What Should We Ask About Intelligence? Robert J. Sternberg from The American Scholar, Spring 1996 5 1. 10 15 20 25 30 35 40 45 Alice was the admissions officer's dream. She had 800s on her boards, close to 4.0 average, and glowing letters of recommendation from her teachers. She was accepted by every graduate program to which she applied. We were thrilled when she decided to matriculate at Yale's doctoral program of psychology. 2. The tests and other predictors of success were correct as far as they went: Alice excelled in her first year of course work, competing with just one other student for the highest academic average in an already highly selective program. Then something went wrong – something big. By the time Alice was done with the program, she was roughly in the bottom 20 percent of her cohort. And she was highly motivated to succeed. 3. What went wrong with Alice is what has gone wrong with thousands and thousands of students: they are brilliant when it comes to remembering and even analyzing ideas, but they are dim when it comes to generating their own ideas. They may have 700s or even 800s on their boards, and often IQs of 140s and above, but they seem to lack even an ounce of creativity. In other terms, they are analytically, but not creatively, intelligent. 4. Contrast the fate of Alice to that of Barbara. Barbara applied to Yale's graduate program in psychology with good but not outstanding grades. More notable were her superb letters of recommendation from eminent people and her record of published work. The impressive creativity of this work was apparent to almost anyone who took the trouble to read it. But Barbara's test scores, although not awful, were modest. Barbara, unlike Alice, was rejected. 5. Barbara was one of the lucky ones. I hired her as a research associate. She demonstrated exceptional creative abilities, and two years later, when she reapplied to our graduate program, she was admitted as the top pick. But for every Barbara who gets a chance, there are unknown thousands like her who are consigned to the academic waste-basket – they never get the chance that Barbara got. 6. Paul might have seemed like the ideal admission's candidate. He combined Alice's analytical ability with Barbara's creative ability. His professors were delighted with him and expected him to be a smash hit on the academic job market. When Paul went to the academic job market, he was asked to interview at every institution to which he applied – an enviable record. His hit rate for getting jobs wasn't quite so enviable, however: he was offered only one position, at the weakest department to which he applied. Clearly, he was far from a desirable commodity, his analytical and creative abilities notwithstanding. 50 55 60 65 70 75 80 2 7. Ironically, Sam, who had received no interview offers at all in the first round, was later offered many of the interview opportunities Paul had initially flubbed. Sam was offered several positions and within a few years had tenure, whereas Paul was out of a job. Sam's work was nothing special, but he knew what his department valued, and he delivered. 8. What went wrong with Paul was straightforward: Paul was so lacking in common sense that he couldn't hide his arrogance even on the one day he needed to hide it – the day on which he had a job interview. And once hired, his arrogance led him quickly to become a pariah among his colleagues. Paul was analytically and creatively intelligent but lacking in practical intelligence. Sam, more modest in analytical and creative intelligence, was able to translate his practical intelligence into good, although perhaps not distinguished, career success. 9. The stories of Alice and Barbara and of Paul and Sam are all true, with only the names changed. They are also, in their themes, common stories in academe. But stories like these are what have led many psychologists, myself among them, to conclude, that conventional notions of intelligence may be correct as far as they go but that they do not go far enough. These psychologists have suggested that conventional notions of intelligence (a) define intelligence too restrictively and (b) often provide reasonable answers, but too narrow questions. The problem is that the answers may be fine, but the questions are not. 10. Today, the field of intelligence is going through a heated battle between adherents to a conventional paradigm that has its roots at the turn of the century and adherents to new paradigms that are attempting to turn the old paradigm on its head. And today, research as well as theory in the field of intelligence more and more is reflecting the revolutionary paradigms. 11. 