D. A. (2006). Monkey Farm: A History of the Yerkes Laboratories of Primate Biology, Orange Park, Florida, 1930-1965. Lewisburg, PA: Bucknell University Press. 8 Friedman, L. J. (1999). Identity’s Architect: A Biography of Erik H. Erikson. New York: Scribner. Furumoto, L. (1991). “From ‘Paired Associates’ to a Psychology of Self: The Intellectual Odyssey of Mary Whiton Calkins.” In G. A. Kimble, M. Wertheimer, & C. L. White, (Eds.), Portraits of Pioneers in Psychology, (Vol. I, pp. 57-72). Washington, D.C: American Psychological Association. Gay, P. (1988). Freud: A Life for Our Time. New York: W. W. Norton and Company. Hoffman, E. (1988). The Right to Be Human: A Biography of Abraham Maslow. Los Angeles, CA: Jeremy P. Tarcher, Inc. Katz, D., Johnson, B. T., & Nichols, D. R. (1998). “Floyd Henry Allport: Founder of Social Psychology As a Behavorial Science.” In G. A. Kimble & M. Wertheimer, (Eds.), Portraits of Pioneers in Psychology, (Vol. III, pp. 122-142). Washington, D.C: American Psychological Association. Lakin, M. (1998). “Carl Rogers and the Culture of Psychotherapy.” In G. A. Kimble & M. Wertheimer, (Eds.), Portraits of Pioneers in Psychology (Vol. III, pp. 245258). Washington, D.C: American Psychological Association. McCauley, C. & Rozin, P. (2003). “Solomon Asch: Scientist and Humanist.” In G. A. Kimble, & M. Wertheimer, (Eds.), Portraits of Pioneers in Psychology (Vol. V, pp. 249-261). Washington, D.C: American Psychological Association. Murray, H. A. (1967). “Henry A. Murray”. In Boring, E. G. & Lindzey, G. (Eds.), A History of Psychology in Autobiography (Vol. 5, pp. xxx-xxx). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Runyan, W. M. (2005). “Evolving Conceptions of Psychobiography and the Study of Lives: Encounters With Psychoanalysis, Personality Psychology, and Historical Science.” In W. T. Schultz, (Ed.), Handbook of Psychobiography (pp. 19-41). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Rutherford, A. (2006). “Mother of Behavior Therapy and Beyond: Mary Cover Jones and the Study of the ‘Whole Child’.” In D. A. Dewsbury, L. T. Benjamin, Jr., & M. Wertheimer, (Eds.), Portraits of Pioneers in Psychology (Vol. VI, chapter 12). Washington, D.C: American Psychological Association. Scarborough, E. & Furumoto, L. (1989). Untold Lives: The First Generation of American Women Psychologists. New York: Columbia University Press. Schultz, W. T. (2005). “Introducing Psychobiography”. In W. T. Schultz, (Ed.), 9 Handbook of Psychobiography (pp.3-18). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Zigler, E. & Gilman, E. (1998). “The Legacy of Jean Piaget.” In G. A. Kimble & M. Wertheimer, (Eds.), Portraits of Pioneers in Psychology (Vol. III, pp. 145-160). Washington, D.C: American Psychological Association. 10