Literature review for S-TEAM WP6.9 Project: Improving teachers’ capacities to motivate students Lars Brian Krogh & Hanne Møller Andersen Centre for Science Education Aarhus University Denmark Contents IMPROVING TEACHERS’ CAPACITIES TO MOTIVATE STUDENTS ............................................................... 0 LARS BRIAN KROGH & HANNE MØLLER ANDERSEN ...................................................................................... 0 CENTRE FOR SCIENCE EDUCATION ...................................................................................................................... 0 AARHUS UNIVERSITY ................................................................................................................................................. 0 DENMARK ....................................................................................................................................................................... 0 CONTENTS ...................................................................................................................................................................... 1 FRAMING THE LITERATURE REVIEW................................................................................................................... 2 PROJECT BACKGROUND ........................................................................................................................................... 2 DANISH STUDENTS’ MOTIVATION FOR SCHOOL SCIENCE ................................................................................................. 2 DANISH TEACHERS’ ORIENTATIONS TOWARDS TEACHING AND MOTIVATIONAL PRACTICES ............................................ 5 DANISH INITIATIVES TO IMPROVE IBSE AND TEACHERS’ MOTIVATIONAL PRACTICES ..................................................... 7 LEARNING TO MOTIVATE – ENGAGING TEACHERS WITH MOTIVATIONAL THEORY ........................ 9 MOTIVATION IN SCIENCE CLASSROOMS - A MULTIDIMENSIONAL CONSTRUCT ............................... 10 MOTIVATIONAL THEORIES AND CONSTRUCTS OF IMPORTANCE TO OUR S-TEAM-PROJECT. ..... 11 SELF-EFFICACY ............................................................................................................................................................. 11 SELF DETERMINATION THEORY (SDT) ......................................................................................................................... 12 ATTRIBUTIONAL THEORY .............................................................................................................................................. 12 GOAL ORIENTATION (AND SELF-REGULATION) ............................................................................................................. 12 EXPECTANCY-VALUE .................................................................................................................................................... 13 EMPHASES FOR IMPLEMENTATION OF SELECTED MOTIVATIONAL THEORIES IN SCIENCE EDUCATION .................................................................................................................................................................. 13 SELF-EFFICACY ............................................................................................................................................................. 13 GOAL ORIENTATION (AND SELF-REGULATION) ............................................................................................................. 15 SELF DETERMINATION THEORY .................................................................................................................................... 16 ATTRIBUTIONAL THEORY .............................................................................................................................................. 19 INSTRUMENTS USED TO ASSESS MOTIVATIONAL CONSTRUCTS .............................................................. 19 Self-efficacy instruments: ......................................................................................................................................... 19 Self-Determination Theory Instruments: .................................................................................................................. 20 Goal-orientation instruments: .................................................................................................................................. 20 Students’ attributions instrument: ............................................................................................................................ 20 1 Framing the literature review The aim of this project is to improve teachers’ capacities to motivate students. To reach this goal we will create an in-service teacher training program and a training package introducing teachers to key elements of contemporary motivational theory, and we will help them transform this theoretical knowledge into practical knowledge applicable to their daily teaching to the benefit of students’ motivation. The selection of motivational theories is in accordance with IBSE (cf. description in (Rocard et al. 2007)) and IBSE is seen as a central aspect of improving students’ motivation in science teaching. So, our literature review will be focused on the following components: students’ motivation, teachers’ use of strategies to improve students’ motivation and teachers’ appropriation of theoretical knowledge and implementing theories into their own practice. The project targets upper secondary science teachers, and consequently the literature review focuses on students’ motivation and initiatives taken in Danish upper secondary school. Project Background Danish students’ motivation for school science A number of research studies have documented, that Danish students enter upper secondary school with moderate motivation for school sciences, particularly low in physics (Krogh and Thomsen 2000b; Krogh, Arnborg, and Thomsen 2001; Andersen and Nielsen 2003; The Danish Evaluation Institute 2001). The authors have been involved in the only two longitudinal Danish studies (Krogh, Arnborg, and Thomsen 2001) of upper secondary students’ attitudes towards science subjects (physics, chemistry, biology). A number of findings are of relevance to the present project: Students’ engagement or interest in school science(s) is not ultimately formed by the age of 14! Recent reports (e.g. Osborne and Dillon 2008, p.14) claim that young peoples’ interest is largely formed by the age of 14, but our Danish studies of physics (Krogh and Thomsen 2000b) and chemistry/biology (Andersen and Nielsen 2003; Andersen 2007) revealed that students going from lower secondary school (“Folkeskolen”) to upper secondary school (“Gymnasium”) had very dynamic changes in their engagement in the different school sciences. Both studies indicated that only 1/3 of the students did not change their attitudes towards science (on a 4 point Likert scale)! So a new school setting, teachers with a different educational background, and new conceptualizations of subjects seem to influence students’ engagement and interest in science. The longitudinal studies also demonstrated that student engagement developed very differently in the three subjects: During the two first years of upper secondary school students’ attitudes towards physics had a highly significant decline (15% on average scores), while students on the average maintained quite a stable engagement with chemistry, and significantly improved their engagement with biology (approximately 10%). Again, our results add complexity to the trend emphasized by major reviews of students’ attitudes towards science (Gardner 1975; Schibeci 1984; Osborne, Simon, and Collins 2003): “A clear feature of the research is the decline in attitudes towards science from age 11 onwards that is documented by a number of studies…. These all show how children’s interest and attitude to 2 science declines from the point of entry to secondary school (Osborne, Simon, and Collins 2003 p.1060). We would like to stress that students’ attitudes can actually change dramatically during upper secondary school, and that some subjects, curricula and teaching strategies seem to be more suited to facilitate and sustain students’ motivation than others. Not necessarily because they represent “better teaching” in a normative sense, but maybe they better fit adolescents’ priorities and values. Teaching strategies and style influence students’ engagement with science. When it comes to the relationship between teaching strategies and students’ engagement only a few studies exist, and the authors of this article have been involved in most of them. Through a series of studies using different methodologies we have investigated how various aspects of the school science subculture, teaching/learning strategies, and interaction in the classroom have influenced students’ motivation. The main studies have been: 1. A large scale quantitative study of physics (N=2243, Krogh and Thomsen 2000b) investigating the use of Subject Centered, and Constructivist/Student Centered “teaching styles” in Danish upper secondary school. The differentiation was based on two sets of 11 indicators. Students’ and teachers’ responses were used to classify the orientation of 100 physics classrooms either as Subject Centered (S) or Constructivist (C). Students’ attitudes were measured, along with their performances on a conceptual test related to the curriculum. Classrooms highest on both dimensions (High S-High C) came out as the most engaging, and Low S-High C classrooms came out to be the second most engaging. The analysis lead to the following conclusion: “Students’ attitudes towards science seem to be more positive, when constructivist elements are introduced.” (Krogh and Thomsen 2000a). 