Framing of Memorable Messages Running Head: EFFECTS OF FRAMING MEMORABLE BREAST CANCER MESSAGES The Effects of Framing of Memorable Breast Cancer Messages on Engagement in Detection or Prevention Behaviors This research was supported by the Breast Cancer and the Environment Research Centers grant number U01 ES012800 from the National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences (NIEHS), and the National Cancer Institute (NCI), NIH, DHHS. Its contents are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official views of the NIEHS, NCI, or NIH. 1 Framing of Memorable Messages 2 Abstract Memorable messages can to be guides to actions such as practicing health behaviors. Message framing suggests that gain framed messages are well suited to promote disease prevention behaviors, whereas loss-framed messages are better for promoting detection behaviors. A set of self-reported memorable messages about breast cancer was examined. About one-fourth of the reported messages were framed, and most of them were gain-framed. Gainframed messages were found to be significantly associated with prevention behaviors. Framing of Memorable Messages 3 The Effects of Framing of Memorable Breast Cancer Messages on Engagement in Detection or Prevention Behaviors Cancer is a health issue that cannot be ignored. It affects the lives of millions of people; the American Cancer Society (2008b) predicted that in 2008 alone, 1,437,180 new cancer cases would be diagnosed in the United States. Breast cancer is one of the most prevalent types, as it is the second leading cancer diagnosed in women, aside from skin cancer (American Cancer Society, Inc., 2008b). Due to the salience of breast cancer in our society, messages containing information about the disease are continuously being distributed through various media, interpersonal, and medical sources. The messages can be used to improve healthy behaviors and even save lives, because they contain information about breast cancer awareness, detection, prevention, and treatment (Pribble, et al., 2006). However, in order for these messages to have any effect, they must be both encountered and remembered by women. Thus, research has been conducted to study “memorable messages,” which are interpersonal messages that are “remembered for extremely long periods of time and which people perceive as a major influence on the course of their lives” (Knapp, Stohl, & Reardon, 1981, p. 27). Another important feature of messages is the way in which they are framed. Research on message framing, which refers to emphasizing either the benefits (gainframed) or the costs (loss-framed) of a behavior, has studied people’s interpretations of, and reactions to, different kinds of framing (for a discussion, see Rothman, Bartels, Wlaschin, & Salovey, 2006). This research effort collected reports of women’s memorable messages about breast cancer and asked them to report whether or not they had engaged in breast cancer detection or prevention behaviors. The reported messages were examined to see if framing was present, and Framing of Memorable Messages 4 the data were analyzed to determine if certain types of framing were associated with selfreported prevention and detection behaviors. The remainder of this paper will discuss breast cancer, review the research on memorable messages and message framing, provide the hypotheses and research questions, explain the method, review the results, and discuss the implications of the findings. Breast Cancer As previously stated, breast cancer is the second-most common cancer in women besides skin cancer. It is estimated that every woman has a 1 in 8 chance of developing breast cancer at some point in their lifetime (American Cancer Society, Inc., 2007). The National Cancer Institute (2008) estimates that 182,460 women will be diagnosed with and 40,480 women will die of breast cancer in 2008. For women, it is the second leading cause of cancer death, after lung cancer (American Cancer Society, Inc., 2007). The median age for breast cancer diagnosis is 61, but cases have been found in women as young as 20 years old (National Cancer Institute, 2008). It is important to distinguish between breast cancer prevention and breast cancer detection behaviors, which are both discussed throughout this paper. The definition used here for prevention behaviors is anything that reduces a woman's risk of getting breast cancer. The American Cancer Society (2008a) lists the following as behaviors that may reduce breast cancer risk: limiting alcohol use, exercising regularly, maintaining a healthy body weight, choosing to breast feed for at least several months, avoiding post-menopausal hormone therapy, and avoiding chemicals with estrogen-like properties. This study focuses specifically on exercise and healthy diet as forms of breast cancer prevention. Conversely, breast cancer detection behaviors are any acts which help to find existing cancer within the breasts. The following are listed by the American Cancer Society (2007) as methods to detect breast cancer: mammograms, clinical Framing of