JOURNAL OF RESEARCH IN SCIENCE TEACHING VOL. 41, NO. 9, PP. 936–960 (2004) An Investigation of Experienced Secondary Science Teachers’ Beliefs About Inquiry: An Examination of Competing Belief Sets Carolyn S. Wallace,1 Nam-Hwa Kang2 1 Science Education Department, University of Georgia, 212 Aderhold Hall, Athens, Georgia 30602-7126 2 Curriculum and Instruction, College of Education, University of Nevada, 4505 Maryland Parkway, Las Vegas, Nevada 89154 Received 4 February 2003; Accepted 23 February 2004 Abstract: The purpose of this study was to investigate the beliefs of six experienced high school science teachers about (1) what is successful science learning; (2) what are the purposes of laboratory in science teaching; and (3) how inquiry is implemented in the classroom. An interpretive multiple case study with an ethnographic orientation was used. The teachers’ beliefs about successful science learning were substantively linked to their beliefs about laboratory and inquiry implementation. For example, two teachers who believed that successful science learning was deep conceptual understanding, used verification labs primarily to illustrate these concepts and used inquiry as a type of isolated problem-solving experience. Another teacher who believed that successful science learning was enculturation into scientific practices used inquirybased labs extensively to teach the practices of science. Tension in competing beliefs sets and implications for reform are discussed. © 2004 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. J Res Sci Teach 41: 936– 960, 2004 Those interested in promoting reform in science education are faced with a difficult paradox: We recognize the crucial role of teacher expertise and practical knowledge in promoting reform- based curriculum (Richardson, 1996; Keys & Bryan, 2001; Anderson & Helms, 2001; van Driel, Beijaard, & Verloop, 2001), yet research indicates that science teachers are reluctant to make meaningful reforms within the complex culture of schools (Cronin-Jones, 1991; Duffee & Aikenhead, 1992; Tobin & McRobbie, 1996; Yerrick, Parke, & Nugent, 1997; Huberman & Middlebrooks, 2000; Munby, Cunningham & Lock, 2000; van Driel et al., 2001). Approximately two decades of research has indicated that teachers are creative, intelligent decision makers with well-established beliefs about the needs of their students and their own roles in the context of Correspondence to: C.S. Wallace; E-mail: wallacec@uga.edu DOI 10.1002/tea.20032 Published online 11 October 2004 in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com). © 2004 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. EXPERIENCED SECONDARY SCIENCE TEACHERS’ BELIEFS ABOUT INQUIRY 937 schooling (Wildy & Wallace, 1995). Many teachers readily participate in state and national professional development opportunities and have collaborated enthusiastically with university- based researchers to improve their own science learning and teaching. However, a set of cultural beliefs (Tobin & McRobbie, 1996; Munby et al., 2000; Keys & Bryan, 2001), such as, the need for examination preparation and the compartmentalized nature of science, permeates school science culture, becomes internalized by teachers, and mediates the implementation of innovative practice. During 1999–2001, we investigated the beliefs of experienced teachers who have been enacting what they believe to be inquiry-based science teaching in their own classrooms. Our goal was to frame a series of case studies for inquiry beliefs to gain a better understanding of teachers’ professional practical knowledge about inquiry. Our rationale to study experienced teachers with an interest in inquiry, rather than exemplary practitioners of inquiry was threefold. First, we wanted to explore the possibilities and limitations of inquiry in typical high school classrooms. We questioned how teachers would respond to inquiry in the context of school culture with all its ordinary constraints; thus, we focused on teachers that taught typical high school classes and did not have special permission to deviate from the mandated curriculum. Second, we were interested in illuminating the roles of competing belief sets, specifically related to the construct of inquiry, and how teachers come to highlight one belief set over another in particular situations. We reasoned that teachers who had not already moved to a total adoption of inquiry-based methods might better exhibit these competing belief sets. Third, we attempted to move beyond a description of teacher beliefs and practices of inquiry towards an explanation of why teachers practice the way they do. Examining teachers with a range of inquiry practices could provide information that could contribute to theoretical knowledge on teacher practice. There have been a series of studies describing how teacher beliefs about students, learning, teaching, and the nature of science impact teaching practices and form barriers to the implementation of reform-oriented curricula (Brickhouse, 