Morality and Justice 1 Running head: MORALITY AND JUSTICE The Role of Leader Morality in the Interaction Effect of Procedural Justice and Outcome Favorability Xiao-Wan LIN City University of Hong Kong Mailing Address: Department of Management, City University of Hong Kong, 83# Tat Chee Avenue, Kowloon Tong, Kowloon, Hong Kong; Email: mgxwlin@cityu.edu.hk; Tel: +853-21942564; Fax: +852-27887220. Hong-Sheng CHE Beijing Normal University, China Mailing Address: School of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, 19# Xin Jie Kou Wai Street, Beijing, China; Email: hongshengche@sina.com; Tel: +86-10-58800349. Kwok LEUNG City University of Hong Kong Mailing Address: Department of Management, City University of Hong Kong, 83# Tat Chee Avenue, Kowloon Tong, Kowloon, Hong Kong; Email: mgkleung@cityu.edu.hk; Tel: +852-27887969. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Xiao-Wan LIN. Leader Morality 2 Abstract The present research explored the role of leader morality in the interaction effect of procedural justice and outcome favorability, and attempted to connect justice and morality construct in a new direction. Two studies in different settings and using different designs (a scenario experiment and a survey) yielded convergent results. When leader morality was high, the interaction effect of procedural justice and outcome favorability was significant, and fair procedures can mitigate the negative effect of low outcome favorability. When leader morality was low, however, the interaction between procedural justice and outcome favorability was absent. Leader Morality 3 The Role of Leader Morality in the Interaction Effect of Procedural Justice and Outcome Favorability Since Adams (1965) proposed the equity theory, organizational justice has attracted a great deal of attention from management and psychology scholars. In the early phase of this line of research, researchers focused on the justice of outcomes, or distributive justice. Thibaut and Walker’s (1975) seminal work shifted the attention toward fairness issues concerning procedures, mechanisms, and processes used to determine outcomes, broadly known as procedural justice. A number of studies have found that employee attitudes and behaviors are not only affected by outcomes, but also by procedures. Generally, people react more positively to favorable rather than unfavorable outcomes, and fair rather than unfair procedures. Commonly examined outcome variables include outcome satisfaction, job satisfaction, organizational commitment, trust, evaluation of authority, organizational citizenship behaviors, withdrawal, negative reactions and performance (see Colquitt, Conlon, Wesson, Porter, & Ng, 2001). The consequences of outcomes and procedures have also been tested in different settings, such as selection (e.g., Gilliland, 1994; Ployhart & Ryan, 1998), pay relocation (e.g., Cobb & Frey, 1996; Dulebohn & Martocchio, 1998; Welbourne, 1998), Organizational change (e.g., Spreizer & Mishra, 2002; Tyler & De Cremer,2005), performance appraisal (e.g., Brown & Benson, 2003), and tenure and promotion decisions (e.g., Ambrose & Cropanzano, 2003). Based on more than forty previous studies, Brockner and Wiesenfeld (1996) concluded that there exists an interaction between procedural justice and outcome favorability. Specifically, the effect of outcome favorability is stronger when procedures are unfair than fair. Because high procedural justice can attenuate the negative effect of Leader Morality 4 low outcome favorability, this interaction pattern is also known as compensatory effect of procedural justice (Kwong & Leung, 2002). While the interactive relationship between procedural justice and outcome favorability is very robust (e.g., Cremer & Knippenber, 2003), Lind (2001) pointed out that the conclusion is far too general, and this interaction may vary across different contexts. Indeed, a number of factors have been found to qualify the interaction between procedural fairness and outcome favorability (e.g., Brockner, 2002; Kwong & Leung, 2002 ; Van den Bos, Vermunt, & Wilke 1997). However, these studies focused on self-perceived attributes of the recipient of a justice decision (e.g., Brockner, Chen, Mannix, Leung, & Skarlicki, 2000) or of the relationship between the parties involved (Kwong & Leung, 2002). To the best of our knowledge, no attempt has been made to examine whether the attributes of decision-makers in a justice context can qualify the compensatory effect of procedural justice. This gap is unsatisfactory because decades of research on person perception clearly shows that people’s attitudes and behaviors are affected by their perception of the attributes of other social actors. For example, perceived agreeableness of individuals emerged as a powerful determinant of others’ associative intentions toward them, and perceived conscientiousness of individuals is strongly related to the trusting intentions they received (Bond & Forgas, 1984). Perception of the emotionality of a disputant affected one’s choice of conflict resolution strategy for resolving a dispute with this person (Morris, Leung, & Iyengar, 2004). The present paper adds to the previous research by adopting a person perception perspective in examining the impact of perceived leader morality on the compensatory effect of procedural justice. More specifically, we argue that leader morality qualifies the interaction effect of procedural justice and outcome favorability. When leader morality is high, procedural justice could compensate for the negative effect of unfavorable Leader Morality 5 outcomes; however, when leader morality is low, the compensatory effect of procedural justice disappears. The ethical dimension of leadership is uniquely important because of the leader’s potential to influence the ethical climate of an organization and employees’ ethical attitudes and behaviors (Schminke, Ambrose, & Neubaum, 2005; Treviño, Brown, & Hartman, 2003). From a practical perspective, a leader who lacks morality and integrity may destroy a company much faster than a leader who promotes a bad strategy (Black, Morrison, & Gregersen, 1999), which is vividly illustrated by the Enron case. Previous research on leader morality primarily focused on its main effect on employee attitudes and behaviors (e.g., Cheng, Chou, Wu, Huang, & Farh, 2004; Koh & Boo, 2004; Parry & Proctor-Thomson, 2002; Schiebel & Pochtrager, 2003). The present research further explored the more intricate effect of leader morality as a moderator of the interaction between procedural justice and outcome favorability. It also should be noted that, in recent years, the important role of morality in the justice domain has been more and more recognized (e.g., Cropanzano, Goldman, & Folger, 2003; Cropanzano & Rupp, 2002; Folger, 2001; Leung & Tong, 2003). Scholars attempt to use morality to explain why people care about justice and how they react to injustice. Echoing the call for linking morality to justice constructs, the present research attempts to explore the joint effects of leader morality and organizational justice from a new perspective. The study will not only shed new light on the boundary conditions of compensatory effect of procedural justice, but also provide new evidence for the importance of leader morality in organizational justice phenomena. Leader Morality 6 Leader Morality Although leader morality has drawn a great deal of attention from academics and practitioners, its definition and measurement are still dubious. In fact, the terms “leader morality”, “integrity”, and “ethicality” are often considered as interchangeable, and their measurements often overlap. Craig and Gustafson (1998) developed a 31-item scale of perceived leader integrity and the scale covered several domains, such as, truth-telling, compliance with policies and procedures, and maliciousness. Law, Lee, Farh, and Pillutla (2001) identified a new component of justice—moral justice—based on the justice/injustice incidents described by participants that