85 90 95 100 What kinds of abilities do the revolutionary theories of intelligence compass? One such theory, Howard Gardner's theory of multiple intelligences, comprises seven abilities, which Gardner believes are distinct and relatively independent intelligences: (a) linguistic intelligence, used in reading a novel, writing a poem or an article such as this one, or generating an extemporaneous talk; (b) logical mathematical intelligence, used in solving mathematical problems, proving logical theorems, or completing categorical or other forms of syllogisms; (c) spatial intelligence, used in finding one's way in unfamiliar terrain, figuring out how to fit suitcases into the trunk of a car, or figuring out where in the playing field a baseball's batter's fly ball will land; (d) musical intelligence, used in remembering a tune, singing a song, or composing a sonata; (e) bodily-kinesthetic intelligence, used in dancing ballet, performing gymnastics, or playing tennis; (f) interpersonal intelligence, used in figuring out what other people mean from what they say, decoding what their facial expressions communicate, or deciding what is appropriate to say in an interaction with a superior; and (g) intrapersonal intelligence, understanding why one takes rejection so poorly, why one tends to be overconfident in certain instances, or why one has failed in achieving an important personal goal. 3 105 110 115 12. Another such theory, my own triarchic theory of human intelligence, follows from observations such as those of Alice, Barbara, Paul, and Sam, as described earlier in the article. This theory holds that intelligence has three major aspects: analytical, creative and practical. Conventional good test takers and good students tend to excel in analytical intelligence but not necessarily in the creative and practical aspects of intelligence. 13. An implication of this theory is that the reason conventional intelligence tests predict school achievement as well as they do is that schools, like conventional tests, tend to emphasize analytical skills far more than they emphasize creative and practical skills. Indeed, the latter kinds of skills may even be punished, as when students who depart from a teacher's expectations or point of view find themselves graded down for having done so. 14. 120 125 130 135 140 145 150 But if tests predict academic performance fairly well, why do we need revolutionary conceptions of intelligence, or, indeed, any new conceptions at all? After all, the tests are doing fairly well what they were designed to do. Why not leave well enough alone? 15. In one study, a colleague and I sought to address the question of just what the tests predict by validating the Graduate Record Examination (GRE) as a predictor of various kinds of performance in our own graduate program in psychology at Yale. Together with Wendy Williams, I asked professors to rate all their primary advisees who were in the psychology doctoral program between 1980 and 1991. Forty professors provided ratings on a 1 (low) to 7 (high) scale of 167 graduate students (68 males, 99 females) on five scales: (a) analytical abilities, (b) creative abilities, (c) practical abilities, (d) research abilities, and (f) teaching abilities. Means of these ratings did not differ significantly between the sexes. 16. We also had available students' scores on the GRE verbal, quantitative, and analytical sections, and on the advances test (for 73 of the students, because this test is optional). We further obtained the students' GPAs during their first and second years of graduate training. 17. We found that the GRE provided some prediction of first-year grades (correlations of 0.18 for the verbal section, 0.14 for the quantitative section, 0.17 for the analytical section, and most impressively, 0.37 for the advanced test). All of the correlations were statistically significant except that for the quantitative score. Thus, overall, the best predictor of future achievement was, perhaps not surprisingly, past achievement. But the GRE did not predict second-year grades. 18. The only consistent predictor of any of the more important measures of graduate performance (i.e., ratings of analytical, creative, practical, research, and teaching abilities, as well as of dissertation quality) was the GRE analytical section, and this prediction was for men only. Five of six correlations for this section were statistically significant for men, with a median correlation of 0.30, whereas none of the correlations were statistically significant for women, with a median correlation of 0.02. Thus, of eight sets 4 of correlations (four GRE scores for each of the two sexes), only one set provided statistically significant results. 155 160 19. The message of the GRE study is largely negative: for the most part, a conventional test of intellectual abilities did not provide much prediction of interesting aspects of academic performance. There was some prediction, but it was only of first-year grades, and beyond that, the prediction was for men only, and only for the analytical test. Would we stand to gain anything by a broader theory and broader tests of intelligence? 20. 165 170 175 180 185 190 195 200 Another study I was involved with, conducted over a five-year period in collaboration with Michel Ferrari, Pamela Clinkenbeard, and Elena Grigorenko, addressed this question. We sought to learn whether prediction of academic performance at the college level would improve if we used broader tests; more important, though, we asked whether students would perform better if they were taught and their achievement assessed in ways that reflected their patterns of abilities. 21. We invited high schools from around the country and abroad to nominate students for a summer college-level psychology course to be taught at Yale University. Nominated students were then encouraged to take a test of intelligence based on the triarchic theory of human intelligence. The test measured not only the analytical abilities assessed by conventional IQ tests, but also creative and practical abilities. It measured these abilities in the verbal, quantitative, and figural (geometric) domains, using both multiple-choice and essay test items. 