1990; CroninJones, 1991; Gallagher, 1991; Tobin & McRobbie, 1996; Munby et al., 2000). These studies have been extremely valuable in characterizing the relationship between beliefs and practice. With the present study, we sought to explain the teachers’ actions about inquiry in terms of their own goals and purposes of instruction, as well as their epistemologies for how students learn science. Highlighting teachers’ constructed definitions of inquiry and the purposes for which they use it will provide keys to understanding under which belief conditions teachers may be more apt to adopt inquiry-based approaches. Therefore, this study contributes to the knowledge base for professional development and reform. The research questions for the within-case analyses included: (1) What are teachers’ beliefs about the nature of successful science learning? (2) What are teachers’ beliefs about inquiry-based teaching and learning?, and (3) What are the relationships among the elements of the teachers’ beliefs systems as indicated both through espoused beliefs and through classroom practices? The within-case analyses are presented as a series of belief profiles for each of the six participants. Theoretical Frameworks Teacher Beliefs and Practical Knowledge We draw on the theoretical constructs of both teacher beliefs and practical knowledge. There is a strong literature base for applying the construct of teacher beliefs to research on inquiry. Reviewing the literature, Pajares (1992) asserted that beliefs are ‘‘the best indicators of the decisions individuals make throughout their lives’’ (p. 307). Beliefs structures play a major role in 938 WALLACE AND KANG teacher decision making about curriculum and instructional tasks (Nespor, 1987; Pajares, 1992; Richardson, 1996). Nespor (1987), whose seminal work established beliefs as a theoretical construct, asserted that teachers rely on their core belief systems rather than academic knowledge when determining classroom actions. The rapid pace and illstructured nature of educational environments promotes decision making based on core affective elements and evaluations rather than step-by-step problem solving. Beliefs are made up of episodic knowledge, characterized by remembered stories and events, affective elements, such as feelings about students, and existential presumption, beliefs about the existence or nonexistence of entities, such as ‘‘immaturity’’ ‘‘ability’’ and ‘‘laziness’’ (Nespor, 1987). Remembered events, feelings, subjective evaluations, and presumptions are likely to play a large part in teachers’ decisions to incorporate inquiry-based instruction. There is a complex interaction between teacher beliefs, which are mental, and teacher actions, which take place in the social arena. Our view is that teacher actions represent one aspect of a teacher’s beliefs and should not be perceived as separate entity from the belief system as a whole. What a teacher actually does in the classroom is representative of her beliefs. Researching teacher actions as well as cognitively perceived beliefs helps us to understand the complexities of a belief system as it played out in context (Richardson, 1996; Meijer & Verloop, 1998). Thus, interview and observation research is a reflective, iterative process that encompasses the entire belief system. In-service teachers continue learning to teach through the elaboration of their practical knowledge (Richardson, 1996). Beliefs are an important component of practical knowledge and serve as the filter through which practical knowledge is developed. Practical knowledge includes constructed knowledge of subject matter, as well as, contextualized knowledge of the classroom (Munby et al., 2000). In a recent review, van Driel and colleagues signified the importance of practical knowledge in fostering reform, ‘‘It is generally agreed that a teacher’s practical knowledge guides his or her actions in practice. Consequently, practical knowledge can be seen as the core of a teacher’s professionality’’ (2001, p. 142). Research on teachers’ practical knowledge has revealed that it is based on experience, action oriented, rooted in context, related to views of subject matter, tacit, and integrated (van Driel et al., 2001). Sociocultural Perspectives on Teaching and Learning A second relevant framework for this study is that of a sociocultural perspective on teaching and learning. First, sociocultural theory impels us to recognize the culture of secondary school not simply as an obstacle to reform, but as the realistic construction in which teachers move and work. In researching teachers’ beliefs about inquiry instruction, it is important to consider not only their personal cognitive understandings, but also how these understandings have been developed socially and within the context of the culture of the classroom. Gee (1990) points out that social and cultural beliefs form the foundation of discourse and meaning generation. According to Gee, cultural understandings lie at the heart of making meaning