were related to the personal integrity of their leaders. This construct consists of nine aspects, including rejection of nepotism, responsibility, keeping promise, creating opportunities for subordinates’ personal development, and compliance with law. Treviño et al. (2003) identified three dimensions of ethical leadership: People-orientation (e.g., respect and mentor people), Visible ethical actions and traits (e.g., role-model; be honest; be a listener; trustworthy; and have integrity), and Setting ethical standards and accountability. Recently, Hsu, Cheng, Huang, and Farh, (2004), based on an inductive approach, developed a scale of moral leadership that includes seven dimensions: impartialness, justness, integrity, trustworthiness, responsibility, broad-mind, and lead by example. A synthesis of the various measurement scales suggests that the major characteristics of a moral leader include selflessness, conscientiousness, trustworthiness, benevolence toward subordinates, compliance with policies and regulations, and the display of exemplary ethical behaviors. Existing empirical studies on leader morality have mainly focused on three directions. Firstly, as discussed above, some studies have explored the content domain of ethical/moral leadership, and developed some measurement instruments (e.g., Brown, Leader Morality 7 Treviño, & Harrison, 2005; Craig & Gustafson, 1998; Hsu, Cheng, Huang, & Farh, 2004; Treviño et al., 2003); secondly, some studies have examined antecedents of unethical behaviors in organizations, and identified a number of important individual, organizational, and relationship factors (e.g., Brass, Butterfield, & Skaggs, 1998; Kahn, 1990); thirdly, some studies have focused on the consequences of leader morality (e.g., Cheng et al., 2004; Koh & Boo, 2004; Parry & Proctor-Thomson, 2002; Schiebel & Pochtrager, 2003). The present research extended this third line of work and examined the consequences of leader morality in the domain of organizational justice. It is well-known that leader morality is closely related to leader effectiveness (e.g., Kanungo & Mendonca, 1996). Leader morality can enhance followers’ respect, identification, compliance without dissent, and extra effort (Cheng et al., 2004), satisfaction with leadership, and motivation (Parry & Proctor-Thomson, 2002). Leader’s morality is also the best predictor of trust in leaders (Morgan, 1989). Job satisfaction and organizational commitment are also positively related to top managers’ ethical behavior (Koh & Boo, 2004). Before we explore how leader morality impacts the interaction effect of procedural justice and outcome favorability, we first state a well-known finding as our first hypothesis, namely, leader morality affects employee attitudes positively. The Role of Leader Morality in the Compensatory Effect of Procedural Justice Why can high procedural justice attenuate the negative impact of unfavorable outcomes? Brockner and Wiesenfeld (1996) provided several explanations, among which Lind and Tyler’s (1988) group-value theory or Tyler and Lind’s (1992) relational model is perhaps most important. This theory assumes that people value their long-term social relationship with other individuals, groups, or organizations, and link procedural justice judgments to the desire to be a full-fledged, full-status member of a group, organization, or Leader Morality 8 society (Lind & Earley, 1992; Tyler, 1989). When procedures are fair, people feel valued and respected. Because procedures are considered as relatively stable, people feel reassured about their identity and esteem in the future, leading them to care less about the current outcomes (Brockner & Wiesenfeld, 1996). Brockner and his colleagues (Brockner, Siegel, Daly, Tyler, and Martin, 1997) provided another explanation for the interaction effect of procedural justice and outcome favorability. They found that it was not procedural justice per se, but the degree of trust elicited by procedural justice that interacted with outcome favorability to influence employee attitudes and behaviors. They argued that unfavorable outcomes elicit a sensemaking process in which people attempt to evaluate the trustworthiness of organizational authorities, and that people may have used judgments of procedural justice to make inferences about the trustworthiness of the authorities. When procedural justice is high, the trust in the leader offsets the negative impact of unfavorable outcomes. Although seemingly different, the relational model and trust explanations converge in that when individuals are concerned about their relationships with other people, groups, or organizations, they will use the information about procedural justice to make inference about the nature of the relationships, including whether their identity and standing can be reassured, or whether the authority can be trusted. In line with the argument, several studies have revealed that the compensatory effect of procedural justice is qualified by the psychological importance people accord to a relationship (e.g., Brockner et al., 2000; Kwong & Leung, 2002). Brockner et al. (2000) found that the compensatory effect of procedural justice is operative only among people with interdependent self-construal, who regard themselves as connected to others and view their relationships with others as important, but not among people with independent selfconstrual. A direct demonstration of this effect is provided by Kwong & Leung (2002). Leader Morality 9 Their studies in two different settings (interpersonal dispute resolution and salary freeze or cut in an organizational context) consistently showed that procedural justice reduced the negative impact of low outcome favorability only when the relationship involved was regarded as important. The present research continues this line of enquiry by examining the boundary conditions of the compensatory effect of procedural justice from a new perspective. As mentioned previously, previous research has ignored the possibility that perceived attributes of authority figures may qualify the interaction between procedural justice and outcome favorability. This research tested the role of perceived leader morality as a moderator of the interactive relationship between procedural justice and outcome favorability. To develop a theoretical foundation for predicting how leader morality may impact the interactive relationship between procedural justice and outcome favorability, we first note that leader morality is known to influence employees’ feeling about the importance of their relationships with leaders. For instance, Brower, Schoorman and Tan (2000) proposed a model of relational leadership, which assumed that parties to a leader-member relationship evaluate the ability, benevolence and integrity of each other in establishing a leader-member exchange relationship. In other words, leader morality is an important factor that determines the strength of a leader-member relationship. In addition, perceived leader morality is associated with identification with a leader (Cheng et al., 2004), and perceived leader ethicality positively influences followers’ organizational commitment (Koh, & Boo, 2004). These findings suggest that people value their relationship with moral leaders and the organizations they represent, and accord more importance to this relationship. Leader Morality 10 Given our analysis that moral leadership is related to a heightened importance accorded to a leader-member relationship, we hypothesize that when perceived leader morality is high, procedural justice becomes important and consequential because people will use it to infer how they are treated by the authority (including whether their identity and status will be reassured, whether the authority is trustworthy), and the compensatory effect of procedural justice is operative. In contrast, if perceived leader morality is low, people are unlikely to value their relationship with a leader. In such a context, the significance of procedural justice