Memorable Messages 5 breast exams, breast self-exams, and magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). Mammograms and breast self-exams are the focus in this study in terms of detection behaviors. Memorable Messages Research has discovered that memorable messages are widespread and concern many topics. They have been studied within the contexts of final conversations about religious faith (Keeley, 2004), gendered socialization (Dallimore, 2003), aging (Holladay, 2002), and newcomer socialization in organizations (Stohl, 1986), among other topics. Memorable messages are seen as meaningful to the receiver, and they can affect behavior and guide sense-making processes (Holladay, 2002). According to Knapp et al. (1981) the reasons these messages are seen as memorable involve the nature of the message receiver, the sender, and the message itself. Usually, memorable messages are brief and provide rules for problem solving. Studies similar to this one have specifically examined memorable messages about breast cancer. Smith, Atkin, Skubisz, Munday, and Stohl (in press) found that women who are the most likely to recall memorable breast cancer messages have had personal experience and close friends or relatives who have had experience with breast cancer. Other research by Smith et al. (in press) has found that memorable breast cancer messages topics include early detection, awareness, treatment, and prevention of breast cancer, and the most common sources of these messages are the media, friends, family, and medical professionals. They also examined the impact of different sources of messages on prevention or detection behaviors, and found that medical professionals were substantially more likely to influence breast cancer detection than breast cancer prevention behaviors. However, research has yet to study the impact framing has on the behavioral effects of memorable messages. Message Framing Framing of Memorable Messages 6 Message framing refers to emphasizing either the benefits (gain-framed) or the costs (loss-framed) of a behavior. Various researchers (Dillard & Marshall, 2003; Rothman & Salovey, 1997; Wilson, Purdon, & Wallston, 1988) have concluded that each kind of framed message can take two forms—the outcome of the behavior can be described as desirable or undesirable, and the outcome can be described as being either attained (acquired, achieved, made more likely) or avoided (averted, not realized, made less likely)—leading to four possible framing scenarios. In their meta-analysis of framing studies, O'Keefe and Jensen (2006) supplemented this disambiguation by also considering the kernel states of framed messages. In their words, "The kernel state is the basic, root state mentioned in the message's description of the consequence" (p. 7). For example, in the message "If you take your hypertension medication, you will reduce your risk of heart disease," the kernel state is "heart disease," which is clearly undesirable. All of these methods of conceptualizing framed messages can placed into a 2 x 2 array, shown in Figure one, which expresses the four different scenarios in terms of the kernel states in messages. -- Insert Figure 1 here -Message framing was first discussed as a part of prospect theory, which was postulated by Tversky and Kahneman as a result of an experiment (1981). Their findings revealed that participants were much more likely to prefer a behavior perceived as "risky" if it was lossframed. These results have been applied to the area of health communication, with scholars hypothesizing that disease detection behaviors, which are considered risky due to the possibility of finding a disease, will be more likely to be influenced by loss-framed messages; in turn, disease prevention behaviors, which are less risky, will be more likely to be influenced by gain- Framing of Memorable Messages 7 framed messages (For a discussion, see Rothman, Bartels, Wlaschin, & Salovey, 2006; Salovey, Schneider, Apanovitch, 2002). Numerous studies have tested this hypothesis, leading to inconclusive results. One line of research has supported it: studies have found that loss-framed messages were more effective than gain-framed messages for encouraging mammography use (Banks et al., 1995; Schneider et al., 2001), breast self-exams (Meyerowitz & Chaiken, 1987), PAP test utilization (Rivers, Salovey, Pizarro, Pizarro, & Schneider, 2005) and HIV screening (Kalichman & Coley, 1995). Other research has found that gain-framed messages were more effective at encouraging prevention behaviors like using sunscreen (Detweiler et al., 1999;), using infant car seats (Christophersen & Gyulay, 1981), exercising (McCall & Ginis, 2007) and using condoms (Linville, Fischer, & Fischhoff, 1993). Nevertheless, two large meta-analyses yielded results that were not completely supportive of the detection/prevention hypothesis (O'Keefe & Jensen, 2006; O'Keefe & Jensen, 2007). Both meta-analyses found that gain-framed messages were significantly more likely to be more persuasive in terms of prevention behaviors. However, the 2007 