as humans attempt to understand the language of others. Beliefs and values fall into patterns that involve assumptions and choices about meaning based on simplified models of the world. Gee describes these cultural models as ‘‘something like movies or videotapes in the mind’’ (Gee, 1990, p. 78) that represent idealized or typical realities. Humans react to language and actions based on their cultural models. Cultural models, therefore, necessarily impact communication in the classroom as teachers and students interact together. Similarly, cultural models impact researchers’ and teachers’ understandings of classroom instruction and reform initiatives. The construct of cultural models informs our research by framing the work in authentic communication between researchers and teachers and by attempting to establish a common language for reflective discourse on inquiry. EXPERIENCED SECONDARY SCIENCE TEACHERS’ BELIEFS ABOUT INQUIRY 939 We recognize that teacher beliefs are strongly influenced by school culture and there has been quite a bit of research on this phenomenon (Tobin & McRobbie, 1996; Yerrick et al., 1997; Munby et al., 2000). It has been shown, for example, that teacher beliefs about the need to cover the mandated curriculum are so strong that science teachers subvert reform-oriented curriculum, yet claim publicly that they are teaching it (Olson, 1981; Yerrick et al., 1997). We sought to recognize these cultural beliefs as a driving social factor in classroom decisions about inquiry. Thus, we reject Tobin and McRobbie’s (1996) characterization of cultural beliefs as ‘‘myths’’ of science teaching in that they are real to teachers. Our goal was to explore the integration of cultural beliefs and individual beliefs as they impact decisions about inquiry-based science teaching. Literature Review Inquiry The concept of inquiry-based science teaching was well articulated by Joseph Schwab during the 1960s (Schwab, 1962). Schwab protested the teaching of science as a presentation of facts already known, which he called a ‘‘rhetoric of conclusions.’’ He envisioned a school curriculum that more accurately represented the scientific endeavor as engaged in by practicing scientists, including active questioning and investigation. Since that time, the idea of students posing questions, designing experiments, collecting data, and drawing conclusions has been almost universally appealing and at the same time very difficult to implement in real classrooms. A comprehensive report on the efficacy of inquiry-based curricula from the 1970s indicated that despite ten years of development and inservice teacher education, the amount and quality of inquiry-based teaching was well below the desired state (Welch, Klopfer, Aikenhead, & Robinson, 1981). Teachers in the study perceived several barriers to inquiry-based teaching including safety issues, lack of equipment, management difficulties, and the need to teach a mandated curriculum. Most importantly, the inquiry-based curricula of the 1970s had been largely designed by scientists, with emphases on cognitive rigor and the structure of scientific reasoning. The Welch et al. report (1981) indicated that teachers felt these curricula did not engage or meet the needs of most of their students. Since these early endeavors, inquiry has once again become the focus of science teaching with the National Science Education Standards calling for inquiry as the ‘‘central strategy for teaching science’’ (NRC, 1996, p. 31). Inquiry-based pedagogy may be thought of a set of concepts about scientific investigation and the reasoning skills that promote the use of those concepts, rather than as a specific teaching model or set of strategies (Keys & Bryan, 2001). Concepts to be learned by the students include how to pose a scientific question, identify and conduct procedures to answer the question, look for patterns and meaning in their data, construct a knowledge claim to answer their question, support their claim with evidence, and explain their findings in light of a framework of scientific information (NRC, 1996). Recent research indicates that for those teachers who persist in promoting inquiry, either by posing interesting questions for children to answer, or by facilitating children to pose their own questions, inquiry-based learning can be a very successful practice. One element of success is that children have positive attitudes towards inquiry. Students like to be involved in asking their own questions and formulating ways to answer those questions (Crawford, Krajcik, & Marx, 1999; Gibson & Chase, 2002; Hand, Wallace, & Yang, 2004). A second element of success is that students improve their ability to ask researchable questions and to coordinate questions with knowledge claims and evidence as they become more accustomed to inquiry-based learning (Crawford et al., 1999; Hand et al., 2004). Many elementary teachers have embraced open inquiry EXPERIENCED SECONDARY SCIENCE TEACHERS’ BELIEFS ABOUT INQUIRY 941 and have written stories