2. In a follow-up study of students’ attitudes towards physics were modeled with Generalized Linear Models (Krogh and Thomsen 2005). Seventeen constructs were entered as explanatory variables and 3 of those accounted for more than 51% of the variation in attitudes: Physics Self-Concept, a value-oriented cultural border crossing factor Reputation (science as strange, different, boring), and Personal Teacher Interest (in students). 3. A third study (Andersen 2007) has investigated how students’ motivation and learning develop in problem-based and student centered learning environments, where students are working with authentic problems of their own choice, but derived from the teacher’s driving question originating from the field of chemistry and biology. Typically the problems involved students’ practical work with student autonomy to select, define, and design experimental elements. Teachers took on the roles of supervisor, scaffolding students’ activities whenever they needed it. The study clearly demonstrated how problem based teaching improved students’ engagement in biology among students in a technical gymnasium. 4. Fourth, a study of value-orientations among students in upper secondary urban schools (Krogh 2006) revealed 5 high-priority orientations: Relatedness (2 types), Autonomy, KnowledgePerformance, and Potentiation. According to contemporary conceptions of values they guide students’ attitudes and actions, in other words they are latent structures behind students’ goals, 3 intentions and behavior. The research demonstrated highly significant relations between students’ value-orientation and their preferences related to their physics learning environment, and their ideas of future jobs. 5. Finally J. Dolin et al (Dolin et al. 2001) have developed a Danish project “Authentic School Physics” (Autentisk Fysik) emphasizing students’ practical work with problems of an open-ended nature. Students should “physicalize” authentic problems for possible investigation, and they should design and carry out an investigation, and then, based on their data they should formulate empiricalmathematical laws. Despite the project’s emphasis on more epistemological aspects of the scientific enterprise, it was concluded that the two year project had sustained students’ interest better than traditional practices in control classes. Based on these Danish studies we will conclude that: Certain kinds of IBSE-oriented pedagogy or “orientations towards teaching” tend to improve students’ motivation in school science. The conception of Constructivist teaching style (study 1), the description of problem-based and student centered learning environment (study 3) and the “Authentic School Physics” - approach coincide with the definition of Inquiry-Based Science Education (IBSE) offered in Rocard et al (2007): “In contrast the second [method] has long been referred to as the “Inductive Approach”. This approach gives more space to observation, experimentation and the teacher-guided construction by the child of his/her own knowledge. This approach is also described as a ‘bottom-up’ approach. The terminology evolved through the years and the concepts refined, and today the Inductive Approach is most often referred to as Inquiry-Based Science Education (IBSE), mostly applied to science of nature and technology. By definition, inquiry is the intentional process of diagnosing problems, critiquing experiments, and distinguishing alternatives, planning investigations, researching conjectures, searching for information, constructing models, debating with peers, and forming coherent arguments. (Linn, Davis, & Bell, 2004).” This relationship between IBSE and the use of Problem-based/constructivist approach in school science is supported by a recent report from the Danish Ministry of Education preparing a National Strategy for Science, Technology and Health Education (Arbejdsgruppen til forberedelse af en national strategi for Natur 2008) that found: ”Teaching strategies applied, particularly in compulsory school, have traditionally not been marked by contemporary views of science as a culture and scientific ways of thinking. Gradually though, a project- and problem-based approach in closer accordance with how science works is gaining ground (“IBSE”= Inquiry Based Science Education). Even though this effort is still progressing in Denmark much can be done to facilitate the individual teacher, the local teams of science teachers, as well as school managers, and district councils” In this way we have established the link between IBSE-oriented teaching and students’ motivation in a Danish context. And we hypothesize that IBSE has a motivational effect due to a number of socialpsychological constructs: Students’ subject-related Self-concepts (self-efficacy/sense of competence) 4 Students’ autonomy (allowing them to pursue personal/authentic learning goals and organize their own learning processes) Students’ sense of relatedness to others in the classroom (peers, teachers). Value-orientations, as motivational constructs (e.g. situated goals based on values). These social-psychological constructs serve as essential mediating devices that explain why IBSE-strategies tend to motivate. If we want to understand why these strategies work we would have to go to motivational theories built around these concepts. And if we want to qualify teachers’ use of IBSE-strategies for motivational purposes teachers must be introduced to the motivational theories emphasizing such concepts. This is the rationale behind our local AU S-TEAM project. Danish Teachers’ orientations towards teaching and motivational practices Internationally there is a growing body of research on “teachers’ orientations towards teaching and learning (Grossman 1990; Magnusson, Krajcik, and Borko 1999; Abell 2007). Orientation towards teaching was defined by Grossman as the “overarching conceptions of teaching a particular subject”. Magnusson et al have explicated a number of orientations related to different teaching goals, and some of these coincide with our notion of IBSE, e.g. Conceptual Change, Discovery, Project-Based Science, Inquiry, and Guided Inquiry. Research has shown that teachers’ orientations towards teaching and learning heavily influence their actual teaching practices, which indicates that any durable change of teachers’ practice must be supported by a paralleled change in their orientation towards teaching and learning. In a Danish context there has been very little research on teachers’ orientation towards teaching, and we have only found a few studies offering fragments of relevant knowledge (Andersen and Krogh 2009; Zeuner et al. 2008) In the context of an in-service training program to accommodate a recent reform in upper secondary school, teachers of different subjects were asked to write short essays about their conceptions of their academic disciplines, the relationship between academic discipline and school subject, and their conceptions of teaching and learning of these subjects (Andersen and Krogh 2009). In analyzing and comparing the responses from science teachers the authors found that: “Most science and math teachers (app. 60%) emphasise the importance of students taking an active part in teaching and learning activities (e.g. problem solving and practical work), but biology teachers differ from physics/mathematics teachers. While many physics/mathematics teachers (app. 30%) conceptualise the optimal way of teaching as a sequence, being initiated by the teacher’s explanation of important subject matter and followed by students working on related problems, biology teachers do not envision this kind of standard procedure. Instead they mention variation as an important element and several biology teachers point to the fact there is no best way of teaching biology; the optimal way depends on the given situation and the students present in the classroom. In contrast to physics/mathematics teachers, biology teachers did not mention teachers lecturing when they described the best way to learn biology. Physics/mathematics teachers were much more focused on students’ capability to do problem solving than biology teachers. This might be explained by a more widespread use of problem solving in the evaluation of students’ abilities in mathematics and physics. A variety of teacher roles were mentioned in teachers’ essays, and typically teachers did not associate optimal teaching with one particular role. About 50% of the teachers in physics, biology, and mathematics mentioned the role of a “sculptor”, 5 who shapes and communicates subject matter knowledge to the students; 25% of the physics and biology teachers pointed to the importance of being an “inspirator”; while no teachers of mathematics saw this as a part of their teacher role. In addition, teachers in all three subjects mentioned “coach” or “supervisor” when they described the optimal teacher role - approx. 