will be attenuated, and the compensatory effect of procedural justice will disappear. The moderating effect of leader morality constitutes the second hypothesis of the research. Overview of Studies Two studies were conducted in different settings to test the hypotheses. In Study 1, undergraduate students participated in a scenario experiment involving a personnel selection context to test the causal effects of the independent variables: Leader morality, procedural justice, and outcome favorability. To overcome the lack of external validity of Study 1, Study 2 asked full-time employees to recall a real performance appraisal incident to evaluate the two hypotheses proposed. In addition, the contexts of the two studies differed so that convergent results would provide strong evidences for the generality of the effects. Sweeney and McFarlin (1993) showed that procedural justice is a more important predictor of “organizational outcomes” (e.g., organizational commitment) than of “personal outcome” (e.g., pay satisfaction). In order to reveal the change of the compensatory effect of procedural justice under different conditions, we chose employees’ attitudes to their organization as the dependent variables. In Study 1 (recruitment setting), Leader Morality 11 “attractiveness of the organization to applicants” was used as the outcome variable, since the target persons in the scenario had not joined the organization. In study 2 (performance appraisal setting), “pride in organization” was used as the outcome variable. These two dependent variables, while not identical, are conceptually related, because both reflect a positive attitude toward the organization. Our hypotheses are formally stated as follows: Hypothesis 1: Leader morality positively affects employees’ attitudes toward their organizations (attractiveness of the organization to applicants in Study 1, and pride in organization in Study 2). Hypothesis 2: There exists a triple interaction effect of leader morality, procedural justice, and outcome favorability on employees’ attitudes toward their organizations (attractiveness of the organization to applicants in Study 1, and pride in organization in Study 2). When perceived leader morality is high, procedural justice can reduce the negative effects of low outcome favorability. When leader morality is low, the compensatory effect of procedural justice is absent. Study 1 Method Participants Participants consisted of 96 undergraduate students in a university in Beijing, China, including 21 males and 75 females. They were from different departments (e.g., department of foreign language, Chinese, history). Design and Procedure Participants responded to a scenario during class, which was modeled upon similar scenarios used in previous research (e.g., Brockner et al., 2000; Van den Bos et al., 1997). Participants were asked to read a vignette and imagine they were the target person taking part in a selection process of a famous organization. They were applying for a desirable Leader Morality 12 position in the organization, and had completed a series of nine selection tests including intelligence tests, personality measurements, and interviews. When waiting for the results, they overheard some statements about the morality of the human resource (HR) manager who was responsible for the hiring results. A week later, they received a letter informing them of the selection procedures and the results. Two levels of outcome favorability, procedural justice and leader morality were manipulated in the vignette, resulting in eight versions. Participants were randomly assigned to receive one of the eight versions of the vignette. Each version was read by 9 -14 participants, and the gender ratio was similar across different conditions. After reading the vignette, they were asked to complete a short questionnaire including the manipulation checks and the dependent variable. Manipulation of outcome favorability. In the high outcome-favorability condition, participants were told that they were given the offer. In the low outcome-favorability condition, participants were told that they did not get the job. Manipulation of procedural justice. In the high procedural-justice condition, participants were told that the hiring decision was based on their performance on all the nine selection tests. In the low procedural-justice condition, participants were told that the hiring decision was based on their performance on only one of the nine tests, and other pertinent information was ignored. Manipulation of leader morality. Leader morality was manipulated by eight aspects that were derived from various scales for measuring leader morality reviewed above: not abusing power to pursue personal gains, being responsible, treasuring subordinates’ talent, complying with the law, being tolerant, not coercing subordinates, living on promises, and serving as a good role model. In the high leader-morality condition, participants were told that they heard from some friends that the HR manager responsible for the hiring results was a tolerant and responsible person who complied with the law, lived on promise, Leader Morality 13 treasured subordinates’ talent, never abused power to pursue personal gain, never coerced subordinates, and was a good role model. In the low leader-morality condition, participants were told that they heard from some friends that the HR manager was a person who was jealous of subordinates, often abused power, shirked responsibilities, pursued personal vendetta, coerced subordinates, failed to keep promises, scorned the law, and was a bad role model. Manipulation checks. The effectiveness of the outcome favorability manipulation was assessed by the item “I consider the outcome as favorable.” The effectiveness of the procedural justice manipulation was assessed by the item “I believe that the selection procedures were fair.” The item for checking the manipulation of leader morality was “The HR manager is a moral person.” A 6-point scale with endpoints labeled with totally disagree (1) and totally agree (6) was used. Dependent variable. Since the target person in the vignette had not joined the company, we used the attractiveness of the organization to the respondent as the dependent variable. Two items were used: “The company is worth joining” and “I will recommend the organization to my friends.” As before, a 6-point scale was used. Results Participants’ responses to the questionnaire were analyzed with analysis of variance. Manipulation checks Analyses revealed highly significant main effects of the manipulated variables on the corresponding manipulation check items. Participants reported that outcome was more favorable in the high than in the low outcome-favorability condition (M = 5.17 vs. 2.26 respectively), F(1, 88) = 102.61, p < .001. They reported that procedural justice was higher in the high than in the low procedural-justice condition (M = 4.10 vs. 2.90 respectively), F(1, 88) = 15.20, p < .01. Moreover, they reported that leader morality was higher in the high than in Leader Morality 14 the low leader-morality condition (M = 3.53 vs. 2.29 respectively), F(1, 88) = 23.03, p < .001. Other significant effects were also found, but they did not suggest any problem with the effectiveness of the manipulations1. Test of hypotheses All main effects (outcome favorability, F(1, 88) = 21.25, p < .001; procedural justice, F(1, 88) = 13.83, p < .001; leader morality, F(1, 88) = 16.18, p < .001) were significant. The three main effects revealed that the attractiveness of the organization was higher among participants who received a favorable than an unfavorable outcome (M = 3.90 vs. 2.88, respectively), who were subject to high than low procedural justice (M = 3.77 vs. 2.97, respectively), and who perceived the leader as high than low in leader morality (M = 3.81 vs. 2.98, respectively). Hypothesis1 was supported because a moral leader was able to heighten the appeal of an organization to job applicants. A two-way interaction between