The Effects of Operant Contingencies on Drug Sensitization and Tolerance to Chronic Administration of Amphetamine Cheryl Newbold, Student, Psychology Department Abstract Previous research has shown that drug sensitization, or the increase in the effect of a drug with repeated exposure, is related to relapse to drug taking in animal models of human behavior. Therefore, it is important to understand how the environment affects the development of sensitization so that human drug abusers’ environments can be modified such that relapse to drugs will be less likely to occur. Among other possible explanations, sensitization has shown to be more likely to occur when drug-related behaviors (i.e. locomotor activities) do not impede on an animal’s ability to obtain important goals. We believe this shows evidence that the development of sensitization or tolerance could be dependent upon operant contingencies, or different rates of reinforcement available to the organism. Therefore, in the proposed study, rats that have been given injections of d-amphetamine will respond for food reinforcers on two different schedules. In one schedule, decreases in response rate will cause decreases in the reinforcement rate. This is hypothesized to result in either tolerance or no sensitization, since gaining reinforcers would be incompatible with drug-induced locomotor activity. The theory is that the rat will learn to suppress those drug effects to obtain reinforcers. In the other schedule, decreases in response rate will not decrease the rate of reinforcement as substantially. This is hypothesized to result in sensitization, since responding would not be incompatible with drug-related behaviors, and therefore does not have to suppress those behaviors to gain reinforcers. This study is important in order to determine if drug sensitization and tolerance can be a function of the reinforcement contingencies in the organism’s environment. These results would not only be helpful in treating and preventing drug abuse, but in the medical field as well, in administering morphine or other painkillers to patients. Literature Review Drug sensitization is the increasing effect of a drug in response to a smaller amount of the drug. Previous studies have suggested that drug sensitization is more likely to result in relapse to drug taking after abstinence. Understanding how the environment is related to sensitization is therefore important in studying drug abuse. Several factors have been investigated and shown to be associated with the development of sensitization in animal models, including the schedule of drug administration (Robinson & Becker, 1986), individual differences of the subjects (Piazza, Deminiere, Le Moal, & Simon 1989; Robinson & Becker 1986; Sax & Strakowski 2001) and the novelty of the environment (Robinson & Berridge 2000). However, these results have not given us a complete understanding of when tolerance or sensitization will occur after chronic drug use. Other studies have shown that rats are more likely to become sensitized to a drug if it does not interfere with their goal-directed behaviors (Wolgin and Hertz 1995; Pinkston and Branch 2003). Conversely, if drug-related behaviors (such as locomotor activity) inhibit the rat’s ability to feed, they are more likely to show tolerance to the drug. The most convincing evidence of this is Wolgin and Hertz (1995), in which the development of tolerance or sensitization to drugs was determined by how compatible feeding was with drug-related behaviors. Rats were given either acute or chronic injections of cocaine, and fed either with a bottle or a cannula. Bottle-feeding involves more movements and goal-directed behaviors than feeding with a cannula. At acute doses, bottle-fed rats showed more stereotyped movements than cannula-fed rats. However, at chronic doses, bottle-fed rats developed tolerance, and cannula-fed rats developed sensitization. This suggests that rats will learn to suppress those drug-induced behaviors that impede on their ability to gain food, since drinking from a bottle uses movements and behaviors inconsistent with the locomotor activity caused by drugs. On the other hand, if milk is injected into the rats’ mouth, there are no behaviors related to feeding that are competing with drug-related behaviors. Because it has been shown that drug sensitization can be determined by how the drug effect interacts with the animal’s ability to feed, it is possible that sensitization can be mediated by operant contingencies of food reinforcement. This was investigated by Pinkston and Branch (2003) in which cocaine was given to pigeons in two different groups. One group was exposed to a fixed-ratio 20 schedule of food reinforcement, in which 3-second access to food was given after 20 consecutive key-pecking responses. The second group did not receive exposure to any type of schedule. They found that the first group exhibited tolerance, and the second group showed sensitization to cocaine. These results are consistent with the idea that sensitization can be determined by the animal’s ability to gain food; the first group had to suppress their drug-induced behaviors to gain food, whereas the second group did