40% of physics and mathematics teachers and less biology teachers. Roughly 20% of the teachers did not identify any role as the optimal teacher role, instead the emphasized flexibility and the fact that the optimal teacher role depended on the situation and circumstances.” This investigation paints a quite complex picture of orientations held by Danish science teachers, and there are clear subject-specific flavours. It also indicates that some teachers have an orientation towards IBSE and other bottom-up activities, where students are undertaking enquiry, trying to find the answers to their own questions, while other teachers want students to follow standardised patterns to come up with answers to problems formulated by the teacher. It is questionable if the traditional sequencing of physics/math lessons coincides with IBSE-standards. Even though students are active during problem solving, the teaching just represents a traditional, deductive approach, which is confirmed by these teachers’ frequent use of the “teacher-as-a-sculptor” metaphor. Student activity does not automatically lead to IBSE. Biology teachers seems to be more oriented towards genuine student-centred approaches than physics teachers, a result which is substantiated by a pilot-study including 77 pre-service teachers (28 physics teachers, 18 biology) reported in (Krogh 2006). In a survey designed to explore differences in the Ethos of school physics and biology, pre-service teachers were asked to rank various statements indicating their priorities related to the teaching of their subject. Significant differences were found between teachers of physics and biology on the following three dimensions: Life-World Relation, Organization, and Motivation. Biology teachers emphasized aspects of biology that represented authentic parts of the student’s life-world; while physics teachers were oriented towards presenting the strangeness of physics. Furthermore, biology teachers were more aware of the importance of giving room to social interaction and learning than their physics colleagues. Finally, biology teachers applied motivational thinking where variation of teaching was an important parameter, while physics teachers were more focused on cognitive challenge as a main driving force offered to students. All the available studies of physics in upper secondary school (Krogh and Thomsen 2000a; Danmarks Evalueringsinstitut 2001; Krogh 2006) support the picture of physics teachers as being rather distant from IBSE-oriented practices. The study from The Danish Evaluation Institute used a rich empirical base from students and teachers to arrive at the following conclusion: “Many students say that there is far too much teacher-led whole-class instruction, and many teachers share the view that this approach is the most efficient when it comes to communicating content clearly and within short time” (p. 44). The evaluation committee concludes that most physics teaching is conducted in a traditionalized manner that is inconsistent with contemporary pedagogy, and the teaching does not facilitate students’ autonomous learning (p.44). The earlier mentioned investigation of teaching style in physics classrooms (Krogh and Thomsen 2000b), showed that only one (!) of the hundred classrooms could be characterized as constructivist/studentcentered. In other words, constructivist/student-centered orientations were definitely not implemented in Danish physics classrooms when the investigation was carried out in 1999. In a later study (Krogh 2006) 6 including an empirical analysis of The Ethos of School Science1, a rich mix of empirical evidence was used to establish the Ethos of School Physics as a pattern along eight dimensions, including the Life-world Relation, Organization, and Motivation dimensions. The investigation corroborates the characteristics of physics as remote from students life-worlds, organized in individualizing manners, having a cognitive oriented understanding of students’ motivation. Other highly significant and IBSE-related characteristics of school physics were its Epistemological Closure, and its Communicative One-sidedness, indicating that students are generally not engaged in finding answers to their own questions. The communication is dominated by transmission and teacher directed talk, and the communication is mainly based on scientific terms, representations, and genres. All significant characteristics of the empirical Ethos of School Physics imply that physics excludes students and their experiences, in other words the opposite of the IBSE-intentions. As a part of the evaluation of a major reform of Danish upper secondary school2 Beck & Zeuner have investigated changes in teachers’ teaching practice (Zeuner et al. 2008). The survey was based on reports from 2500 teachers across the curriculum. The participants were classified into 6 categories based on their valuing of two bipolar dimensions - individual vs. social learning and students’ production of knowledge vs. students’ reproduction of knowledge. Beck & Zeuner classified teachers into categories related to “didactic paradigms”, but their work is impaired by somewhat arbitrary numerical criteria used to discriminate between categories. However, it seems fair to say that most teachers (including science) appear in an undifferentiated middle-ground where productive/reproductive and social/individual aspects of learning are balanced, and where they are flanked only by minor groups of teachers who could be characterized as clear-cut constructivist and objectivist, respectively. The data presented indicate that there are approximately 50% more objectivist than constructivists among physics teachers, and twice as many constructivists as objectivists among biology teachers. This confirms the traditional difference between the two school subjects, but it also paints a less rigid picture of physics teachers than previous studies. However, Beck & Zeuner’s research is based on teachers’ self-reports, while the previous studies also included students’ perceptions of what was going on in the classroom. There is a need to triangulate the recent findings of Beck & Zeuner with data from students, in order to validate the apparent reform-induced development towards more IBSE oriented practices in science teaching. Danish initiatives to improve IBSE and teachers’ motivational practices Danish educational policy has long been concerned about students’ lack of interest in science and motivation for pursuing S&T careers. A Committee set up by the Danish Ministry of Education (Andersen et al. 2003) recommended that sustained students’ interest in science should be used as a success-criteria for the Science-for-all-effort they proposed (p. 18). A subsequent Committee provided a national strategy for Science, Technology & Health education (Arbejdsgruppen til forberedelse af en national strategi for Natur 2008). The strategy included an aim to enhance students’ interest in STH, and an aim to ensure better quality and relevance of STH-teaching at all levels of the educational system. This report addressed 1 With the The Ethos of School Science Analysis, TESSA-framework In 2005 there was a big reform of Danish upper secondary school, as part of this reform all learning objectives were stated in terms of competences instead of compulsory content specification. New interdisciplinary subjects were introduced, intended to reveal to students the characteristic features of the scientific enterprise, and to let students recognize the relative strengths and limitations of knowledge produced within scientific, humanist and social science epistemologies, respectively. These new subjects institutionalized cooperation, between teachers of different science subjects and between teachers of science and teachers from other faculties. These radical changes in curricula and organizational structures would be expected to increase transfer of teacher knowledge and teaching strategies, and induce changes in orientations towards teaching and teaching practices. 2 7 students’ interest in science both from a “recruitment” perspective and a science-for-all-perspective. The Danish report was clearly inspired by a concurrent international report (Osborne and Dillon 2008), but unlike the original the Danish report assumes that the two legitimate concerns can be reconciled. A particular recommendation is to improve relevance of content and quality of pedagogy as a means to stimulate students’ learning and interest (p.19). Of special importance to the present review is that the Danish report refers to IBSE as “a tool to get science into a dialogue with more adolescents, in particular with more girls” (p. 8). Furthermore, the report sees teachers’ subject matter knowledge and PCK as ultimate prerequisites to enhance students’ learning and interest in science (p.18). Therefore, teachers’ PCK and development of science teachers’ traditional practices are important target areas in the national strategy outlined. However, rhetoric is one thing, and practical educational policy is another. The Ministry of Education has launched very few initiatives to address the issue of students’ interest and motivation for science. In 1999 the Ministry of Education launched a major Development Program for upper secondary school but no particular emphasis was given to the promotion of students’ interest and motivation in science. More recently, the government has decided to establish a number of regional resource-centers for science education being proposed by a strategic committee. Little is known about the actual mission-statements of these resource-centers and even less is known about the strategies that they might apply to engage science teachers, and if/how they might help teachers motivate students. A reasonable guess is that they will not target this issue explicitly, but