procedural justice and leader morality, F(1, 88) = 6.69, p < .05, was found. This two-way interaction was not interpreted given the significant triple interaction (outcome favorability × procedural justice × leader morality), F(1, 88) = 5.13, p < .05. To understand the nature of the triple interaction, simple interaction effect analyses and simple main effect analyses were conducted. In the high leader-morality condition, the interaction between outcome favorability and procedural justice was significant, F(1, 88) = 5.49, p < .05. As showed in Figure1, high procedural justice could reduce the influence of outcome favorability. Analysis of simple main effects showed that outcome favorability showed a strong and positive effect only when procedural justice was low, F(1, 88) = 24.55, p < .001, but not when procedural justice was high, F(1, 88) = 1.98, p > .1. In the low leader-morality condition, the interaction between outcome favorability and procedural justice, F(1, 88) = .66, ns, and the main effect of procedural justice, F(1, 88) Leader Morality 15 = .69, ns, were not significant. Only the main effect of outcome favorability was significant, F(1, 88) = 4.16, p < .05. This pattern of results supported Hypothesis 2. Insert Figure 1 about here Discussion The current study provided support for our key hypothesis that when leader morality is high, the interaction between procedural justice and outcome favorability resembles the pattern that Brockner and Wiesenfeld (1996) depicted. That is, fair procedures can reduce the negative effect of low outcome favorability. When leader morality is low, however, the compensatory effect of procedural justice is absent, and people’s reactions are significantly affected by outcome favorability regardless of whether the procedures are fair or not. Although the manipulation of the independent variables (leader morality, procedural justice, and outcome favorability) established their causal effects, there are two limitations of the design. First, in justice research, although results based on scenarios typically resemble behavioral results based on experiments (e.g., Van den Bos, 1999) and survey results based on real experiences (e.g., Brockner et al., 2000), it is prudent to replicate the results in a real-life setting. Second, because the participants were full-time students, it is important to evaluate our hypotheses with participants who have real working experiences. To overcome these two limitations, Study 2 replicated the results of Study 1 in a field setting involving full-time employees. Furthermore, Study 2 was a conceptual replication of Study 1 and involved a different context, namely, performance appraisal. If converging results are obtained, the generality of the findings is greatly enhanced. Leader Morality 16 Study 2 Method Participants A total of 181 working adults (42 men and 139 women) who enrolled in a part-time program in a University in Beijing, China participated voluntarily in the study. Participants of the study met two criteria: (1) they had worked for more than 3 months, and (2) they had the experience of performance appraisal before. Most participants were from 21 to 40 years old (21-30: 41.6%; 31-40: 44.5%), 13.3% of them were 41-50 years old, and only 0.6% were above 50 year old. Nearly all of them (99.4%) finished junior college or above. With regard to the type of organizations they worked for, 18% worked for foreign ventures, 19.3% for state-owned companies, 16.7% for private companies, and 8.7% for international joint-ventures, 9.3% for government, 12.7% for social services organizations such as schools and hospitals, 5.3% for military, and 10% for others.2 Procedure We asked participants to recall the detail of a recent performance appraisal in which they were being appraised. This incident provided the context for the participants to respond to the survey questions. Participants were asked to respond to items concerning the three independent variables and the dependent variable on six-point scales (1 - totally disagree and 6 - totally agree). Measurement Procedural justice. The scale developed by Colquitt (2001) was used, which contained seven procedural elements. Sample items included, “I was able to express my views and feelings during those procedures” and “Those procedures were applied consistently.” Cronbach’s alpha of the scale was .86. Leader Morality 17 Outcome favorability. Two items were used to measure outcome favorability: “Overall, I was satisfied with my performance appraisal results” and “The outcome was favorable to me.” Cronbach’s alpha of the two-item scale was .88. Leader morality. We assessed leader morality by eight items which covered the dimensions of leader morality used in Study 1. The sample items were: “The leader shirked responsibility (reverse)”, “The leader was jealous of subordinates (reverse)”, “The leader lived on promises” and “The leader worked as a good role model.” Cronbach’s alpha of the scale was .83. Dependent variable. The dependent variable was pride in organization, which was measured by four items drawn from scales designed to measure group pride (Tyler, Degoey and Smith, 1996) and scales designed to measure affective evaluations of organizations (O’Reilly & Chatman, 1986), including “I am proud to think of myself as a member of this organization” and “It would be hard to find another organization I would like as much to be a part of.” Cronbach’s alpha of the scale was .75. Results and Discussion Insert Table1 about here Table1 presents the correlations among variables, which show that the variables were moderately related. The highest correlation coefficient was .57, which is still lower than .75, the highest level suggested by Tsui, Ashford, Clair, and Xin (1995) that would result in a serious multicollinearity problem. Additionally, when regressing Pride in Organization on the three independent variables, the range of the variance inflation factors Leader Morality 18 was from 1.24 to 1.63, far less than the critical value of 10. Therefore, the multicollinearity problem was not serious in the study. Insert Table2 about here We conducted hierarchical regression to test our hypotheses. As suggested by Aiken and West (1991), the predictor variables (outcome favorability, procedural justice and leader morality) were firstly centered, and two-way and three-way interaction terms were formed by multiplying the corresponding centered predictors. The three main effects (leader morality, procedural justice and outcome favorability) were entered in the regression model in the first step, followed by the three two-way interactions, and finally by the three-way interaction. As showed in Table 2, the main effect of perceived leader morality was marginally significant in this study (hypothesis 1 was partially supported), Beta = .17, t (177) = 1.90, p < .1, and the key triple interaction among procedural justice, outcome favorability and leader morality was significant, △R2 = .02, F (1, 73) = 4.40, p < .05; Beta = -.22, t (173) = -2.10, p < .05. We followed the steps recommended by Aiken and West (1991) to probe the nature of the triple interaction. For respondents who reported high leader morality, the interaction between procedural justice and outcome favorability was significant, Beta = -.24, t (173) = -2.13, p < .05. High procedural justice compensated for the negative effect of unfavorable outcome, i.e., the effect of outcome favorability was significant (Beta = .42, t (173) = 2.84, p < .01) in the low-procedural-justice condition, but not in the high-procedural-justice condition. For those who reported low leader morality, the interaction was non-significant, Beta = .08, t (173) = .69, p > .1. Figure 2 illustrates the nature of the procedural justice x outcome favorability interaction in different conditions. Leader Morality 19 In summary, the results of Study 1 and Study 2 are consistent in providing strong support to our key hypothesis about the moderating role of perceived leader morality. Insert Figure 2 about