22. Eventually, 199 students became part of the program in the summer of 1993. The program was simultaneously a course and an experiment. Students were selected to fit into one of five categories: high (both with respect to other students and with respect to themselves) in analytical abilities only, similarly high in creative abilities only, similarly high in practical abilities only, relatively high in all three abilities, or relatively low in all three abilities. 23. Students who enrolled were then assigned at random to one of four types of course, identified by the type of instruction and skills emphasized: one type of section emphasized memory skills (the control condition), requiring students to recall and recognize; a second type emphasized analytical-thinking skills, requiring students to analyze, judge, compare and contrast, and evaluate; a third type emphasized creative-thinking skills, requiring students to generate, invent, create, imagine, and suppose; and a fourth type emphasized practical thinking skills, requiring students to use, implement, and apply concepts in their everyday lives. These sections, with their varied kinds of instruction, met in the afternoon and represented one component of the course. The basic instructional content, the text and the morning lectures, were the same for all four types of course. 24. Ideally, a single course would emphasize all four types of skills, so that students could learn both to capitalize on their strengths and to compensate for and correct their weaknesses. But part of our 5 purpose in the study was explicitly to look at the effects of matching versus mismatching of abilities to instruction, and thus the instructional conditions were somewhat "purified" for purposes of the experiment. 205 25. All students were evaluated for memory as well as for analytical, creative, and practical achievements. What did we find? 210 215 220 225 230 235 240 26. First, with regard to our ability tests, the overall correlations between sections (analytical, creative, and practical) were low although statistically significant: 0.23 between the analytical and creative tests, 0.14 between the analytical and practical tests, and 0.15 between the creative and practical tests. Moreover, a factor analysis of the subtests revealed no general factor across all the subtests, but rather specific factors for these various subtests. In other words, the results tended to support the notion that the general factor typically obtained in conventional intelligence tests reflects, in part, the narrow scope of the tests. As the experience of many teachers suggests, students can be strong in some skills but weak in others. 27. Second, regardless of the measure of achievement (homework assignments, multiple-choice and essay examinations, independent projects) we used, at least two and in one case three of the kinds of abilities (analytical, creative, practical) significantly and substantially contributed to the prediction of course performance. At least one of these abilities was always analytical ability, as perhaps befits the traditional emphasis of our instruction. But the key point was that prediction of academic achievement was improved by adding the other abilities into the prediction equation. 28. Third, and most important, a number of different data analyses showed that students performed better when the kind of instruction they received was matched rather than mismatched to their pattern of abilities. In other words, students achieved at higher levels when they were taught in a way that recognized and encouraged their particular pattern of skills. If we teach and assess in ways that benefit primarily analytical students, we may indeed end up recognizing only these students as "smart." But if we teach and assess more broadly, we may be surprised to discover that many students are considerably more intelligent than we might have expected. 29. 245 250 The failure to recognize these students' abilities may have quite serious implications for their careers. We have created a system of tests that values certain kinds of abilities, but not others. It is scarcely surprising that our society has formed what intelligence researchers Richard Herrnstein and Charles Murray and others refer to as a "cognitive elite': in order to gain access to competitive colleges, as well as to competitive graduate programs, one has to test well. Students with lower test scores are often, and in many institutions, routinely, denied entrance to the access routes that would allow them to become distinguished doctors, lawyers, academics, executives, and so forth. The so-called cognitive elite is no fact of nature: it is something we have created, much as other societies (and our own in the past) have created elites based on 255 6 the social class of one's birth. Moreover, it is based on a very limited kind of cognitive ability. 260 30. But would the students denied the access routes to desirable occupations and economic success actually succeed in the occupations that are made so hard for them to enter? At one level, we cannot answer this question, because we never do find out what might have been. But at another level it seems self-evident that abilities other than analytical ones are key in the occupations that our society values. 