simply claim that improving quality of teaching in general terms will automatically improve students’ affective outcomes. During the last years there have been a number of initiatives aiming at the improvement of young peoples’ interest in science, but symptomatically most of these initiatives are directed towards development of teachers’ practice and external agents, i.e. private foundations or EU, have funded them. DASG (Danish Science Gymnasia) was started in spring 2006 and has been funded for a 5 year period by the Lundbeck-Foundation. DASG is a network of gymnasia with a high commitment to develop teaching in mathematics and science. Two of three aims of DASG are to promote students’ interest in math and science, and to motivate students to pursue S&T-careers. These aims are only implicitly addressed in the local projects taking place at individual schools, but many of these projects are oriented towards IBSE. The 2007 themes were: data-capture, use of satelliteobservations-of-earth and nano-technology in science teaching, and the use of CAS and ICT in math teaching. The underlying motivational thinking seems to be that modernizing teaching by introducing front-line technology will draw students’ interest and attention to science. But research indicates that this rationale might be counter-productive when it comes to the recruitment of girls to science. DASG has typically engaged 200-250 teachers a year in thematic development projects, the projects have mainly been driven by participant teachers supported only by a few research informed courses. A number of evaluation reports are available from the webpage of the evaluator (IND, Copenhagen University, http://www.ind.ku.dk/udvikling/projekter/dasg2008/ ). These evaluations indicate that the teachers are quite satisfied with their participation, but the project’s impact on teachers’ 8 practices is not documented. Typical products are teaching sequences developed by individuals or pairs of teachers and materials from pilot-projects which are disseminated only through the project homepage. Science Team K was another initiative funded by the Lundbeck-Foundation from 2003-2007. The aims were to improve science teaching and promote the interest in S&T among students from lower and upper secondary school. Its strategy was to achieve these aims by investigating and facilitating the formation of “Local School Science Cultures” (LSSC’s) with 18 schools in a rural area in Denmark. More than 100 teachers and school administrators were involved in the project. Again in this project the aims of improving teaching and students’ interest in science were only implicitly addressed. External funding was mainly used to buy new lab-equipment and participation in events like the “Danish Festival of Science”, an annual event to promote and celebrate science. The initiative documented (Sølberg 2007) a number of incitements and barriers to the establishment of LSSC’s. Actual changes in teaching practices were not documented, but pre- and post-tests of students’ interest in science using ROSE-items showed that girls’ interest in science improved, but boys’ interest in science tended to be unaltered. IFUN (“Interesse und Fachübergreifendes Unterricht in Naturwissenschaften”, 2005-2007) was a collaboration between the University of Southern Denmark (SDU), Leibniz Institut für die Pädagogik der Naturwissenschaften Kiel, University College Syd and some gymnasia in Denmark and in Germany. It was partially funded by the EU’s Intereg III A program. The second phase of the project intended to answer the research question: “How can science teaching be organized to improve students’ interest in science?”. The cornerstone of the project was to involve teachers in developing sequences of interdisciplinary teaching that “to a larger extent than usual teaching in science builds on students’ own interests and the needs of particular occupations (?)”. How these contradictory aims might be achieved cannot be read from the project homepage and it has not been possible to find any research documentation of the activities and/or its impact on students’ interest. Learning to motivate – engaging teachers with motivational theory We have found only a few research studies describing in-service teacher training explicitly aiming at the improvement of teachers’ motivational practices, and only one of these involved science teachers from upper secondary schools (Cherubini, Zambelli, and Boscolo 2002). Cherubini et al designed a professional development course with 36 teachers (various subjects, including science) from elementary, middle and high schools in Italy. The intervention was organised as 10 3-hour course-units at weekly intervals, and the units were structured around three ‘thematic cores’: 1) Sharing and discussion of participants’ school experiences related to students’ motivation and de-motivation. The group discussions and reflections were sustained by an introduction to motivational theories e.g. task-value, self-regulation, students’ attributions, and self-efficacy. 2) Making participants aware of the possibility of systematically collecting data from the classroom. This awareness was combined with a presentation of data-collection instruments and some practical activities and group discussion. 3) Teachers’ analyzing, planning and discussing potential interventions supporting motivating teaching (a starting point for a subsequent action research project with the participating teachers). Analysis of teacher reflections from the three levels of schools revealed highly 9 significant differences between the teachers’ motivational thinking. Elementary and middle school teachers essentially characterized the positive situations as a combination of ‘attractive’ didactic activities, while high school teachers tended to link greater motivation to student characteristics (bright, hardworking) and a good classroom culture and learning environment (serious work habits, reciprocal respect). At the end of the in-service teacher training the teachers showed an increased ability and interest in reflecting with colleagues (and researchers) on their students’ motivational problems, and an increased interest in collaborative planning of educational interventions. Most importantly, the intervention had produced “remarkable changes in most participants’ beliefs regarding the teacher’s role in developing students’ positive orientation to study and instruction” (p. 286). Another study (Stipek et al. 1998) involved 24 mathematics teachers from elementary school in a professional development program aiming at the enhancement of teachers’ classroom practices and students’ motivation. The teachers were divided into three groups (intensive intervention, support, control). The intervention was organized as a 1-week summer workshop, followed by biweekly meetings. Motivation was only one of four components addressed in the intervention. Only 1 of the days in the summer workshop and approx. 10 hrs of the biweekly meetings focused at motivation issues. The motivational issues explicitly addressed were ability/effort-attributions, self-efficacy, goals (masteryperformance) and emotions (shame, fear, anxiety, pride). Teachers’ classroom practices were video-taped, and the recordings were used for a coding of teachers’ and students’ behaviour in the classroom. In addition, students also filled out a questionnaire concerning their motivation. The results showed that teachers in the intervention group systematically scored better than the other groups on all motivational measures (emphasis on effort, mastery orientation, encouragement of student autonomy, providing substantive feedback etc.). However, the differences were only significant when the intervention group was compared with the control group. But one must be aware of the size of the teacher groups as they only consisted of 7-9 teachers, and this low sample-size may erode otherwise significant results. Martin (Martin 2008) investigated the effect of a multidimensional educational intervention to improve selected high-school students’ academic motivation. The intervention consisted of 13 student-modules built around “The Motivation and Engagement Wheel” developed by Martin. This represents a pragmatic selection of elements of self-determination, goal orientation, self-efficacy, self-worth and attributional theory etc. The intervention had a positive effect on students’ motivation, showing an increase in valuing, mastery orientation, persistence, and other targeted parameters. In addition to these intervention studies, there is a rich literature using motivational theories as theoretical lenses for analytical purposes. Often their practical implications are farfetched, and transfer and utility to practice is questionable. However, some studies like the one of Green (Green 2002) are so concrete that they relate directly to specific elements of practice (here studying explicit verbal motivational practices in exemplary teachers’ classrooms with expectancy-value-theory as a lens). This gives useful insights into the ways teachers communicate expectancies and task-value to students. Motivation in science classrooms - a multidimensional construct Through our work with this literature review we have observed a clear difference between the studies performed by motivational psychologists and classroom intervention focusing on teachers’ and students’ 10 motivational practice. Studies carried out by motivational psychologist are often restricted to the exploration