analysis attributed this almost completely to dental-hygiene behaviors, and did not find any differences in persuasiveness for the preventive behaviors of safer-sex behaviors, skin cancer prevention behaviors, or diet and nutrition behaviors. Only the 2006 analysis tested messages in relation to detection behaviors and did not find any difference in the persuasiveness of gain- and lossframed messages. Research Questions and Hypotheses Though the research is mixed, the possibility that gain or loss framed messages can affect detection and prevention behaviors differentially is worth examining. Memorable messages, Framing of Memorable Messages 8 because they have been shown to affect behavior and guide sense-making processes, are a valuable topic of research in the realm of breast cancer prevention and detection. Research, however, has yet to investigate the intersection of framing and memorable messages and the effects it may have on the responses elicited. Additionally, to the best of the knowledge of the authors, this is the first study to examine a set of naturally occurring messages and determine their frames, as opposed to performing the usual practice of manipulating the messages experimentally. To examine the framing of breast cancer memorable messages reported by women, this study used the same classification of message framing as used by O'Keefe and Jensen in their metaanalyses (2006; 2007). Thus, our research questions address the frequencies of framed messages in general and each of the four framing possibilities: RQ1. What percentage of messages are either gain or loss framed? RQ2. What percentage of messages are gain-framed due to an undesirable kernel state that is avoided? RQ3. What percentage of messages are gain-framed due to a desirable kernel state that is attained? RQ4. What percentage of messages are loss-framed due to an undesirable kernel state that is attained? RQ5. What percentage of messages are loss-framed due to a desirable kernel state that is avoided? This study also sought to test the previously discussed prevention/detection hypothesis to examine whether memorable messages framed as gain or loss were associated with prevention or detection behaviors. This is reflected by the following hypotheses: Framing of Memorable Messages H1: Gain-framed messages will be significantly associated with prevention behaviors. H2: Loss-framed messages will be significantly associated with detection behaviors. 9 Method Participants Participants were recruited from breast cancer awareness organizations, advocacy groups, a large Midwestern university, and a medium-sized Western university in the United States. In sum, 359 female respondents completed the survey. Participants ranged from 18 to 85 years old. Caucasians composed the majority of the sample, 85%. African Americans (4%), Latinos (3%), Asian Americans (2%), Native Americans (2%), Pacific Islanders (3%), and those choosing not to respond (1%) composed the remainder of the sample. When participants were asked about their highest level of education, 11% reported a high school degree or less, 35% participants completed some college, technical school, or earned an associates degree, 32% earned a bachelor’s degree, and 22% had graduate schooling. Instrumentation and Procedure Participants completed a web-based survey questionnaire. The beginning of the survey gave the definition of a memorable message and asked participants if they could recall a memorable message about breast cancer. If the participant did have a memorable message (n = 207, 60%), they were asked to report it. Then, a series of open-ended questions were asked about their personal experience with breast cancer, as well as the characteristics of and responses to their memorable message. Framing of Messages Each of the reported memorable messages was first coded in terms of whether it had a desirable or undesirable kernel state. If the message included a kernel state (some did not), it was Framing of Memorable Messages 10 then listed and coded in terms of whether it was avoided or attained. The messages were coded reliably by two coders with 81% agreement for coding both the gain/loss framing of a message and the kernel states of the messages. Responses that were coded differently were discussed by the two coders and reconciled. Examples of the kernel states that were coded include "detect breast cancer," "death," "enjoy my grandchildren" and "life," among others. Relationship between Memorable Messages and Behaviors In order to assess the relationship between memorable messages and behaviors, respondents indicated whether they had engaged in breast cancer detection behaviors and/or breast cancer prevention behaviors. Responses to the questions, “Did you engage in breast self exams?” and “Did you engage in mammograms?” had a Pearson correlation of .52 (n = 214, p < .05) and were summed to form a measure of detection behaviors that ranged from zero to two. Responses to the questions, “Did you engage in eating healthy food?” and “Did you engage in exercise?” had a Pearson correlation of .71 (n = 210, p < .05). Summing responses to these two questions formed a measure of prevention behaviors that ranged from zero to two. Although the data are categorical, this procedure is meaningful because the categories differ in magnitude rather than quality. Consequently, the Pearson correlation coefficient as employed here can be interpreted in the usual manner. Results Framing of Messages Of reported messages, 23.08% (n=48) were framed. Twelve and a half percent of the messages were framed as having an undesirable kernel state that was avoided, and 68.75% had a desirable state that was attained; summed together, both of those percentages show that 81.25% of all framed messages were gain-framed. One example of an undesirable/avoided message is "I Framing of Memorable Messages 11 do not recall the exact wording, but there was a TV ad about a woman who was saying that women should be tested so that no one will lose a sister like she did." Here, the undesirable outcome of losing a sister is avoided by getting tested. An example of a desirable/attained message is "Early detection saves lives," where "life" is the desirable kernel state that is attained by early detection. Twelve and a half percent of the messages had an undesirable kernel state that was attained, and 6.25% had a desirable kernel state that was avoided; thus, 18.75% of all the framed messages were loss-framed. An example of a undesirable/attained message is "Smoking increases the chances of breast cancer," with "breast cancer" being the undesirable state that is attained/made more likely by smoking. A desirable/avoided message is "If I do not have the mass taken care of now, I may not be able to enjoy my grandchildren," where "enjoy my grandchildren" is the desirable state that is avoided/made less likely by not having a mass taken care of. Behavioral Outcomes Of women who reported a memorable message, 72.46% (n = 150) reported engaging in breast self-exams and 68.12% (n = 141) said that they had a mammogram. 60.89% (n = 126) of women reporting messages said that they engaged in breast self-exams and had a mammogram, whereas 78.26% (n = 162) of women reported engaging in at least one of the detection behaviors. In terms of prevention behaviors, 64.73% (n = 134) of women who reported a message said that they ate more healthy food and 63.29% (n = 131) said that they exercised. 57.01% (n = 118) of women reporting messages said that they engaged in both eating healthy food and exercise, and 69.57% (n =144) engaged in at least one of the prevention behaviors. Relationship between Message Framing and Behaviors Framing of Memorable Messages 12 Hypothesis one. Overall, data were consistent with hypothesis one, which predicted that gainframed messages would be significantly associated with prevention behaviors. Specifically, people who were exposed to messages with an undesirable kernel state that was avoided were more likely to engage in prevention behaviors than people not exposed to those messages (r = .31, p = .03, n = 48). This correlation came mainly from the interaction between undesirable/avoid gain-framed messages and engaging in exercise, as people exposed to those messages were more likely to exercise than those not exposed to it (r = .29, p = .04, n = 48). However, those participants who recalled a memorable message with a desirable kernel state that was attained were less likely to engage in prevention behaviors (r = -.50, p = .0003, n = 48) than those not exposed to such messages. Hypothesis two. Data were not consistent with hypothesis two, which said that loss-framed messages would be significantly associated with detection behaviors. People exposed to messages that contained an undesirable kernel state that was attained were no more likely to engage in detection behaviors than participants who were not (r = -.15, p = .32, n = 46). Similarly, people who reported messages with a desirable kernel state that was avoided were no more likely to engage in detection behaviors (r = .08, p = .62, n = 46). Discussion This research was innovative in comparison to the rest of the message framing literature, as to our knowledge no other studies have taken reported messages and examined them in terms of framing. Usually, studies experimentally create either gain- or loss-framed messages, present them to participants, and then analyze the results. This research brings forth the possibility that individuals can remember the specific framing of messages they hear, even after a long period of time, and that the framing can have differential effects on their behavior. It would be interesting Framing of Memorable Messages 13 for future research to more thoroughly test the long-term effects of framed messages (either experimentally created or reported by participants) to see if they affect behaviors in different ways. This potential for long-term influence is important in the realm of disease prevention and detection, because most behaviors advocated in these messages are behaviors that should be continued for a long period of time. The results of this study support