of their own successes (Keys & Bryan, 2001). However, students need assistance in reframing the culture of the classroom in terms of what is expected of them as successful learners (Crawford et al., 1999). A further consideration for professional development of inquiry-based science is research on the complex and demanding roles for a teacher using inquiry. Crawford (2000) found that an excellent teacher of inquiry-based science teaching exhibited many nontraditional roles in the classroom, including scientist, diagnostician, motivator, guide, innovator, monitor, mentor and collaborator. Typically, teachers are not prepared for these roles during teacher education and most teachers do not see these roles exemplified by their peers in the school. Clearly, inquiry-based teaching requires education in new ways of working with children that go far beyond simple notions of teacher as facilitator (Minstrell & van Zee, 2000). Sustained and supportive professional development is the most promising approach to making a lasting impact. Beliefs About Science Teaching Previous research indicates that teacher beliefs about students and student learning, the nature of science, epistemology, and the role of the teacher are all significant elements of teacher beliefs systems that may impact views of inquiry. Teacher beliefs about the limitations of their students in terms of ‘‘ability’’ or ‘‘maturity’’ can be an obstacle to more student-centered instruction. For example, Cronin-Jones (1991) conducted two case studies of middle grades teachers implementing a constructivist-based curriculum. She found that both teachers held strong beliefs that students of this age group needed explicit direction, that students learn best through repeated drill and practice, and that factual content acquisition is the most important student outcome. These beliefs prevented the teachers from enacting the curriculum in ways that the developers intended. Cronin-Jones concluded, ‘‘In order to ensure more congruence between intended and implemented curricula, developers should put more effort into determining and considering existing teacher belief structures before developing new curricula’’ (1991, p. 248). Similarly, teachers’ understandings of the nature of science may create barriers to im- plementing inquiry-based instruction. Previous studies have indicated that many teachers have a view of the nature of science as an objective body of knowledge created by a rigid ‘‘scientific method’’ (Duschl & Wright, 1989; Brickhouse, 1990; Gallagher, 1991). Frequently teachers have no educational background in the history or philosophy of science, nor do they have first hand experience practicing science. Thus, they tend to portray science as a collection of facts, principles, and concepts with little or no instructional attention given to the processes by which scientific knowledge is made public and validated (Gallagher, 1991). One implication of this dilemma is that without a firm understanding of how scientists work, teachers may be inhibited to involve students in activities that explore questioning, deviate from exact procedures, interpret data, or obtain a variety of explanations for the phenomena. Hashweh (1996) characterized science teachers as learning constructivists, learning empiricists, knowledge constructivists and knowledge empiricists. Learning and knowledge constructivists tended to believe that scientific knowledge is tentative and invented, recognized students’ naive science ideas, and believed that the learner has an active role in constructing knowledge. In contrast, learning and knowledge empiricists tended to believe that scientific knowledge is an objective collection of facts, were not aware of naive conceptions, believed in reinforcement as a method of learning, and emphasized the ‘‘scientific method,’’ both as a paradigm for scientists and for instruction. Hashweh found that these epistemological beliefs affected the nature of science teaching with constructivist-oriented teachers eliciting and 940 WALLACE AND KANG recognizing students’ prior conceptions, and using a wide variety of teaching strategies to develop conceptual understandings. In contrast, empiricist-oriented teachers recognized students’ prior conceptions as incorrect, and used fewer teaching strategies to convey the correct information to students. Research on beliefs that undergird science instruction have shown that teachers construct cultural beliefs that impede reform (Tobin & McRobbie, 1996). Tobin and McRobbie (1996) identified four major ‘‘myths’’ of secondary science instruction: the transmission myth, the efficiency myth, the myth of rigor, and the myth of preparing students for examinations. The secondary chemistry teacher in their study viewed himself simultaneously as a powerful keeper and transmitter of chemistry knowledge, and as a relatively powerless individual in terms of transforming the chemistry curriculum. The authors assert that the story of this chemistry teacher is representative of