of a single motivational theory, being appropriate for work having a theoretical purpose. In contrast, intervention studies involving teachers and students are more practice related, and the complexity of active classrooms calls for a multidimensional approach. In this case the authors’ personal experiences with science teaching in upper secondary schools coincide with the emerging recognition of classroom complexity. In the research community there is a growing realization (see quotations in Martin 2008, p.241) that narrow theoretical understanding has little to offer practical teaching and developmental issues. Instead more effort should be made to arrive at “use-inspired basic research” and more integrative frameworks. In particular, Pintrich (Pintrich 2003) has underscored the importance of considering, conceptualizing, and articulating a model of motivation from salient theorizing related to self-efficacy, attributions, valuing, control, self-determination, goal orientation, need achievement, and self-worth. Ford (Ford 1992) also has attempted to integrate the salient and seminal multiplicity of motivational theories into a single theoretical framework. However, Ford’s framework is of a more general nature and has more to offer to theoretical understanding of motivation than classroom research and development. The intervention studies mentioned above all share a common feature: their multidimensional and pragmatic approach to selecting and combining elements of a number of motivational theories. For the design of our teacher training modules and our teacher training package we will be using a similar approach. Motivational theories and constructs of importance to our S-TEAM-project. Guided by recommendations in the research literature (Pintrich 2003; Martin 2008) (e.g. Pintrich 2003; Martin 2008) and grounded in our experiences as teachers of secondary science education we have decided to build our intervention around elements from a number of motivational theories. The next part of the review briefly describes the selected theories, core components and constructs. More elaborate descriptions can be found in the central references entered, or in some of the general textbooks on motivation in education (Pintrich and Schunk 2002; Ford 1992; Brophy 2004). Self-efficacy Self-efficacy was first introduced as a motivational construct by Bandura, who has build a highly influential research program around it (Bandura 1997). Self-efficacy is a person’s situation-specific belief that he or she can succeed in a specific domain or task. Generally, students will be more inclined to take on a task if they believe they can succeed. Students with high self-efficacy in a task are more likely to make more of an effort, and persist longer, than those with low efficacy. The stronger the self-efficacy or mastery expectations, the more active the efforts. On the other hand, low self-efficacy provides an incentive to learn more about the subject. As a result, someone with a high self-efficacy may not prepare sufficiently for a task. Research shows that the ‘optimum’ level of self-efficacy is a little above ability, which encourages people to tackle challenging tasks and gain valuable experience. Often the key to motivating and engaging struggling learners is to get them to believe that they can succeed (Margolis and McCabe 2006) Self-efficacy is not a static attribute, but is affected by a person’s experiences and is postulated to change according to four sources – emotional arousal (physiological reactions), vicarious learning, enactive mastery and social (verbal) persuasion. 11 A number of studies have documented that Self-efficacy is the best predictor of students’ persistence and achievement (Fencl and Scheel 2005). Bandura has explored the nature of the relation, and found that Selfefficacy has both direct and in-direct influence on e.g. students’ achievement (Bandura 1997). Self Determination Theory (SDT) SDT is another major motivational research program, initiated by Deci and Ryan (Deci et al. 1991; Ryan and Deci 2000). According to self-determination theory, intrinsic motivation arises from three “innate needs”: autonomy, relatedness and competence. The basic needs perspective has been extracted from research undertaken both within and outside of educational settings. Typically the research has revealed how external regulation influences students’ (and others) Self-Determined behavior and tend to undermine their intrinsic motivation. A large number of studies have documented how teachers’ actions and teaching strategies interfere with students’ opportunities to fulfil their basic needs (Reeve 2002; Martin and Dowson 2009; Black and Deci 2000). Attributional theory Attribution theory is a cognitive theory of motivation, assuming that individuals are rational decision makers and have a drive towards understanding and mastering their interactions with the environment. Especially they would like to understand the causal determinants of their own and others behaviors. Perceived causes or ‘attributions’ of actions are crucial as they may incite or prevent future actions. In a series of important studies (see references in Pintrich and Schunk 2002, p.93) B. Weiner has provided both rationale and important explication of the theory for use in educational settings. Here, a number of perceived causes are emphasized, first of all ability, effort, task difficulty, and luck (Weiner 1979). Furthermore the attributions are categorized according to three causal dimensions, indicating whether they are stable (or can be changed), have internal (or external) locus to students, and are controllable (or not) to students. Particularly powerful are students’ perceptions of what causes failure. If these are attributed to stable traits, that a student cannot control it does not make much sense to the student to engage in work to improve performance. In such a case attributions clearly are dysfunctional, from a learning perspective. Functional attributions are optimistic; they tell learners that success is possible and that making an effort and using strategies in a correct way is likely to lead to success. Most importantly, it has been demonstrated that students’ attributions can develop, as a consequence of long-term, focused effort from teachers and others (Margolis & McCabe, 2006). Goal orientation (and Self-regulation) Generally, Goal Orientations are defined as purposes individuals have for engaging in specific behaviors. A number of dichotomies have been established as highly relevant to describe achievement motivation and learning behavior. Ames (Ames 1992) has emphasized goal-orientations as being either Mastery or Performance-oriented, where a mastery-orientation is associated with deep engagement with the task and greater perseverance in the face of setbacks. Performance-orientation means that individuals are oriented towards extrinsic indicators of success and external recognition. Research has consistently found that a performance-orientation leads to de-motivation when ever students’ perform badly. Closely related with Ames’ notions of Mastery/Performance-orientations are the dichotomy of Ego- and task-involvement proposed by Nicholls et al (Jagacinsky and Nicholls 1984). Task-involvement is associated with a genuine engagement with the task for its own sake, while ego-involvement implies that engagement 12 is driven by external factors (e.g. praise) or confirmation of how good one is/self-appraisal. Egoinvolvement is stimulated by evaluative situations in which the emphasis is on competition and comparison. From a learning perspective ego-orientation is problematic because it makes it draws attention and effort away from processing the task towards rewards or self-appraisal processes. The consequence is surface learning and deteriorating motivation as a response to bad performance or evaluation from others. Task involvement is stimulated by situations where greater understanding or acquisition of new skills is considered as an end in itself. Task involved subjects believe more in the efficacy of effort, and they often work harder to obtain their goals. Task involvement can be elicited by telling subjects to “concentrate on the task, try to see it as a challenge, and enjoy mastering it” (Graham 1994). Among other valuable contributions to goal-orientation theories we have found the distinction between approach and avoidance goals (Elliot 1997) important, as it differentiates two distinct versions of performance-orientation. An Approach orientation makes for a positive drive to outperform others, while an Avoidance orientation results in a negative motivation to perform, simply to avoid failure and looking dumb. The Self-regulation theory of Boekarts et al (Boekaerts and Corno 2005) operates with two types of goals that students enter to educational settings: Growth Goals (mastery) and Well-being goals, which the students strive to balance. Compared to traditional goal-orientation theories the category of Well-being goals is an important extension (also present in the Taxonomy of Human Goals (Ford 1992, p.88)). Students’ self-regulation is further facilitated when students has access to well-refined volitional strategies manifested as good work habits. This enhances their capacity to stay on the growth track (i.e. metacognitive knowledge to interpret strategy failure and knowledge of how to buckle down to work). If they do not have any suitable strategies when a stressor blocks learning, they will get of the well-being track (Boekaerts and Corno 2005). Expectancy-value Many motivational theories include some kind of expectancy and value constructs, but the one proposed by Eccles and Wigfield and co-workers is probably the most useful when it comes to academic motivation (Wigfield, Eccles, and Rodriguez 1998; Eccles and Wigfield 2002). The authors have built a very complex model of achievement behaviour, but the two most important factors are expectancy and task value. Expectancy is perceived probability of success (very closely related to efficacy beliefs), and task value has been found to have several flavours (attainment value, intrinsic interest, utility value, and perceived costs). Only when students have both reasonable expectations to succeed and the task is considered of (task-) value motivation will be strong enough to drive actual behavior. Emphases for implementation of selected motivational theories in science education Self-efficacy Collaborative learning has been found to positively contribute to the self-efficacy of students in introductory physics and chemistry courses (Fencl and Scheel 2005). 