265 270 275 280 285 290 295 300 305 31. Consider, for example, the sciences. To get high grades at the high school and often college level, students need primarily to memorize textbooks and lectures, and, sometimes, to solve problem sets at the backs of chapters and on tests. But how similar are those tasks to the tasks confronted by scientists? Hardly similar at all. As Harriet Zuckerman has pointed out, eminent scientists are those who have good taste in the problems they study - ones who ask good questions. They are the ones who design broad, elegant, and usually empirically testable theories, or who design powerful experiments to test such theories. Excellent novelists as well as literary scholars also need to be creative, whether in the writing of novels, in the generation of literary theories, or in coming up with novel interpretations of authors' ideas. Indeed, outstanding experts in any field need to combine creative with analytical abilities. 32. Scholars as well as others also need the practical abilities to be able to communicate their work effectively and to be able to persuade people that their work is worthy of attention. Academics often tend to dismiss practical abilities at the same time that they know that such abilities are key to getting articles accepted by journals, grant proposals funded by government agencies, students to pay attention in classes, and the like. 33. In a series of collaborations with Richard Wagner, Wendy Williams, Joseph Horvath, and George Forsythe, I have found that practical abilities among adults show virtually no correlation with IQ-like, analytical abilities across a variety of domains. Although scores on tests of practical intelligence such as this one do not correlate with conventional ability measures, they do predict various criteria of job success. This prediction is over and above that obtained from IQ tests. 34. The prediction is not only for academics. In a study of business executives conducted at the Center for Creative Leadership, Wagner and I found that the best predictor of performance on two managerial simulations was our own test of practical intelligence, followed by a conventional intelligence test, and then various personality measures. In a study of sales people, our test, but not a conventional intelligence test, predicted various measures of sales performance. 35. Work suggesting a separation of the more "academic" aspects of intelligence, as measured by conventional intelligence tests, and more practical aspects of intelligence is not limited to my own group at Yale. It is widespread and growing. For example, Steve 7 Ceci at Cornell has found that men who consistently picked winners at a racetrack were generally of average IQ and that there was no correlation between their IQ and their ability to pick winners. Jean Lave at Berkeley found that housewives who could easily distinguish which of two products was a better buy had great difficulty on a paper-and-pencil test of mathematical operations. And Terezhina Nunes, now at the Institute of Education in London, found that Brazilian street children who were failing math in school could nevertheless do the mathematics to keep a successful street business thriving. 310 315 36. 320 325 330 335 340 The message of these and similar studies is not that conventional views of intelligence are wrong but rather that they are highly incomplete. They deal only with a sliver of what revolutionary scientists now believe intelligence to comprise. Thus, when the traditionalists discuss what we know about intelligence, they are really discussing, from the revolutionary point of view, only a narrow part of intelligence. They are answering questions, perhaps, about IQ more than about intelligence, broadly conceived. 37. There is a revolutionary war going on, as there often tends to be in fields where the excitement is the greatest. The war has not yet been won by either side. Moreover, it is not, strictly speaking, a war with only two sides. The revolutionaries have divisions among themselves. For example, I do not accept all of the assertions of Gardner's theory of multiple intelligences, nor does Gardner accept all of the assertions of my triarchic theory. But we and other revolutionaries are united by our belief that IQ represents too narrow a conception of intelligence. We are also united in the view that intelligence can be modified and have done research supporting this view. 38. The traditionalists are holding strong in what they see as a solid fort that has withstood vigorous attacks. The revolutionaries, in contrast, see gaping and ever-expanding holes on all sides of this fort. As always, one set of data can lend itself to varied interpretations. Ultimately, perhaps, all parties to the battle will lose, as even newer and more appealing theories and tests of these theories come to the fore. But at the end of the day, that's what intellectual battles are about - the never-ending search for understanding, whether of intelligence or anything else. 3340 words 345 8 Bar Ilan University A. Ben-Hillel What Should We Ask about Intelligence? Pre-reading 350 355 EFL Department August 2009 You are on the admissions board of Yale University in the Department of Psychology and Education. There is only one place open in the graduate program. You and your colleagues are trying to decide which of the following four students should get that place. Whom do you choose? Alice has been accepted to every program at every university she has applied to. As an undergraduate, she had a straight-A average (in Israel, the equivalent to an average of 90-100), and near-perfect test scores. 