the findings of the O'Keefe and Jensen meta-analyses (2006; 2007), which also found that gain-framed messages were more effective than loss-framed messages at persuading individuals to engage in disease prevention behaviors. This has important implications in the field of health communication, as disease prevention is increasingly an important goal for health educators. It is difficult to persuade people to engage in disease prevention behaviors because they often require a lot of personal effort (e.g. exercise, eating healthy, etc.), so it is valuable for researchers to know what ways of presenting the information are more persuasive than others. More research is needed in this area, however, as O'Keefe and Jensen found only a small effect for gain-framed messages (2006), and in one meta-analysis said that it was almost solely due to dental hygiene behaviors (2007). An interesting finding of this study was that participants reporting gain-framed memorable messages that were undesirable/avoided were more likely to engage in prevention behaviors than people who had not been exposed to them, but those who reported a desirable/attained gain-framed memorable message were less likely to engage in prevention behaviors. The research using the "kernel states" conceptualization is sparse, as it was first introduced by O'Keefe and Jensen in their 2006 meta-analysis. In both of their studies, they found that the phrasing of kernel states (whether they were desirable/undesirable or avoided/attained) did not have a significant effect on how persuasive the messages were (2006; Framing of Memorable Messages 14 2007). It would be useful for future research to further test this conceptualization to see if there are indeed differential effects in terms of the way the framed message is presented. Generalizations from these results must be made with caution due to the small sample size. The analysis of the data in this study was limited, due to the fact that few of the memorable messages reported had any framing (48/208). Most of these messages were gain-framed, which left very few loss-framed messages to analyze (n=9). However, they do raise interesting questions about the variant effects of gain- and loss-framed messages, the consequences of using different constructions to create the framed messages, and the long-term effects of being exposed to framed messages. Though it was critical to examine the framing of self-reported messages, it did limit the sample size. Thus, future research in this area should control framing in experimental messages and then test for its effects. Conclusion Although this study had a small sample size to work with, the results show that the intersection of framing and memorable messages is worthy of examination. If researchers can discern that framing a message in a certain way makes it more likely to be remembered, then that would be valuable information, as memorable messages have been shown to guide behaviors. Additionally, this study represents a significant step in the message framing literature and presents many possibilities for future research, which could address how memorable the framing of a message is over time, the long-term effects of message framing on behaviors, and the natural occurrence of framing in interpersonal communication, among other topics. Especially significant is the fact that this study lends support to the idea that gain-framed messages are more likely to result in disease prevention behaviors, and adds another level of specificity by showing that messages which discuss methods to avoid an undesirable kernel state Framing of Memorable Messages 15 are more likely to be associated with prevention behaviors. This also provides opportunities for future research, as additional study using the kernel states conceptualization could illuminate more information about effectively using framing in messages. This, in turn, could aid campaign creators, medical professionals, the media, and interpersonal sources in presenting breast cancer prevention and detection messages to others in the most persuasive way possible. Framing of Memorable Messages 16 References American Cancer Society, Inc. (2007). Breast cancer facts & figures 2007-2008. Retrieved November 30, 2008 from http://www.cancer.org/downloads/STT/BCFF-Final.pdf American Cancer Society, Inc. (2008a). Can breast cancer be prevented? Retrieved November 30, 2008 from http://www.cancer.org/docroot/CRI/content/CRI_2_4_2X_Can_breast_cancer_be_preven ted_5.asp?sitearea= American Cancer Society, Inc. (2008b). Cancer Facts & Figures 2008. Retrieved November 30, 2008 from http://www.cancer.org/downloads/STT/2008CAFFfinalsecured.pdf Banks, S. M., Salovey, P., Greener, S., Rothman, A. J., Moyer, A., Beauvais, J., et al. (1995). The effects of message framing on mammography utilization. Health Psychology, 14, 178-184. Christophersen, E. R., & Gyulay, J. E. (1981). Parental compliance with car seat usage: A positive approach with long-term follow-up. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 6, 301312. Dallimore, E.J. (2003). Memorable messages as discursive formations: The gendered socialization of new university faculty. Women’s Studies in Communication, 26, 214-265. Detweiler, J. B., Bedell, B. T., Salovey, P., Pronin, E., & Rothman, A. J. (1999). Message framing and sunscreen use: Gain-framed messages motivate beach-goers. Health Psychology, 18, 189-196. Dillard, J. P. & Marshall, L. J. (2003). Persuasion as a social skill. In J. O. Greene & B. R. Burleson (Eds.), Handbook of communication and social interaction skills (pp. 479-513). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Framing of Memorable Messages 17 Holladay, S.J. (2002). “Have fun while you can,” “You’re only as old as you feel,” and “Don’t ever get old!” An examination of memorable messages about aging. Journal of Communication, 52, 681-697. Kalichman, S. C., & Coley, B. (1995). Context framing to enhance HIV-antibody-testing messages targeted to African American women. Health Psychology, 14, 247-254. Keeley, M. (2004). Final conversations: Survivors’ memorable messages concerning religious faith and spirituality. Health Communication, 16, 87-104. Knapp, M. L., Stohl, C., & Reardon, K. K. (1981). “Memorable” messages. Journal of Communication, 31, 27-41. Linville, P. W., Fischer, G. W., & Fischhoff, B. (1993). AIDS risk perceptions and decision biases. In J. B. Pryor & G. D. Reeder (Eds.), The social psychology of HIV infection (pp. 5-38). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. McCall, L. A. & Ginis, K. A. M. (2004). The effects of message framing on exercise adherence and health beliefs among patients in a cardiac rehabilitation program. Journal of Applied Biobehavioral Research, 9, 122-135. Meyerowitz, B. E., & Chaiken, S. (1987). The effect of message framing on breast selfexamination attitudes, intentions, and behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 500-510. National Cancer Institute (2008). SEER stat fact sheets: Cancer of the breast. Retrieved November 30, 2008 from http://seer.cancer.gov/statfacts/html/breast.html O’Keefe, D. J., & Jensen, J. D. (2006). The advantages of compliance or the disadvantages of noncompliance? A meta-analytic review of the relative persuasive effectiveness of gainframed and loss-framed messages. Communication Yearbook, 30, 1-43. Framing of Memorable Messages 18 O'Keefe, D. J. & Jensen, J. D. (2007) The relative persuasiveness of gain-framed and lossframed messages for encouraging disease prevention behaviors: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Health Communication, 12, 623-644. Pribble, J. M., Goldstien, K. M., Fowler, E. F., Greenberg, M. J., Noel, S. K., & Howell, J. D. (2006). Medical news for the public to use? What’s on local TV news. American Journal of Managed Care, 12, 170-176. Rivers, S. E., Pizarro, D. A., Pizarro, J., Schneider, T. R., & Salovey, P. (2005). Message framing and pap test utilization among women attending a community health clinic. Journal of Health Psychology, 10, 67–79. Rothman, A. J. & Salovey, P. (1997). Shaping perceptions to motivate healthy behavior: The role of message framing. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 3-19. Rothman, A. J., Bartels, R. D., Wlaschin, J., & Salovey, P. (2006). The strategic use of gain- and loss-framed messages to promote healthy behavior: How theory can inform practice. Journal of Communication, 56, S202-S221. Salovey, P., Schneider, T. R., Apanovitch, A. M. (2002). Message framing in the prevention and early detection of illness. In J. P. Dillard and M. Pfau (Eds.) Persuasion Handbook: Developments in Theory and Practice (pp. 391-406). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications. Schneider, T. R., Salovey, P., Apanovitch, A. M., Pizarro, J., McCarthy, D., Zullo, J., & Rothman, A. J. (2001). The effects of message framing and ethnic targeting on mammography use among low-income women. Health Psychology, 20, 256-266. Framing of Memorable Messages 19 Smith, S. W., Atkin, C., Skubisz, C. M., Munday, S., & Stohl, C. (in press). The impact of personal and/or close relationship experience on memorable messages about breast cancer and the perceived speech acts of the sender. Journal of Cancer Education. Smith, S. W. Munday, S., LaPlante, C., Kotowski, M. R., Atkin, C., Skubisz, C. M., & Stohl, C. (in press). Topics and sources of memorable breast cancer messages and their impact on prevention and detection behaviors. Journal of Health Communication. Stohl, C. (1986). The role of memorable messages in the process of organizational socialization. Communication Quarterly, 34, 231-249. Tversky, A. & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science, 211, 453-458. Wilson, D. K., Purdon, S. E., & Wallston, K. A. (1988). Compliance to health recommendations: A theoretical overview of message framing. Health Education Research: Theory and Practice, 3, 161-171. Framing of Memorable Messages 20 Undesirable kernel state Desirable kernel state Avoided kernel state Gain-framed Loss-framed Attained kernel state Loss-framed Gain-framed Figure One: 2 x 2 array describing kernel state types and framing