13 Strategies requiring students to interact socially or to creatively and conceptually work with course content were fund to be the most effective at building students’ physics self-efficacy (Fencl and Scheel 2005). Some of the strategies also influenced Classroom Climate (Question and answers, inquiry labs, and conceptual problem assignments). Learning outcomes improved within the collaborative and inquiry oriented setting, but the relation between student outcomes and self-efficacy measures was not explored in any detail. Apparently, the collaborative learning setup offers opportunities for one or more of the four fundamental sources of Self-efficacy to be functional (see Bandura 1997, chapter 3 serves as a general sourcebook for ideas of interventions). Enactive mastery: Enactive mastery experience is the most influential source of efficacy information. Experiencing success is essential to boost confidence and the willingness to keep trying. Consequently, struggling learners must have assignments and tasks of moderate challenge, where they can succeed with moderate effort, giving them a chance to do well and to interpret their success in ways that strengthen their self-efficacy. Instructional tasks should be designed at a level slightly above the learner’s current performance level – moderately challenging. Teachers should regularly assess present levels of achievement and plan accordingly. However, "the impact of performance attainments on efficacy beliefs depends on what is made of those performances" (p. 81). The extent to which people build their efficacy from enactive mastery experiences depends on Preexisting Self-knowledge Structures, Perceived Task Difficulty, Effort Expenditure, Selective Self-monitoring and Reconstruction of Enactive Experiences (see Bandura, chapt. 3 for more details). All of these aspects hold potential for intervention, and some overlap with constructs from other theories (e.g. Effort Expenditure overlap with elements of attribution theory). Vicarious experience: Struggling learners can benefit from observing friends model a task, where they demonstrate a skill or a learning strategy. The modeller often verbally explicates what he is doing and thinking at each step, thereby making it possible for a struggling learner to develop an internal imagery needed to conceptualize and implement the targeted skills and learning strategies. By observing how coping models overcome mistakes, struggling learners of similar ability often realize they also have a change to achieve. Verbal persuasion and feedback: On some occasions it might be relevant to persuade students to persist with certain learning tasks. However, in most classroom settings it seems more relevant to provide learners with formative and task-oriented feedback, which may convince them that they can manage the task (Margolis and McCabe 2006; Fencl and Scheel 2005). Notice here, that motivational theories tend to overlap: specific actions may benefit students from several theoretical perspectives, and a particular action may have a direct influence on constructs of one motivational theory, and indirectly influence constructs of another. Physiological reaction: As Bandura puts it: “Therefore, the fourth major way of altering efficacy beliefs is to enhance physical status, reduce stress levels and negative emotional proclivities, and correct misinterpretations of bodily states" (p. 106). Struggling learners sometimes have a negative physiological reaction before, during and after engaging in a challenging task. Relaxation training and ways to challenge irrational thoughts might be helpful for some students (Margolis and McCabe 2006). 14 Goal orientation (and Self-regulation) Teachers can organise classroom structure to promote students mastery orientation. Epstein (Epstein 1989) (Epstein 1989) has developed an intervention scheme with the acronym TARGET, which describes how master orientations can be facilitated. The TARGET scheme focuses on six modifiable aspects of classroom structure: types of tasks, lines of authority, means of recognition, grouping methods, evaluation practices and use of time (Ames 1992). The Table from Ames (Ames 1992) relates specific Instructional Strategies to Student Motivation Patterns in the TAR and E-dimensions of TARGET: Structure Instructional Strategies Motivation Patterns Task Focus on the meaningful aspects of learning activities Focus on effort and learning Design tasks for novelty, variety, diversity, and student interest Design tasks that offer reasonable challenge to students Help students establish short-term, selfreferenced goals Support development and use of effective learning strategies Authority Focus on helping students participate in the decision making Provide “real” choices where decisions are based on effort, not ability evaluations High intrinsic interest in activity Attributions to effort-based strategies Use of effective learning and other self-regulatory strategies Active engagement Positive affect on high effort tasks Feelings of belongingness “Failure-tolerance” Give opportunities to develop responsibility and independence Support development and use of selfmanagement and monitoring skills Evaluation/ Recognition Focus on individual improvement, progress, and mastery Make evaluation private, not public Recognize students’ effort 15 Provide opportunities for improvement Encourage view of mistakes as part of learning Classroom investigations have shown that each of these instructional strategies can improve students effort and engagement, but they are mutually dependent and interact in a multiplicative manner (Ames 1992). These findings clearly overlap with recommendations from research on Self-regulated learning, e.g. in the appraisal of tasks and opportunities to learn (Boekaerts and Corno 2005; Boekaerts 2002). Teaching of specific learning strategies to promote students’ self-regulation is described in (Margolis and McCabe 2006). Among the recommendations are: Organising learning situations in such a way that students are encouraged to begin the learning process by generating learning goals from their own goal hierarchy. Designing teaching that allows students to experience situational meaningfulness. Students become concerned with emotional well-being (instead of mastery goals) when environmental cues signal that things are not going well. Here students’ willingness to maintain learning goals and persist toward mastery in the face of difficulty depends on their awareness of and access to volitional strategies. Students’ ability to overcome obstacles improves, when they can call upon such an meta-cognitive understanding of volitional strategies. An instructional focus on meta-cognitive skills and strategies is beneficial. Further, classroom culture can facilitate self-regulation, when teachers provide models, serve as coaches and establish environments conducive to self-regulation (Boekaerts and Corno 2005). The use of learning communities is suggested as an example of a supportive learning environment. Self Determination Theory The application of SDT in education has recently been reviewed by Niemiec and Ryan (Niemiec and Ryan 2009). More specific investigation of how specific classroom strategies can promote students’ selfdetermined behavior (sense of competence, relatedness, and autonomy) can be found in (Reeve 2002). Here Reeve and co-workers have found ways to classify teachers’ orientations/dispositions as being autonomy-supportive or controlling. Next they have identified “what autonomy-supportive teachers do” (see Table NN). Here various teacher instructional behaviours are listed, and it is indicated whether these behaviours are more frequent among autonomy- supportive teachers (AS>C) or among controlling teachers (C>AS). Most importantly we have summarized the outcome-results from the text of Reeeve in an additional outer-right column. This indicates if the instructional behaviour is positively related to students’ sense of self-determination (SDT) and/or competence (Comp). 