here General Discussion Theoretical Analysis of the Moderating Effect of Leader Morality The consistent results from two different settings (personnel selection and performance appraisal), two different designs (outcome favorability, procedural justice and leader morality were either manipulated or measured), and two different dependent variables (attractiveness of organization and pride in organization) provided strong support to our key hypothesis. That is, fair procedures can reduce the negative impact of low outcome favorability when perceived leader morality is high, but not when leader morality is low. Our reasoning was based on the relational model. When leader morality is high, people accord more psychological importance to their relationship with the leader, and care more about the nature of the relationship. As a result, procedural justice exhibits a compensatory effect and reduces the negative effect of unfavorable outcomes. When leader morality is low, however, people are less concerned with their relationship with the leader, thus attenuating the compensatory effect of procedural justice. While our account of the moderating effect of leader morality is consistent with previous theoretical analysis and empirical findings, we do not want to rule out other plausible mechanisms. One such alternative account is concerned with the close association between leader morality and authenticity. Authenticity, a term used in authentic leadership theory, refers to the extent to which a leader is perceived as true to his/her self. Leader Morality 20 Authentic leadership is usually regarded as a construct that incorporates transformational and ethical leadership (e.g., Avolio, Gardner, Walumbwa, Luthans, & May, 2004). When transformational leadership is “true to self and others, it is characterized by high moral and ethical standards” (Bass & Steidlmeier, 1999, p.191). That is to say, authenticity can be augmented by leader morality (Fields, 2007), and authentic leaders tend to have more influence on followers because authenticity promotes their credibility in the eyes of followers (Fields, 2007; Kouzes & Posner, 2002). Thus, it is quite possible that when leader morality is high, the leader is also perceived as more authentic and his/her actions have more influence on followers, giving rise to the compensatory effect of procedural justice. On the contrary, when leader morality and hence authenticity is low, the influence of what the leader does (i.e., procedural justice) will be weak. As a result, even high procedural justice cannot compensate for the negative effect of unfavorable outcomes. To examine the validity of this reasoning, it would be worthwhile to take authenticity of leader into account in the future studies. Another plausible account of our findings is based on relative status, because leader morality may elicit thoughts about relative status. When viewing a leader as moral, followers may feel a larger gap in relative status vis-à-vis the leader. Chen, Brockner, and Greenberg (2003) found that procedural justice can reduce the negative effect of unfavorable outcomes for lower-status people, but not for higher-status people. Thus, the larger relative status that may be associated with leader morality may account for the pattern of results we obtained, and this possibility should also be evaluated in future research. Theoretical Implications of Findings There are at least four important theoretical implications of our findings. At a more general level, the present study extends recent thinking on the role of morality in the justice Leader Morality 21 domain. Although leader morality is an important topic in management, its importance in organizational justice has just received attention recently. Folger (2001) lamented that “we’ve continued the search for phenomena related to fairness, but we’ve lost sight of how fairness related to morality.” There have been recent calls by justice researchers for paying more attention to moral values (e.g., Cropanzano et al., 2003). Folger’s (2001) theory of deonance, Cropanzano et al.’s (2003) deontic justice, Cropanzano and Rupp’s (2002) deontological model, and Leung and Tong’s (2003) normative model of justice are recent attempts to connect justice research to theorizing in morality and business ethics. One commonality of these recent theoretical analyses is the attempt to explore the role of morality in understanding why people care about justice and how people respond to injustice. These different frameworks converge in the assumption that one impetus for acting fairly or responding negatively to injustice lies in people’s motivation to fulfill moral obligations independent of the consequences of their actions and outcomes. Our results provide a novel direction for extending these recent attempts to link justice and morality constructs, and suggest that morality provides a basis for people to decide whether they would identify with authority figures and groups. In our context, if leaders are seen as moral, people are willing to engage in long-term relationships with them, leading to an active role of procedural justice in compensating for negative outcomes. Low leader morality results in a transactional mode of interaction, in which instrumental concerns loom large and overshadow the importance of justice. We may extrapolate from our findings to argue that similar processes may operate when people face a group, such as the firm they work for, or their government, and their willingness to invest in the group depends on the perceived morality of the group. For instance, if a firm is seen as unethical, employees are likely to deemphasize justice issues, and their behaviors may be primarily Leader Morality 22 driven by the quality of their outcomes provided by the firm. It is important to explore the role of perceived morality in influencing the importance of justice at the group level. The second implication is specific to leadership research. Some researchers argued that leader morality is especially important in the Chinese cultural context (e.g., Farh & Cheng, 2000; Ling, Chia, & Fang, 2000). However, in the Global Leadership and Organizational Behavior Effectiveness (GLOBE) Project, researchers have identified several attributes reflecting integrity and morality that are important for outstanding leadership in all the cultures studied (Hartog et al., 1999). Given the universal importance of leader morality, we need to evaluate its impact on follower attitudes and behaviors. As described in the Introduction, prior research has primarily focused on the main effects of leader morality. Our results have pointed to a more intricate effect of leader morality: Its moderating effect on the importance of justice. Our results suggest that it is fruitful to explore how leader morality qualifies the impact of various antecedent variables that shape important job attitudes and behaviors. The third implication is concerned with moderators of the interaction between procedural justice and outcome favorability. As noted before, previous research involved only the attributes of self (e.g., self-construal, Brockner et al., 2000) or of a relationship (e.g., importance of relationship, Kwong & Leung, 2002). The present research pioneered the impact of an attribute of authority figures in an exchange relationship on the interactive relationship between procedural justice and outcome favorability. Echoing results obtained in the person perception tradition, which show that people’s attitude and behaviors toward others are influenced by perceived attributes of others (e.g., Bond & Forgas, 1984; Morris et al., 2004; Tse, Francis, & Walls, 1994), the present research brings this person perception perspective to organizational justice research. Our results suggest that perceived attributes of authority figures play a significant role in organizational justice phenomena, Leader Morality 23 and that this perspective opens up a new avenue for future research. For instance, other attributes