not have that opportunity. However, there have been few studies on how operant contingencies can affect reaction to drugs, and further research should be done to better understand the factors that are more likely to result in sensitization. In the proposed study, rats will be used as the subjects, and it will use a withinsubjects design. There will be operant contingencies in both components to better determine how behavior and rates of reinforcement interact to influence drug effects. It is still unknown how behavior to different reinforcement rates in an animal’s environment change drug effects, and the goal of this study is to investigate those effects. This is a critical experiment to carry out, as it will be filling a gap in the literature. Research Methods Subjects These experiments will use a “small-N” design. Each subject will experience all components of the experiment, so its behavior in one component will be compared to its behavior in the other component. These methods are standard for this discipline, and minimize the number of animals needed for research while yielding sufficient data. Four male Long-Evans rats (Charles River, Michigan, USA), will be approximately 90 days old upon arrival to the behavior lab located in the Laboratory Animal Research Center (LARC) at USU. All experiments and housing of the animals will take place in the behavior lab and will be in compliance with the USDA Animal Welfare Act, PHS "Policy for the Humane Care and Use of Animals,” U.S. Government Principles for the Utilization and Care of Vertebrate Animals Used in Testing, Research, and Training; and the Animal Welfare Policies of the University. This study is approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC) under the advisor’s current protocol (IACUC #1098). After free access to food and water for two weeks, food intake will be restricted to reduce the rats to 85% of their free-feeding weight for an additional two weeks. The rats will be maintained at this weight through post session feeding as necessary. Rats will be housed individually with unrestricted access to water under a 12:12 reversed light-dark cycle. Sessions will be conducted daily at the same time. Apparatus Eight Coulbourn operant chambers (29 cm long, 24 cm wide and 29 cm high), in sound-attenuating enclosures, will be used. The front panel has a houselight at the top center, and two response levers centered 13 cm apart, above which are three 28 V DC lamps. A solenoid-operated feeder, with a light, dispenses 45-mg food pellets. Control of experimental events and data recording will be conducted with Med-Associates interfacing and software using a personal computer located in an adjacent room. Procedure Rats will first be trained to take food pellets from the dispenser in the chamber by delivering pellets after 20 seconds regardless of their behavior. When a pellet is delivered, the house light will be extinguished and the feeder aperture will be lit for .4 s. When a rat is reliably and quickly eating food pellets, lever press training will begin. Rats will be trained to press the levers through an autoshaping procedure in which the lights above the bar will be lit, and rats will put their front feet on the bar to investigate the bar, which will result in reinforcement (food pellet delivery) after each bar press response. Once a rat is reliably pressing the levers, the response requirement will gradually increase to a variable ratio (VR) 60 schedule of reinforcement on the right lever, which provides reinforcement after an average of 60 responses are made. The response requirement on the left lever will gradually increase to a variable interval (VI) 60 second schedule, in which food is given after an average of 60 seconds. These schedules are being used to examine how behavior in response to different rates of reinforcement will interact with the drug effects. The VI and VR schedules and stimuli signaling the schedules will be counterbalanced on the right and left levers for half of the rats. Formal data collection will begin when responding is considered stable (showing no systematic variation in daily response rates for at least 10 sessions, as determined by visual assessment by the primary investigators). During baseline, rats will be given intraperitoneal injections of saline 15 min prior to the beginning of each session. In all phases, sessions will be preceded by a 15 min chamber blackout immediately following injections. This is to allow time for drug absorption when applicable. The rats will respond on a multiple VI 60 VR 60 schedule for 30 sessions to determine response rates without the influence of d-amphetamine. The duration of each session will be 30 min or 48 reinforcers, whichever occurs first. Rats will then be given acute d-amphetamine injections 15 min prior to each session in varying doses (0.3, 1.0, 3.0, 5.6, 10.0 mg/kg of d-amphetamine and saline, respectively) twice per week to determine the dose-effect curve (see Fig. 1). D-amphetamine sulfate salt will be ordered through the L.A.R.C.’s attending veterinarian. The same multiple VI 60 VR 60 schedule will be available as in baseline. Determinations of the effects of each dose will be made