16 Reeve et al (1999) Deci et al (1982) Students’ perception Teacher’s instructional behaviors Time spent talking AS=C C>AS Time spent listening AS>C - SDT+Comp Time spent holding instructional material C>AS - -SDT Time given to students for independent work AS>C AS>C SDT+Comp Solutions given C>AS C>AS Teacher’s Conversational Statements Directives/Commands C>AS C>AS Should, Must, Have-to statements AS=C C>AS Deadlines Statements - AS=C Praises of Quality of Performance AS>C AS>C Praises of Student AS=C C>AS Encouragements AS=C - Criticisms - C>AS Hints given AS=C AS=C Comp Solutions/answers given C>AS C>AS -Comp Questions of what student want AS>C AS=C Controlling questions AS=C C>AS Responses to student-generated questions AS>C - Self-disclousure statements AS=C AS=C Empathic, Perspective-taking Statements AS>C - Give students opportunity to talk Comp Comp Comp 17 A number of SDT-recommended instructional behaviours can be derived from the Table: Compared to controlling teachers autonomy supportive teachers spent more time listening to the students and they give students’ more time for independent work, besides they more often respond to student-generated ideas and they praise quality performance, in addition they are more empathic and able to take students’ perspective than controlling teachers. Teachers can also increase students’ sense of autonomy by exposing them to interesting and worthwhile tasks fostering an internal locus of control, which can promote a task rather than an ego involvement and cultivate a sense of volition. Students’ perception of choice can be facilitated by a flexible interpersonal environments and opportunities to choose how, when and with whom they want to work with the task. The subjective perceived locus of control, high volition, and perceived choice and highly intertwined. Summing it up, an autonomy-supportive context will facilitate development of students’ self-regulation (identified regulation) by the following means: (1) providing the students with a meaningful rationale as to why the task or lesson is important (2) establishing an interpersonal relationship that emphasizes choices and flexibility rather than control and pressure; and (3) acknowledging and accepting the negative feelings associated with engaging in arduous activities (Reeve 2002). At the personal level it requires certain personal skills to be an autonomy supportive teacher such as an ability to: take the perspective of the student, acknowledge students’ feelings, provide rational for requests and uninteresting lessons, and an ability to communicate in a non-controlling language (Reeve 2002). Autonomy support requires a willingness to enter into relationships from the students’ perspective to encourage initiative, nurture competence, and communicate in ways that are non-controlling and information-rich (Reeve 2002, p.190). The relatedness-dimension of SDT (and a selection of other motivational theories) is discussed by (Martin and Dowson 2009) who summarize the following points from research to teachers’ interactivity practice (p. 344): a) “Students’ sense of support (e.g., being liked, respected, and valued by the teacher) predicts their expectancies for success and valuing of subject matter. Indeed, support from teacher is a consistently influential factor in motivation and achievement (Goodenow, 1993a). b) Students who believe that their teacher is caring also believe they learn more (Teven & McCroskey, 1997). c) Students’ feelings of acceptance by teachers are associated with emotional, cognitive, and behavioral engagement in class (Connell & Wellborn, 1991). d) Teachers who support a student’s autonomy tend to facilitate greater motivation, curiosity, and desire for challenge (Flink, Boggiano, & Barrett, 1990). e) Teachers higher in warmth tend to develop greater confidence in students (Ryan & Grolnick, 1986). Conversely, research also supports the following conclusions: f) When teachers are more controlling, students tend to show less mastery motivation 18 and lower confidence (Deci, Schwartz, Sheinman, & Ryan, 1981). g) Teachers who are not perceived as warm typically evince lower motivation and achievement among students (Kontos & Wilcox-Herzog, 1997b).” Attributional theory The most important recommendation of attribution theory is that teachers develop attributional awareness and systematically stress that success and failure are due to controllable factors and not to students’ dispositions or traits. Another important aspect is maintaining positive beliefs capabilities, sustaining a belief that performance can change, e.g. as a consequence of access to new strategies (Margolis and McCabe 2006; Pintrich and Schunk 2002). Students’ attribution can be based on “informational cues” such as prior performance history and social norms, or it can be based on “indirect attributional cues” such as teacher communication and feedback. Adequate feedback should be specific, task-oriented, formative and accurate, instead of being overly positive in order to maintain students’ self-esteem (Pintrich and Schunk 2002, p.136): ”Students will not believe teachers’ attributions that are not credible”. Unintentionally teachers may provide attributional cues through their interactivity with students (Graham 1994, p.34), often in subtle and situated ways: “Pity vs. anger” (affective displays by teachers), “the emotional reaction of teachers may be among the most important antecedents of perceived personal competence”. Depending on the situation both affective reactions from teachers may deliver dysfunctional attributional cues. “Praise vs. Blame” or ”Praise and lack of blame”. Generally these ego-oriented cues should be avoided or used with great caution, e.g. with an awareness that lack of blame in a situation where peers are blamed for similar performances will be decoded by a student as an implicit low-ability positioning. Criticism can convey much positive information and praise. “Help vs. neglect”: Help might function as a low ability cue, especially when it is un-solicited. Depending on the student neglect may be decoded as trust that he will manage on his own or as teacher’s abandoning him. Instrumental help vs. gratuitous help. The type of help also can give away cues, e.g. providing tools or hints when appropriate functions very differently from supplying answers outright (the ‘gratuitous’ type). Gratuitous help tend to induce ‘learned helplessness” on the side of the learner. Instruments used to assess motivational constructs Self-efficacy instruments: Pajares has summarized the use of efficacy instruments (Pajares 1996): 19 “Researchers assess self-efficacy beliefs by asking individuals to report the level, generality, and strength of their confidence to accomplish a task or succeed in a certain situation (see Table 1). In academic settings, self-efficacy instruments may ask students to rate their confidence to solve specific mathematics problems (Hackett & Betz, 1989), perform particular reading or writing tasks (Shell, Colvin, & Bruning, 1995), or engage in certain self-regulatory strategies (Bandura, 1989). Assessments of other expectancy beliefs include asking students to report how well they expect to do in an academic subject (i.e., performance expectancies, Meece, Wigfield, & Eccles, 1990), whether they understand what they read (i.e., perceptions of competence, Harter, 1982), and whether they are good in an academic subject (i.e., academic domainspecific self-concept, Marsh, 1992; also ability perceptions, Meece et al., 1990)”. More specifically, Bandura himself has provided examples and guidance in constructing efficacy-scales (Bandura 2006). Of particular relevance to the present research project with teachers Bandura has developed and validated a Teacher Self-efficacy Instrument, and series of research studies have applied the STEBI-A/B (“Science Teaching Efficacy Beliefs Instrument”) to explore teachers efficacy beliefs. The use of STEBI-B was reexamined in a recent publication (Bleicher 2004). An example of efficacy instruments for use with students within the area of college Biology can be found in (Baldwin, Ebert-May, and Burns 1999). Self-Determination Theory Instruments: The Intrinsic Motivation Inventory (IMI) has been used extensively to study aspects of SDT. It can be downloaded from the homepage of University of Rochester http://www.psych.rochester.edu/SDT/measures/IMI_description.php , where its use and its psychometric properties are documented by Ryan and colleagues. From the same homepage specific instruments to measure Perceived Autonomy Support (“The learning climate questionnaire”) also can be downloaded. Goal-orientation instruments: One of the dominant instruments to study students’ goal orientations and their relation to classroom and teacher characteristics is the Patterns of Adaptive Learning Scales by Midgley and others. Its use and psychometric properties is documented in the Manual that can be downloaded from a homepage related to the inventors (Midgley and et al 2000). Students’ attributions instrument: Attributional Style Questionnaire (ASQ) (Peterson 1982) comprises twelve hypothetical situations consisting of six positive and six negative events. Each situation is followed by four questions, to which respondents are firstly required to provide a major cause of the situation. The following three questions evaluate the three fundamental attribution dimensions and hence measure the degree to which the subject’s response is internal or external (i.e., locus), stable or unstable, and global or specific. 20 References 1. Abell, S. Research on Science Teacher Knowledge. In Handbook of Research on Science Education. Edited by S. Abell and N. Lederman. Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers, 2007. 2. Ames, C., "Classrooms - Goals, Structures, and Student Motivation," Journal of Educational Psychology 84, no. 3 (1992): 261-271. 