of authority figures, such as some Big-Five personality factors (e.g., agreeableness and conscientiousness), which were found to be related to others’ associative and trusting intentions (Bond & Forgas, 1984), may also show moderating effects on effects of justice. As discussed above, perceived authenticity of the leader is another interesting moderator for the interaction between procedural justice and outcome favorability. Finally, our findings suggest an intriguing theoretical question of how trust relates to the interaction effect of procedural justice and outcome favorability. Previous theories and research suggest a mediating role of trust in the interaction. Fairness heuristic theory assumes that people use fair treatment as a proxy for interpersonal trust (Lind, 2001), and people are especially attentive and sensitive to fairness-relevant information when they have little information about the trustworthiness of an authority figure (Van den Bos, Wike, & Lind, 1998). Brockner et al. (1997) showed in three studies that the interaction between trust and outcome favorability mediated the interaction between procedural justice and outcome favorability, and concluded that trust elicited by procedural justice, but not procedural justice per se, may drive its interaction with outcome favorability to influence people’s attitudes and behaviors. In contrast, given that leader morality is likely to elicit trusting reactions from followers (e.g., Morgan, 1989), our findings suggest that trustworthiness of an authority figure may also play a moderating role in the interaction between procedural justice and outcome favorability. It would be interesting to explore the conditions under which the mediating or the moderating role of trust in the interaction between procedural justice and outcome favorability would be operative. In other words, future research needs to examine in more depth when people’s judgment of a leader’s trustworthiness is derived from perceived procedural justice (i.e., the mediating role), and Leader Morality 24 when people use the trustworthiness of a leader to determine the importance of their relationship with the leader, which gives rise to a moderating role of trust in influencing the compensatory effect of procedural justice. Limitations and Suggestions for Future Research Although our studies yielded consistent findings with important implications, they do have some limitations that need to be addressed. In calling attention to these limitations, we also provide some suggestions for future research. Firstly, the use of scenarios in Study 1 created a hypothetical setting for the participants, and the results obtained lacked external validity. Although Study 2 corrected this weakness by focusing on a real performance appraisal incident that participants had encountered, it is useful to employ behavioral simulations to replicate Study 1 in the future. The measure of actual behavior will strengthen the external validity of the results in Study 2, while retaining the causal inferences that an experimental study provides. Secondly, all the independent and dependent variables were measured simultaneously and from the same source in Study 2, resulting in the problem of common method bias. We do not believe that the results obtained were caused by common method variance for two reasons. First, in Study 1, the independent variables were manipulated, and the results were free from the common method problem. The similarity of the results across the two studies argues against a common method explanation. Second, a Monte Carlo study conducted by Evans (1985) showed that artificial interaction cannot be created by correlated errors produced by common method variance. In fact, correlated errors function to reduce the chance of obtaining a significant interaction effect, which suggests that common method variance makes it harder for significant interaction effects to emerge. In any event, it is always useful to replicate our results with data from different sources to settle the suspicion based on common method variance. Leader Morality 25 Thirdly, the dependent variables of the two studies pertain to responses to organizations. Previous research showed that the nature of dependent variables may impact the pattern of the interaction between procedural justice and outcome favorability (e.g., Brockner, Heuer, Magner, Folger, Umphress, Van den Bos, et al., 2003). Whether the nature of the dependent variables also influences the role of leader morality in the compensatory effect of procedural justice is definitely worth exploring. Brockner et al. (2003) argued that if a dependent variable is concerned with employees’ self-evaluation, self-attribution would arise from procedural justice perception, which will heighten the effect of outcome favorability. It is unclear what role leader morality will play with regard to this type of dependent variables. In general, other dependent variables should be included in gauging the effects of leader morality on the interaction effect of procedural justice and outcome favorability in the future research. Finally, there may be some concerns with the characteristics of the participants in the two studies. For example, there were more females than males in Study 2, and Sweeney and McFarlin (1997) suggested that gender differences exist in the importance that people place on distributive and procedural justice. To rule out the potential bias caused by gender, we added gender and two other demographic variables (age and educational level) into the hierarchical regression and found that the main effects of the demographic variables were all insignificant and the key triple interaction was still significant3. Another concern is that the participants of the two studies were either full-time or part-time students from a single university. However, participants of Study 2 were from diverse backgrounds. We believe that the basic processes examined in our research should not be drastically affected by the background of the participants. It is of course necessary to replicate our results with participants recruited from other contexts. A related issue is that our participants were Chinese, and it is also useful to evaluate our findings in other cultural contexts, although Leader Morality 26 Leung (2004) concluded after a comprehensive review that basic justice processes are quite robust across cultural boundaries. Leader Morality 27 References Adams, J. S. (1965). Inequity in social exchange. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (vol. 2, pp.267-199). New York: Academic Press. Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Ambrose, M. L., & Cropanzano, R. (2003). A longitudinal analysis of organizational fairness: An examination of reactions to tenure and promotion decisions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 266-275. Avolio, B. J., Gardner, W., L., Walumbwa, F. O., Luthans, F., & May, D. R. (2004). Unlocking the mask: A look at the process by which authentic leaders impact follower attitudes and behaviors. The Leadership Quarterly, 15, 801-823. Bass, B. M., & Steidlmeier, P. (1999). Ethics, character, and authentic transformational leadership behavior. The Leadership Quarterly, 10, 181-217. Black, S., Morrison, A., & Gregersen, H. (1999). Global explorers: The next generation of leaders. New York: Routledge. Bond, M. H., & Forgas, J. P. (1984). Linking person perception to behavior intention across cultures: The role of cultural collectivism. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 15, 337-352. Brass, D. J., Butterfield, K. D., & Skaggs, B. C. (1998). Relationship and unethical behavior: A social network perspective. Academy of Management Review, 23, 1431. Leader Morality 28 Brockner, J. (2002). Making sense of procedural fairness: How high procedural fairness can reduce or heighten the influence of outcome favorability. Academy of Management Review, 27, 58-76. Brockner, J., Siegel, P. A., Daly, J. P., Tyler, J. P., & Martin, C. (1997). When trust matter: The moderating effect of outcome favorability. Administrative Science Quarterly, 42, 558-583. Brockner, J., & Wiesenfeld, B. M. (1996). An integrative framework for explaining reactions to decision: The interactive effects of outcomes and procedures. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 189-208. Brockner, J., Chen, Y. R., Mannix, E. A., Leung, K., & Skarlicki, D. R. (2000). Culture and procedural fairness: When the effects of what you do depend on how you do it. Administrative Science Quarterly, 45, 138-159. Brockner, J., Heuer, L., Magner, N., Folger, R., Umphress, E., van den Bos, K. et al. (2003). High procedural fairness heightens the effect of outcome favorability on self-evaluations: An attributional analysis. Organizational Behavior & Human Decision Processes, 91, 51-68. Brower, H. H., Schoorman, F. D., & Tan, H. H. (2000). A model of relational leadership: The integration of trust and leader-member exchange. Leader Quarterly, 11, 227250. Brown, M., & Benson, J. (2003). Rated to exhaustion? Reactions to performance appraisal processes. Industrial Relations Journal, 34, 67-81. Brown, M. E., Treviño, L. K., & Harrison, D. A. (2005). Ethical leadership: Asocial learning perspective for construct development and testing. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 97, 117-134. Leader Morality 29 Chen, Y. R., Brockner, J., & Greenberg, J. (2003). When is it "a pleasure to do business with you?" The effects or relative status, outcome favorability, and procedural fairness. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 92, 1-21. Cheng, B. S., Chou, L. F., Wu, T. Y., Huang, M. P., & Farh, J. L. (2004). Paternalistic leadership and subordinate responses: Establishing a leadership model in Chinese organizations. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 7, 89-117. Cobb, A. T., & Frey, F. M. (1996). The effects of leader fairness and pay outcomes on superior/subordinate relations. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 26, 14011426. Colquitt, J. A. (2001). On the dimensionality of organizational Justice: A construct validation of a measure. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 386-400. Colquitt, J. A., Conlon, D. E., Wesson, M. J., Porter, C. O. L. H., & Ng, K. Y. (2001). Justice at the millennium: A meta-analytic review of 25 years of organizational justice research. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 425-445. Craig, S. B., & Gustafson, S. B. (1998). Perceived leader integrity scale: An instrument for assessing employee perceptions of leader integrity. Leadership Quarterly, 9, 127145. Cremer, D. D., & Knippenberg, D. V.(2003). Cooperation with leaders in social dilemmas: On the effects of procedural fairness and outcome favorability in structural cooperation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 91, 1-11. Cropanzano, R., & Rupp, D. E. (2002). Some reflections on the morality of organizational justice. In S. W. Gilliland, D. D. Steiner, & D. P. Skarlicki (Eds.), Emerging perspectives on managing organizational justice (pp. 225-278). Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishers. Leader Morality 30 Cropanzano, R., Goldman, B., & Folger, R. (2003). Deontic justice: The role of moral principles in workplace fairness. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 24, 10191024. Dulebohn, J. H., & Martocchio, J. J. (1998). Employee perceptions of the fairness of work incentive pay plans. Journal of Management, 24, 469-488. Evans, M. G. (1985). A Monte Carlo study of the effects of correlated method variance in moderated multiple regression analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 36, 305-323. Farh, J. L., & Cheng, B. S. (2000). A cultural analysis of paternalistic leadership in Chinese organizations. In J. L. Li, A. S. Tsui, & E. Weldon (Eds.), Management and organizations in the Chinese Context (pp. 84-130). London: Macmillan. Fields, D. L. (2007). Determinants of follower perceptions of a leader’s authenticity and integrity. European Management Journal, 25, 195-206. Folger, R. (2001). Fairness as deonance. In S. Gilliland, D. Steiner, & D. Skarlicki (Eds.), Theoretical and cultural perspectives on organizational justice (pp. 3-33). Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing. Gilliland, S. W. (1994). Effects of procedural and distributive justice on reactions to a selection system. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 691-701. Hartog, D. N. D., House, R. J., Hanges, P. J., Ruiz-Quintanilla, S. A., Dorfman, P. W., & Abdalla, I. A., et al. (1999). Culture specific and cross-culturally generalizable implicit leadership theories: Are attributes of charismatic/transformational leadership universally endorsed? Leadership Quarterly, 10, 219-256. Hsu, W. L., Cheng, B. S., Huang, M. P., & Farh, J. L. (2004). Moral leadership in Taiwanese organizations: Developing the construct and the measurement. Paper Leader Morality 31 presented at the 1st Annual conference of International Association of Chinese Management Research, Beijing, China. Kahn, W. A. (1990). Toward an agenda for business ethics research. Academy of Management Review, 15, 311-328. Kanungo, R. N., & Mendonca, M. (1996). Ethical dimensions of leadership. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. Koh, H. C., & Boo, E. H. Y. (2004). Organisational ethics and employee satisfaction and commitment. Management Decision, 42, 677-693. Kouzes, J. M., & Posner, B. Z. (2002). The leadership challenge. San Francisco: JosseyBass. Kwong, J. Y. Y., & Leung, K. (2002). A moderator of the interaction effect of procedural justice and outcome favorability: Importance of the relationship. Organizational Behavior & Human Decision Processes, 87, 278-299. Law, K. S., Lee, C., Farh, J. L., & Pillutola, M. (2001). Organizational justice perceptions of employees in the PRC: A grounded investigation. International Conference of the Global Business and Technology Association, Istanbul, Turkey. Leung, K. (2004). How generalizable are justice effects across cultures? In J. Greenberg, & J. A. Colquitt (Eds.), Handbook of organizational justice (pp. 555-586). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Leung, K., & Tong, K. K. (2003). Toward a normative model of justice. In S. W. Gilliland, D. D. Steiner, D. P. Skarlicki (Eds.), Emerging perspectives on values in organizations (pp. 107-120). Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishers Leader Morality 32 Lind, E. A. (2001). Fairness heuristic theory: Justice judgments as pivotal cognitions in organizational relations. In J. Greenberg & R. Cropanzano (Eds.), Advance in Organizational Justice (pp. 56-88). Stanford, California: Stanford University Press. Lind, E. A., & Earley, P. C. (1992). Procedural justice and culture. International Journal of Psychology, 27, 227-242. Lind, E. A., & Lissak, R. I. (1985). Apparent impropriety and procedural fairness judgments. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 21, 19-29. Lind, E. A., & Tyler, T. R. (1988). The social psychology of procedure justice. New York: Plenum Press. Ling, W., Chia, R. C., & Fang, L. (2000). Chinese implicit leadership theory. Journal of Social Psychology, 140, 729-739. Morris, M. W., Leung, K., & Iyengar, S. S. (2004). Person perception in the heat of conflict: Negative trait attributions affect procedural preferences and account for situational and cultural differences. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 7, 127-147. Morgan, R. B. (1989). Reliability and validity of a factor analytically derived measure of leadership behavior and characteristics. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 49, 911-919. O’Reilly, C. A., & Chatman, J. A. (1986). Organizational commitment and psychological attachment: The effects of compliance, identification, and internalization on prosocial behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 71, 492-499. Parry, K. W., & Proctor-Thomson, S. B. (2002). Perceived integrity of transformational leaders in organizational settings. Journal of Business Ethics, 35, 75-96. Leader Morality 33 Ployhart, R. E., & Ryan, A. M. (1998). Applicants' reactions to the fairness of selection procedures: The effects of positive rule violations and time of measurement. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 3-16. Schiebel, W., & Pochtrager, S. (2003). Corporate ethics as a factor for success – The measurement instrument of the University of Agricultural Sciences (BOKU). Supply Chain Management, 8, 116-121. Schiminke, M., Ambrose, M. L., Neubaum, D. O. (2005). The effect of leader moral development on ethical climate and employee attitudes. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Process, 97, 135-151. Spreitzer, G. M., & Mishra, A. K. (2002). To stay or to go: Voluntary survivor turnover following an organizational downsizing. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 23, 707-729. Sweeney, P. D., & McFarlin, D. B. (1993). Workers’ evaluation of the “ends” and the “means”: An examination of four models of distributive and procedural justice. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Process, 55, 23-40. Sweeney, P. D., & McFarlin, D. B. (1997). Process and outcome: gender differences in the assessment of justice. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 18, 83-98. Thibaut, J., & Walker, L. (1975). Procedural justice: A psychological analysis. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Treviño, L. K., Brown, M., & Hartman, L. P. (2003). A qualitative investigation of perceived executive ethical leadership: Perceptions from inside and outside the executive suite. Human Relations, 56, 5-37. Leader Morality 34 Tse, D. K., Francis, J., & Walls, J. (1994). Cultural differences in conducting intra- and inter- cultural negotiations: A Sino-Canadian comparison. Journal of International Business Studies, 25, 537-555. Tsui, A. S., Ashford, S. J., Clair, L., & Xin, K. R. (1995). Dealing with discrepant expectations: Response strategies and managerial effectiveness. Academy of Management Journal, 38, 1515-1543. Tyler, T. R. (1989). The psychology of procedural justice: A test of the group-value model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 830-838. Tyler, T. R., & De Cremer, D. (2005). Process-based leadership: Fair procedures and reactions to organizational change. The Leader Quarterly, 16, 529-545. Tyler, T. R., & Lind, E. A. (1992). A relational model of authority in groups. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 25, pp. 115-191). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Tyler, T. R., Degoey, P., & Smith, H. (1996). Understanding why the justice of group procedures matters: A test of the psychological dynamics of group-value model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 913-930. Van den Bos, K. (1999). What are we talking when we talk about no-voice procedures? On the psychology of the fair outcome effect. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 560-577. Van den Bos, K., Vermunt, R., & Wilke, H. A. M. (1997). Procedural and distributive justice: What is fair depends more on what comes first than on what comes next. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 95-104. Leader Morality 35 Van den Bos, K., Wilker, H. A. M., & Lind, E. A. (1998). When do we need procedural fairness? The role of trust in authority. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1449-1458. Welbourne, T. M. (1998). Untangling procedural and distributive justice: Their relative effects on gainsharing satisfaction. Group & Organization Management, 23, 325-346. Leader Morality 36 Footnotes 1 Besides the main effects of leader morality, procedural justice and outcome favorability on their corresponding judgments, we also found significant main effects of leader morality, F(1, 88) = 5.34, p < .05, and outcome favorability, F(1, 88) = 11.16, p < .01, on judgment of procedural justice, and main effects of outcome favorability, F(1, 88) = 10.73, p < .01, and procedural justice, F(1, 88) = 10.92, p < .01, on judgment of leader morality. However, these four main effects did not qualify the effectiveness of our manipulations, because previous justice literature also suggests that outcome favorability may influence judgment about procedural justice, and vice versa (e.g., Lind & Lissak, 1985). We also found a significant interaction between leader morality and outcome favorability on the judgment of outcome favorability, F(1, 88) = 8.04, p < .01, which showed that the outcome favorability manipulation had a stronger effect on participants’ judgment of outcome favorability in the high than in the low leader-morality condition. However, despite this interaction, the effectiveness of the outcome favorability manipulation was clearly confirmed in each of the two levels of leader morality. 2 When collecting the data, we didn’t ask the participants to specify the type of industry they worked in and their position. However, according to the demographic information provided when students registered for the program, the three largest groups were: finance, insurance, real estate or business services (25.3%), Community, social, or personal services (23.7%), and transportation, storage, and communication (18.9%). With regard to rank, 36.7 percent were at the supervisory level or above, and the rest were at non-supervisory levels. 3 When demographic variables (gender, age, and educational level) were added in the hierarchical regression in Study 2, the main effects of the demographic variables were all nonsignificant (p > .1), and the key triple interaction of procedural justice, outcome Leader Morality 37 favorability and leader morality was still significant, △R2= .02, F(1,170) = 4.14, p < .05. We further tested the interaction effects between gender and the three independent variables, and found that all the three interaction effects were not significant. In addition, including both demographic variables and the three interaction terms involving gender and the three independent variables as control variables in the regression model did not change the pattern of our main findings, except that the key three-way interaction effect became marginally significant (Beta = .20, t = -1.88, p = .06). Leader Morality 38 Table 1 Correlations and Reliabilities of Variables for Study 2 Variable Mean SD 1 1. Procedural justice 3.71 1.23 .86 2. Leader morality 4.03 1.08 .42** .83 3. Outcome favorability 3.88 1.40 .57** .36** .88 4. Pride in organization 3.67 1.08 .36** .29** .39** Note. Coefficient alphas are given on the diagonal. **Correlation is significant at the .01 level (2-tailed). 2 3 4 .75 Leader Morality 39 Table 2 Hierarchical Regression Results for Study 2 Pride in Organization Beta t Step 1 Procedural justice (PJ) .17 1.90+ Leader morality (LM) .13 1.77+ Outcome favorability (OF) .24 2.80** OF ╳ LM .08 .84 OF ╳ PJ - .08 - 1.03 PJ ╳ LM - .02 - .25 -.22 -2.10* R2= .187 F(3,177) = 13.54*** Step 2 △R2= .006 F(3,174) = .45 Step 3 OF ╳ LM ╳ PJ △R2= .02 F(1,173) = 4.40* Note. + p<.1, * p<.05, ** p<.01. Leader Morality 40 Figure Captions Figure 1. Interaction of leader morality, procedural justice, and outcome favorability for Study 1. Figure 2. Interaction of leader morality, procedural justice, and outcome favorability for Study 2. Morality and Justice 41 attractiveness High leader-morality condition 5.06 5.5 5 4.5 4 3.5 3 2.5 2 1.5 4.36 4.33 Low outcome favorability High outcome favorability 2.00 Low High Procedural justice attractiveness Low leader-morality condition 5.5 5 4.5 4 3.5 3 2.5 2 1.5 3.58 3.04 High outcome favorability 2.64 2.64 Low High Procedural justice Figure 1. Low outcome favorability Morality and Justice 42 Pride High leader-morality condition 5.5 5 4.5 4 3.5 3 2.5 2 1.5 4.07 4.22 4.17 3.27 Low outcome favorability High outcome favorability Low High Procedural justice Pride Low leader-morality condition 5.5 5 4.5 4 3.5 3 2.5 2 1.5 3.46 3.03 3.41 2.76 Low High Procedural justice Figure 2. Low outcome favorability High outcome favorability