360 What are the advantages of choosing Alice? What could be some of the disadvantages? 365 370 Barbara had good, but not outstanding, grades as an undergraduate (approximately an 80-85 average). She has superb letters of recommendation from well-known scholars in the field and has already published papers in professional journals. Her test scores, however, are modest. What are the advantages of choosing Barbara? 375 What could be some of the disadvantages? 380 Paul is both imaginative like Barbara and has strong analytical abilities like Alice. In other words, he offers the best of both worlds. However, he is arrogant and difficult to be around. What are the advantages of choosing Paul? 385 What could be some of the disadvantages? 390 Sam has neither Barbara’s creativity nor the analytical intelligence of Alice. However, Sam has excellent practical intelligence which enables him to produce work that is needed and valued in his field. What are the advantages of choosing Sam? 395 What could be some of the disadvantages? 400 Bar Ilan University A. Ben-Hillel EFL Department August 2009 9 405 I. VOCABULARY: Read the introduction and fill in the following sentences with the words below, according to context. The paragraphs in which the words appear are written in parentheses next to each word. Predictor (2) / generate (3) / notable (4) / reapply (5) arrogance (9) / conventional (10) / notion (10) / commodity (7) / 410 1. He is very ___________________, and not very original, in his thinking. He needs to broaden his horizons. 2. She was an excellent scientist, because she was able not only to memorize ideas, 415 but also to ________________ them. 3. Humility, and not _______________________, will earn you respect. 4. The revolutionaries' ___________________ of intelligence is broader than that of 420 the traditionalists. 5. Her expertise was a real __________________: it helped everyone in the group. 6. Exam scores are only one ___________________ of success. Other 425 ___________________ factors include creativity and the ability to cooperate with people. II. GLOBAL READING 430 1) Based on the introductory section (paragraphs 1-10) write down the author’s two main points: 435 Point 1: ______________________________________________________________ Point 2: ______________________________________________________________ 440 2) By skimming, give a title to paragraphs 11-15. _______________________________ 3) By skimming, give a title to paragraphs 1621._______________________________ 445 4) Skim paragraphs 22-36. What kind of information is given in these paragraphs? 450 ___________________________________ (one word) Bar Ilan University A. Ben-Hillel EFL Department August 2009 10 II. CLOSE READING Answer the following questions based on each section in the text. 455 Paragraphs 11-15: 5) In regard to intelligence testing, is the author: 460 a traditionalist or a revolutionary? (Circle the correct answer.) Paragraphs 16-21: 465 6) Which of the following are criticisms that the revolutionaries make of the traditionalists? (Mark as many statements as appropriate.) ___ The current methods for intelligence testing are unsound. 470 ___ There needs to be a stricter definition of I.Q. ___The tasks given on conventional tests are unable to measure intelligence. ___The notion of g (general factor) is biased against people of other races. 475 ___The notion of g (general factor) is too restrictive to define intelligence. ___The traditionalists do not define intelligence broadly enough. 480 7) What does the author’s theory of intelligence claim? Try to state in your own words. 485 ___________________________________________________________________ ___ ___________________________________________________________________ ___ 490 Paragraphs 37-53: 8) In this section, the author ________________ the results of his research (one word). 495 9) What does the writer mean by “casting a broader net” (l. 367)? ___________________________________________________________________ ___ 500 505 ___________________________________________________________________ ___ 10) What does the writer mean by “cognitive elite” (l. 401) and how does it hurt people’s chances to build their careers? _____________________________________________ 11 ___________________________________________________________________ __ 510 Bar Ilan University A. Ben-Hillel 515 EFL Department August 2009 11) Does the writer believe that students who score low on tests are able to do well in their careers? CIRCLE: YES NO Quote a sentence from the text which supports this view. Para. Number: ____ 520 ___________________________________________________________________ ___ ___________________________________________________________________ ___ 525 12) In paragraph 41, the examples of scientists and novelists support the author’s point that: 530 ___________________________________________________________________ ___ ___________________________________________________________________ ___ 535 ***Extra credit (up to 3 points added to the next quiz grade): In a paragraph of 68 sentences, describe IN YOUR OWN WORDS the results of the second study that the author conducted. (Both studies are described in paragraphs 22-36.) 540