twice, and a dose that causes at least a 25% change in response rate as compared to control will be administered chronically each day under the same schedules of reinforcement for 30 sessions. After this chronic treatment phase, the rats will return to baseline conditions and be tested with all doses again to determine the acute dose-effect curve again. If predictions are correct, this dose-effect curve will shift to the left in the VI component, showing sensitization, or an increase in the effect of the drug; and to the right in the VR component, showing tolerance, or a decrease of the effects of amphetamine. For data analysis, a two-way Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) will be used to find significant differences between the components. The two factors in the ANOVA will be the drug condition, acute or chronic, and the operant component, VI or VR. The dependent variable will be the dose at which responding is decreased by at least 50% as compared to control on the dose-effect curve. An alpha level of .05 will be used. Hypotheses: In the VR component, rats’ response rates are hypothesized to increase after chronic drug administration as compared to their own response rates in the acute drug phase. This shows tolerance to the drug. Conversely, rats’ response rates are hypothesized to decrease in response to the VI component after chronic drug use. This signifies sensitization to the drug. Dose-effect curve Responses per minute 40 30 Con Sal 20 10 0.3 1 3 5.6 0 10 Amphetamine Dose (mg/kg) Figure 1. Hypothetical data showing a decrease in response rates with increasing amphetamine doses. Expected Timeline March 6 March 20 April 3 April 17 May 15 June 19 July 18 August 22 Fall 2006 Order rats from Charles-River Rats on free-feeding Rats on food restriction to return to 85% of free-feeding weight Bar press training begins (Dependent upon when responding is stable) Initial injections (Baseline) Chronic administration Return to baseline, rats tested with all doses again Experiment finished. Data analysis and create future research projects Results presented to Student Advocates for Behavior Science (SABS) meeting September 15 Report results to Provost’s Office and receipts to Vice President for Research Office April 2007 Results presented at USU Student Showcase May 25-29, 2007 Results to be presented as poster presentation at the Association for Behavior Analysis 33rd Annual Convention in San Diego, California Conclusion This research is expected to not only contribute to the existing body of knowledge in this discipline and help in understanding how the environment can affect an organism’s reaction to drugs, but will also benefit me as a student. The experience I would gain in the behavior lab would be a valuable asset in applying for graduate studies in behavioral pharmacology. This is an excellent opportunity for me to learn injection procedures on a rat, carry out experimental research, analyze data, and alter procedures as necessary. I believe that this experience will make me a more desirable applicant to graduate school and make the transition to graduate school easier as I would require less training. The expenses described below are vital in carrying out this specific research. Thank you for considering my project to be funded. Budget Item Description Costs Rats Four Male Long Evans rats from Charles River, 81-90 days old, 351-375 g upon arrival Four at $40.85 each = $163.40 + $22.73 (shipping) + $10.70(crate charge) = $196.83 D-Amphetamine Sulfate Salt from Sigma Aldrich 5 grams - $77.60 + $60.00 (Admin fee) + $19.43 (shipping/handling) = $157.03 Amphetamine Animal Care Animal Care from Laboratory Animal Research Center at Utah State University 4 rats, 6 months at $.251/day/each = $180.72 Food reinforcers Bio-Serve dustless precision pellets, 45 mg each One box of 50,000 pellets = $77.00 + 15.00 (shipping) = $92 Syringes Syringes to inject amphetamine from Fisher 5 boxes at $20.18 each = $100.90 + $10.00 (shipping) = $110.90 Total Item Cost $196.83 $157.03 $180.72 $92 $110.90 Total Cost $737.48 Amount requested from URCO funds $368.74 Amount secured from Psychology department at USU $368.74 References: Piazza, P.V., Deminiere, J.M., Le Moal, M., & Simon, H. (1989). Factors that predict invidiual vulnerability to amphetamine self-administration. Science, 245, 1511-1513. Pinkston, J.W., and Branch, M.N. (2003). Sensitization to cocaine in pigeons: Interaction with an operant contingency. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 11, 102-109. Robinson, T.E., & Becker, J.B. (1986). Enduring changes in brain and behavior produced by chronic amphetamine administration: a review and evaluation of animal models of amphetamine psychosis. Brain Research Reviews, 11, 157-198. Robinson, T.E., & Berridge, K.C. (2000). The psychology and neurobiology of addiction: an incentive-sensitization view. Addiction, 95, 91-117. Sax , K.W., & Strakowski, S.M. (2001). Behavioral Sensitization in Humans. Journal of Addictive Diseases, 20, 55-65. Wolgin, D.L., & Hertz, J.M. (1995). Effects of acute and chronic cocaine on milk intake, body weight, and activity in bottle- and cannula-fed rats. Behavioral Pharmacology, 6, 746-753.