3. Andersen, H. M., "Veje til motivation og læring", Steno Department, Aarhus University, 2007). 4. Andersen, H. M. and L. B. Krogh, "Science and mathematics teachers' core teaching conceptions and their implications for engaging in cross-curricular innovations.," Submitted for publication in NorDiNa April 2009 (2009). 5. Andersen, H. M. and V. G. Nielsen. Kemi i gymnasiet - elevernes opfattelse af faget i 1.g/1.htx. 2003. Center for Naturfagenes Didaktik, Aarhus Universitet (In Danish). CND's skriftserie no 6. 6. Andersen, N. O. et al. Fremtidens Naturfaglige Uddannelser - Bd.1: Strategiplan 2003-2008 og videre frem. 2003. København, Undervisningsministeriet (In Danish). 7. Arbejdsgruppen til forberedelse af en national strategi for Natur, Teknik og Sundhed. Et fælles løft. 2008. Copenhagen, Danish Ministry of Education. 8. Baldwin, J. A., D. Ebert-May, and D. J. Burns, "The development of a college biology self-efficacy instrument for nonmajors," Science Education 83, no. 4 (1999): 397-408. 9. Bandura, A. Efficacy - the exercise of control. New York: W. H. Freeman, 1997. 10. Bandura, A. Guide for constructing self-efficacy scales. In Adolescence and education, Vol. 5: Selfefficacy beliefs of adolescents. Edited by F. Pajares and T. Urdan. Greenwich, CT: Information Age, 2006. 11. Black, A. and E. Deci, "The Effects of Instructors' Autonomy Support and Students' Autonomous Motivation on Learning Organic Chemistry: A Self-Determination Theory Perspective," Science Education 84 (2000): 740-756. 12. Bleicher, R. E., "Revisiting the STEBI-B: Measuring Self-efficacy in Pre-service Elementary Teachers," School Science and Mathematics 104, no. 8 (2004): 383-391. 13. Boekaerts, M. and L. Corno, "Self-regulation in the classroom: A perspective on assessment and intervention," Applied Psychology-An International Review-Psychologie Appliquee-Revue Internationale 54, no. 2 (2005): 199-231. 21 14. Boekaerts, Monique, "Bringing about change in the classroom: strengths and weaknesses of the self-regulated learning approach - EARLI Presidential Address, 2001," Learning and Instruction 12, no. 4 (2002): 589-604. 15. Brophy, J. Motivating Students to Learn. 2nd ed. Mahwah, NJ.: Lawrence Erlbaum, 2004. 16. Cherubini, G., F. Zambelli, and P. Boscolo, "Student motivation: an experience of inservice education as a context for professional development of teachers" , Teaching and Teacher Education 18, no. 3 (2002): 273-288. 17. Danmarks Evalueringsinstitut. Fysik i skolen - skolen i fysik. Evaluering af fysik i det almene gymnasium. 2001. København, EVA, Danmarks Evalueringsinstitut. 18. Deci, E. L. et al., "Motivation and Education - the Self-Determination Perspective," Educational Psychologist 26, no. 3-4 (1991): 325-346. 19. Dolin, Jens Volmer et al. Autentisk fysik. 2001. København, Roskilde Universitetscenter. 20. Eccles, J. S. and A. Wigfield, "Motivational beliefs, values, and goals," Annual Review of Psychology 53 (2002): 109-132. 21. Elliot, A. J. Integrating the "classic" and "contemporary" approaches to achievement motivation: A hierarchical model of approach and avoidance achievement motivation. In Advances in motivation and achievement, vol.10. Edited by M. Maehr and P. Pintrich. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press, 1997. 22. Epstein, J. Family Structures and Student Motivation: A Developmental Perspective. In Research on Motivation in Education, vol.3. Edited by C. Ames and R. Ames. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, 1989. 23. Fencl, Heidi and Karen Scheel, "Engaging Students. An Examination of the Effects of Teaching Strategies on Self-Efficacy and Course Climate in a Nonmajors Ppysics Course," Journal of College Science Teaching 35, no. 1 (2005): 20-24. 24. Ford, M. E. Motivating Humans - goals, emotions and personal agency beliefs. London: Sage Publishers, 1992. 25. Gardner, P. L., "Attitudes to science: A review," Studies in Science Education 2 (1975): 1-41. 26. Graham, Sandra. Classroom Motivation From an Attributional Perspective. In Motivation. Theory and Research. Edited by Harold F O'Neil and Michael Drillings. Hillsdale: Lawrance Erlbaum Associates Inc., 1994. 27. Green, S., "Using an expectancy-value approach to examine teachers' motivational strategies," Teaching and Teacher Education 18 (2002): 989-1005. 28. Grossman, P. L. The making of a teacher: Teacher knowledge and teacher education. New York: Teachers College Press, 1990. 22 29. Jagacinsky, C. and J. G. Nicholls, "Conceptions of Ability and Related Affects in Task Involvement and Ego Involvement," Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, no. 5 (1984): 909-919. 30. Krogh, L. B., "'Cultural Border Crossings' within the physics classroom – a cultural perspective on youth attitudes towards physics", Steno Department for Studies of Science and Science Education, 2006). 31. Krogh, L. B., P. Arnborg, and P. V. Thomsen. GFIII-rapport: Hvordan gik det så med fysikundervisningen og elevernes udbytte? - 2.g-opfølgning på GFII-undersøgelsen. 3, del A. 2001. CND's skriftserie. 32. Krogh, L. B. and P. V. Thomsen. GFII-rapport nr. 1: Undervisningsstil og læringsudbytte - en undersøgelse af fysikundervisningen i 1.g. 1. 2000a. Det Naturvidenskabelige Fakultet, Aarhus Universitet. CND' skriftserie. 33. Krogh, L. B. and P. V. Thomsen. Teaching Style and learning outcomes. Paper presented at The XVIII edition of the GIREP International Conference, August 27 to September 1 2000. 2000b. Barcelona, GIREP. Conference Proceeding 34. Krogh, L. B. and P. V. Thomsen, "Studying students´ attitudes towards science from a cultural perspective but with a quantitative methodology: border crossing into the physics classroom," International Journal of Science Education 27, no. 3 (2005): 281-302. 35. Magnusson, S., J. Krajcik, and H. Borko. Nature, Sources, and Development of Pedagogical Content Knowledge for Science Teaching. In Examing Pedagogical Content Knowledge: The Construct and its Implications for Science Education. Edited by J. Gess-Newsome and N. Lederman. New York: Kluwer Academic Publishers, 1999. 36. Margolis, Howard and Patrick McCabe, "Improving Self-Efficacy and Motivation: What to Do, What to Say," Intervention in School and Clinic 41, no. 4 (2006): 218-227. 37. Martin, A. J., "Enhancing student motivation and engagement: The effects of a multidimensional intervention", Contemporary Educational Psychology 33, no. 2 (2008): 239-269. 38. Martin, A. J. and M. Dowson, "Interpersonal Relationships, Motivation, Engagement, and Achievement: Yields for Theory, Current Issues, and Educational Practice”, Review of educational research 79, no. 1 (2009): 327-365. 39. Midgley, C. and et al. Manual for the Patterns of Adaptive Learning Scales. 2000. http://www.umich.edu/~pals/PALS%202000_V13Word97.pdf, University of Michigan. 40. Niemiec, C. P. and R. M. Ryan, "Autonomy, competence, and relatedness in the classroom applying self-determination theory to educational practice," Theory and Research in Education 7 (2009): 133-144. 41. Osborne, J. and J. Dillon. Science Education in Europe: Critical Reflections. Report to the Nuffield Foundation. 2008. London, Kings College. 23 42. Osborne, J., S. Simon, and S. Collins, "Attitudes towards science: a review of the literature and its implications," International Journal of Science Education 25, no. 9 (2003): 1049-1079. 43. Pajares, F., "Self-Efficacy Beliefs in Academic Settings," Review of educational research 66, no. 4 (1996): 543-578. 44. Peterson, C. Semmel A. von Baeyer C. Abramson L. Metalsky & Seligman, "The Attributional Style Questionnaire," CognitiveTherapy and Research 6 (1982): 287-300. 45. Pintrich, P. R., "A motivational science perspective on the role of student motivation in learning and teaching contexts," Journal of Educational Psychology 95 (2003): 667-686. 46. Pintrich, P. R. and D. H. Schunk. Motivation in Education - Theory, Research and Applications. 2nd ed. Upper Saddle River, New Jersey: Merril Prentice Hall, 2002. 47. Reeve, Johnmarshall. Self-Determination Theory Applied to Educational Settings. In Handbook of Self-determination Research. Edited by E. L. Deci and R. M. Ryan. Woodbridge: University of Rochester Press, 2002. 48. Rocard, M. et al. Science Education Now - A Renewed Pedagogy for the Future of Europe. 2007. Luxembourg, Office for Official Publications of the European Communities. 49. Ryan, R. M. and E. L. Deci, "Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being," American Psychologist 55, no. 1 (2000): 68-78. 50. Schibeci, R. A., "Attitudes to science: an update," Studies in Science Education 11 (1984): 26-59. 51. Sølberg, J., "Udvikling af lokale naturfaglige kulturer", Danmarks Pædagogiske Universitet, DK, 2007). 52. Stipek, D. et al., "Can a teacher intervention improve classroom practices and student motivation in mathematics?", Journal of Experimental Education 66, no. 4 (1998): 319-337. 53. The Danish Evaluation Institute. Physics within the school - the school within physics. 2001. The Danish Evaluation Institute, In Danish. 54. Weiner, Bernard, "a Theory of Motivation for Some Classroom Experiences," Journal of Educational Psychology 71, no. 1 (1979): 3-25. 55. Wigfield, A., J. Eccles, and D. Rodriguez, "The Development of Children's Motivation in School Contexts," Review of Research in Education 23 (1998): 73-118. 56. Zeuner, L. et al. Lærerroller - stabilitet og forandring. 68. 2008. Odense, DK, Institut for Filosofi, Pædagogik og Religionsstudier, Syddansk Universitet. Gymnasiepædagogik. 24