many middle and high school teachers that their group has studied over a period of ten years in both the United States and Australia. Beliefs about the necessity of covering the curriculum, preparing students for examinations, and keeping the science classroom efficient are pervasive, and any attempt to foster inquiry-based instruction in the secondary science classroom must take these entrenched cultural beliefs into consideration. Additional studies confirm that teacher beliefs about the nature of knowledge, teaching science, and the mandated curriculum impede and ‘‘filter’’ innovative practice suggested by professional development (Yerrick et al., 1997; Huberman & Middlebrooks, 2000; Munby et al., 2000). Powerful cultural influences, most especially, the school curriculum document continue to shape belief systems and educational decisions. Yerrick et al. (1997) concluded that an intricate cognitive system of resolving and rationalizing mechanisms allowed teachers to believe they had incorporated reform practices without changing their core beliefs. Munby et al. (2000) suggest that when science is separated from its original contexts of investigations and questioning and becomes transplanted in secondary school culture, it becomes inauthentic. This inauthenticity limits the nature of science and the understanding of teachers of the nature of science, thus, limiting professional development and growth. Similarly, Huberman and Middlebrooks (2000) documented the ‘‘dilution’’ of rich inquiry-based science curriculum embedded in the Voyage of the Mimi. A review of these studies presents a grim picture of the possibilities for reform. In a quantitative study, Haney, Czerniak, and Lumpe (1996) reported that teacher beliefs were a strong predictor of their intentions to implement reform-based strategies. They determined that the following four beliefs were most salient to teachers’ intention to initiate inquiry: (1) increase student enjoyment and interest in science; (2) foster positive scientific attitudes and habits of mind; (3) help students learn to think independently; and (4) make science relevant to the students’ everyday lives. The study further indicated that experienced teachers wanted training in inquiry approach and the most powerful component of lasting reform may be the opportunity to experience success with inquiry-based teaching. Methods Research Design This study was an interpretive multiple within-case study that was conducted from an ethnographic perspective. It was interpretive in the tradition inspired by Erickson (1986), in which the participants own meanings and points of view were sought. While not a true ethnography, we used an ethnographic lens to capture a sense of the learning culture in each classroom we observed. A sociocultural constructivist perspective guided the research. 942 WALLACE AND KANG Participants and Professional Development Activities Six experienced teachers were selected for this study, from a pool of 15 teachers who attended summer workshops on inquiry, had an interest in implementing inquiry, and who volunteered to participate in the research. The workshops were conducted during the summers of 1999 and 2000 and included a few follow-up group meetings during the respective fall semesters. The first author of this study was the instructor for the workshops. The goal of the professional development workshops was for teachers to construct their own concept of inquiry-based science teaching within a community of learners. Three types of activities took place during the workshops. First, the participants explored their own and their peers’ initial conceptions of inquiry through concept mapping, journaling, and discussion. Second, they engaged in inquiry-based activities designed by workshop staff, such as, building a ‘‘water strider,’’ observing and creating investigation questions for protists, and investigating the ecology of a local lake ecosystem. Third, participants designed their own inquiry-based laboratory activities to further extend and define their know- ledge. During the entire professional development experience, the participants engaged in numerous discussions about what they believed were the characteristics of inquiry. Rather than impose her own ideas, the researcher supported the teachers’ own knowledge construction (Keys & Bryan, 2001). There is little question that ideas surfacing in the workshops and professional development meetings influenced the participants’ views of inquiry. The researchers conducted interviews with the teachers about their views of inquiry at the beginning of the workshops and these, along with concept maps and journals, were used as data in the study. However, the researchers’ goal was not to capture any change in the participants’ views of inquiry as a result of the workshop. The reasons for this are twofold. First, we do not believe that a 1-week workshop (even with follow-up meetings) can change the core beliefs of a teacher. Second, our philosophy was that of creating a