The Influence of Perfectionism on Procrastination in Online Graduate Education Students Glenda Rakes The University of Tennessee at Martin United States grakes@utm.edu Karee Dunn The University of Arkansas United States kedunn@uark.edu Abstract: Both perfectionism and procrastination are prevalent among online students and detrimental to student performance and possibly to persistence in a degree program. It is important for online faculty to identify factors that may reduce students’ tendency to procrastinate. Because procrastination can lead to decreased academic performance, it is important to better understand the influence students’ perfectionism has on procrastination. The results indicated that higher levels of procrastination were likely to occur if a student described him or herself as being more of a perfectionist. Older students also appeared to also be at a greater risk for procrastination. Suggestions are provided for working with perfectionistic students, especially older perfectionistic students, to help reduce procrastination. Introduction Course offerings and university degree programs offered online have increased at a rapid rate in recent years. Asynchronous programs have also increased dramatically with the advances in technology. By definition, asynchronous online programs separate teacher and student physically, give students unprecedented freedom concerning the management of their time, allow students freedom in choosing where learning takes place (as opposed to regularly scheduled appearances in a physical classroom), and place more responsibility on students for their own learning with the role of the teacher moving towards that of facilitator. These changes in the learning environment increase opportunities for procrastination. Both perfectionism and procrastination have been shown to cause problems for many students, but the problems can be more prevalent in online students and online graduate students in particular. With knowledge of factors that influence procrastination, faculty can help students mitigate this behavior. Thus, the purpose of this project was to explore the influence of perfectionism and age on procrastination. Academic procrastination Schraw (2007) defines academic procrastination as “intentionally delaying or deferring work that must be completed” (p. 12). Steel (2007) discusses the occasional use of the term procrastination to describe its use as a positive, strategic behavior. He describes this use of the term by some as “functional delay” (p. 66). However, in his meta-analysis of the procrastination literature, Steel confirms that such usage is secondary to the traditional use of the term as passive, negative behavior. The use of the term in the present study refers to the traditionally used form of the term procrastination. Research indicates that procrastination can be a detriment to academic achievement because it restricts both the quality and quantity of work produced by a student. Procrastination is described as the quintessential failure of self-regulation (Steel, 2007). It leads to a number of negative results, including a decrease in long-term learning. Students who procrastinate tend to express dissatisfaction with their courses (McCown and Johnson, 1991) and receive lower grades than non-procrastinators (Rothblum, Solomon, & Murakami, 1986, Tice & Baumeister, 1997). Despite the seemingly obvious consequences of this behavior, it is estimated that 80-95% of college students procrastinate (Ellis & Knaus, 1977; O’Brien, 2002). Graduate students in one study demonstrated an even greater tendency to procrastinate on academic tasks at a rate of up to 3.5 times that of a comparison group of undergraduate students (Onwuegbuzie, 2004). In face-to-face courses, the requirement to attend class meetings forces students to regularly focus on course materials. At least part of students’ attention to course materials is distributed equally across the semester (Elvers, Polzella, & Graetz, 2003). Online students in asynchronous online courses do not participate in regular class meetings, so there is an increased temptation to procrastinate, often resulting in frustration and reduced learning. Procrastination and perfectionism Perfectionism is characterized by striving for flawlessness and extremely high standards for performance along with the tendency to be overly critical of one’s behavior (Flett & Hewitt, 2002). Perfectionist beliefs play a role in procrastination. Many believe that perfectionists and procrastinators share important characteristics. Both have high standards (Burke & Yuen, 1983). In addition, both types of individuals fear making mistakes (Solomon & Rothblum, 1984; Flett et al., 1991). Researchers suggest that many people exhibit procrastination because they are perfectionists (Burke & Yuen, 1983; Onwuegbuzie, 2000) and that the percentage of individuals who exhibit both behaviors is higher among the graduate student population (Onwuegbuzie, 2000). Perfectionists often impose extremely high standards on themselves and procrastinate because they do not believe that they can meet those standards. Procrastinators are often unable to complete tasks because they focus excessively on the standards set for them and focus too much over how others will evaluate them (Ferrari, 1989). For these reasons, there is believed to be a strong relationship between perfectionism and academic procrastination (Onwuegbuzie, 2000). The authors hypothesized that procrastination may be influenced by perfectionism as well as age. The Study There is some research demonstrating positive associations between procrastination and perfectionism with undergraduate students in face-to-face course environments. Results of these studies have been mixed. There is also some indication that both perfectionism and procrastination is more prevalent in older students. Because the relationship between perfectionism and procrastination has not been thoroughly researched in the online population and has mixed results in the face-to-face classroom, this study addressed the following research question: Do perfectionism and participant age significantly influence procrastination as measured by Procrastination Assessment Scale-Students (PASS)? Participants The sample for this study consisted of 79 fully admitted graduate students enrolled in an asynchronous online masters program in education an accredited mid-southern university that grants bachelors and masters degrees. Participants were predominantly female (84%), which is typical of education majors, and ranged in age from 21 to 46 years of age (M = 34.57). Participants’ experience with online courses varied from one course to 21 (M = 8.13). Procedures All measures were prepared for presentation online. No identifying information was collected. All responses were voluntary and anonymous. Participants were invited to participate via email and were asked to complete the questionnaire. Measures Almost Perfect Scale-Revised. The APS-R is a 23-item scale that measures the tendency toward perfectionism and includes two subscales (i.e., Discrepancy and High Standards) (Slaney et al., 2001). The APS-R utilizes a 7-point Likert scale (i.e., 1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree). The Discrepancy subscale (12 items) estimates the degree to which individuals perceive discrepancy between their expectations and actual performance. The High Standards subscale (7 items) captures the degree to which individuals tend to set up a high standard (or have a high expectation) for their own performance. The High Standards subscale was used for this study and met criteria for internal consistency with a Cronbach’s alpha of .80. Procrastination Assessment Scale-Students. The PASS is a widely used scale to measure academic procrastination (Ferrari, Johnson, & McCown, 1995). It is a measure containing 44-items that measures the prevalence of academic procrastination and the reasons for procrastination. Respondents were asked to describe their behavior for specific academic tasks. Respondents answered the questions for each academic task using a 5-point Likert scale for two questions: “To what degree do you procrastinate on this task?” (1 = “Never Procrastinate” to 5 = “Always Procrastinate”) and “To what degree is procrastination on this task a problem for you?” (1 = “Not at all a problem” to 5 = “Always a problem”). Cronbach’s alpha for the PASS was .89; thus, the measure was deemed reliable. The prevalence of procrastination subscale was used for the present study. Data analysis In order to examine the influence of Perfectionism and age on the variance in PASS (prevalence) scores, the data were analyzed using linear multiple regression. The PASS (prevalence) score was entered as the dependent variable and the Perfectionism (High Standards) score and participant age were entered as the predictor variables. The significance and size of the coefficient of determination were examined to determine if the independent variables had a significant influence on procrastination. Additionally, the magnitude of impact for each independent variable was examined and interpreted. Findings Preliminary examination of the results indicated there was no extreme multicollinearity in the data (all variance inflation factors were less than 2). Exploratory analysis also indicated that the assumptions underlying the application of multiple linear regression (independence, normality, heteroschedasticity, and linearity) were met. Variable means, standard deviation, and the correlation matrix are presented in Table 3. The regression results (see Table 1) indicated that the set of independent variables significantly influenced 13.9% of the variance in perfectionism (F (2, 76) = 6.14; p < .01). Cohen’s f2 measured effect size and was interpreted as approaching moderate strength at 0.16, 95% CI [-.03,2.59] (Cohen, 1988). Both independent variables uniquely and significantly contributed to the explained variance. In descending order of importance they were age (t = 2.54, p < .05) and perfectionism (t = 2.00, p < .05). Table 1 Results of Regression of Procrastination on Perfectionism and Age Variable b Beta Partial t Perfectionism .21 .22 .22 0.81* Age .02 .27 .28 1.30* Note. * p < .05, R2 = 0.139. Conclusions The results indicated that for this group of students, higher levels of procrastination were likely to occur if a student described him or herself as being more of a perfectionist (r = .26). Older students also appeared to also be at a greater risk for procrastination (r = .31). In the future, more data will be collected in order to more fully test this model using structural equation modeling. Based on the results it appears that steps need to be taken, especially with older more perfectionistic students, to help students reduce procrastination. Research indicates that procrastination may be difficult to change (Wohl, Pychyl, & Bennett, 2010), but some researchers have been successful in reducing procrastination through instructional design. For example, Tuckman (2007) found that utilizing motivational scaffolding decreased procrastination in online learners. Motivational scaffolding included features designed to increase student motivation and collaboration. Additionally, because older adults in this study tended to procrastinate more, it may be important to consider the additional time demands older students face as a result of family, work, and social life. For example, Skibba (2009) found that many online instructors reported that using flexible due dates helped busy working adults achieve higher levels of academic success. With regard to the impact of perfectionism on online graduate students, faculty can support perfectionistic students by building a supportive learning environment and presenting themselves as concerned more with student learning than criticizing performance (Brophy & Rohrkemper, 1989). Online instructors need to be cognizant of the problems caused by both perfectionism and procrastination when designing courses and interacting with students as research does indicate that procrastination can be ameliorated through instructional design (Tuckman, 2007; Skibba, 2009) and perfectionism can be positively influenced by instructor support (Brophy & Rohrkemper, 1989). References Brophy, J., and M. Rohrkemper. (1989). Teachers’ strategies for coping with perfectionist students. Research Series No. 198. East Lansing, MI: Institute for Research on Teaching, Michigan State University. ED 314 401. Burke, J. B., & Yuen, L. M. (1983). Procrastination: Why you do it, what to do about it. Reading, MA: AddisonWesley. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Ellis, A., and Knaus, W. J. (1977). Overcoming procrastination. New York: Signet Books. Elvers, G. C., Polzella, D. J., & Graetz, K. (2003). Procrastination in online courses: Performance and attitudinal differences. Teaching of Psychology, 30(2), 159-162. Ferrari, J.R. (1989). Reliability of academic and dispositional measures of procrastination. Psychological Reports, 64, 1057–1058. Flett, G. L., & Hewitt, P. L. (2002). Perfectionism and maladjustment: An overview of theoretical, definitional, and treatment issues. In P. L. Hewitt & G. L. Flett (Eds.), perfectionism (pp. 5-31). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Flett, G.L., Hewitt, P.L., Blankstein, K. R. & Koledin, S. (1991). Dimensions of perfectionism and Irrational Thinking. Journal of Rational-Emotive and Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy, 9, 185-201. McCown, W., & Johnson, J. (1991). Personality and chronic procrastination by university students during an academic examination period. Personality and Individual Differences, 12, 413-415. O'Brien, W. K. (2002). Applying the transtheoretical model to academic procrastination. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Houston. Onwuegbuzie, A. J. (2000). Academic procrastinators and perfectionistic tendencies among graduate students. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 15(5), 103-110. Onwuegbuzie, A. J. (2004). Academic procrastination and statistics anxiety. Assessment and Evaluation in Higher Education, 29, 3-19. Rothblum, E. D., Solomon, L. J., & Murakami, J. (1986). Affective, cognitive, and behavioral differences between high and low procrastinators. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 33, 387-394. Slaney, R., Rice, K., Mobley, M., Trippi, J., & Ashby, J. (2001). The revised almost perfect scale. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development, 34, 130–145. Tice, D. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (1997). Longitudinal study of procrastination, performance, stress, and health: The costs and benefits of dawdling. Psychological Science, 8, 454–458. Tuckman, B. W. (2007). The effect of motivational scaffolding on procrastinator’s distance learning outcomes. Computers and Education, 49, 414-422. Shraw, G., Wadkins, T., & Olafson, L. (2007). Doing the things we do: A grounded theory of academic procrastination. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99(1), 12-25. Skibba, K. (2009). What faculty learn teaching adults in multiple course delivery formats. Presented at the TwentyFifth Annual Conference on Distance Teaching and Learning; Madison, WI. Solomon, L. J., & Rothblum, E. D. (1984). Academic procrastination: Frequency and cognitive-behavioral correlates. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 31(4), 503-509. Steel, P. (2007). The nature of procrastination: A meta-analytic and theoretical review of quintessential self- regulatory failure. Psychological Bulletin, 133(1), 65-94. Wohl, M. J. A., Pychyl, T. A., & Bennett, S. H. (2010). I forgive myself, now I can study: How self-forgiveness for procrastinating can reduce future procrastination. Personality and Individual Differences, 48, 803-808.