dialogue with the teachers so that all the participants in the learning community could construct a clearer understanding of how inquiry-based instruction might work in real classrooms. The researchers learned as much from the teachers as vice-versa. Therefore, the specific impact of the professional development was not investigated as a focus of the research study. Teacher beliefs before, during, and after the workshop were probed as one unified view of inquiry that is manifested in competing beliefs sets. In addition, teacher practices back in the classroom were used as a primary data source for evidence concerning teacher beliefs. We agree with Richardson (1996) that practices and espoused beliefs should be used together to gain a deep understanding of core beliefs. The six teachers were purposefully selected to represent a range of school settings and subjects. Teacher participant, Pamela (European American, 16 years teaching experience), taught ninth grade physical science at a rural high school. Pamela had been a laboratory technician in chemistry and had raised a family before becoming a teacher in midlife. She had completed a master’s degree in science education during the late 1980s. Jerry (European American, 25 years teaching experience) taught eleventh and twelfth grade chemistry and physics at the same high school as Pamela. Jerry’s background was in biology and he had done graduate work in the field of ecology many years prior to the study. The school where Jerry and Pamela taught was moderately sized and served a largely White population. Many students at the school were below the poverty line and it was acknowledged in the school culture that very few students would go on to a four year college. Jerry enjoyed teaching what he believed were the academic elite students at the school in chemistry and physics. Tom (European American, 20 years teaching experience) taught diverse non-collegebound students in physical science and principles of technology at a suburban high school. He had a EXPERIENCED SECONDARY SCIENCE TEACHERS’ BELIEFS ABOUT INQUIRY 943 master’s degree in science education at the time of the study, which he had obtained many years before. The population at Tom’s school was about 60% White, 30% Latino, and 10% other. Tom’s physical science students were generally ninth graders, while the principles of technology students (considered to be low-achieving) were tenth and eleventh graders. Tom enjoyed teaching students who were younger or placed in lower level classes. Ellen (European American, 15 years teaching experience) taught ninth grade honors and gifted biology at a diverse suburban high school. Ellen had completed her master’s degree in science education just one year prior to the study. Students at Ellen’s high school were approximately 75% White and 25% Black. Ellen wanted to present a challenging course for her high level students. Rose (Nigerian, 12 years teaching experience) taught college preparatory chemistry in a mostly White suburban high school. Rose taught both average students and honors students for chemistry. Rose’s previous educational background was unknown to the researcher, but she had taught secondary chemistry previously in Nigeria. Finally, Jane (European American, 29 years of teaching) taught ninth grade honors biology at the same suburban high school as Rose. Jane, whose husband is a science education professor, was a recognized teacher-leader in the state. She had been active in professional organizations, conferences, and workshops for many years. Coincidentally, Jane had been the cooperating teacher mentor for Ellen during student teaching several years previous. Data Sources and Collection To investigate the teachers’ beliefs and practices about inquiry, multiple data sources were obtained. Three participant teachers were studied during one academic year, and three were studied during the second academic year. Data sources used to determine teachers’ inquiry beliefs and practices included: (1) semistructured formal interviews; (2) informal interviews; (3) field notes from observation and video tapes of classroom teaching; (4) lesson plan and student materials documents; and (5) written reflections of the teachers. The first data collection technique was a series of formal interviews used to probe the teachers’ beliefs about inquiry and other related aspects of teaching and learning throughout the study year. Participants were each interviewed three to four times including, early in study year, midway through the study year, and towards the end of the study year. Initial interview questions included probes of teachers’ beliefs about their students, the science curriculum, their views of the nature of science, purposes for laboratory activities, and inquiry-based teaching. As the study progressed, interview questions were often designed to probe specific teaching actions seen in classroom observations. For example, in midyear Jerry was asked, ‘‘What is the influence of the curriculum on your teaching?,’’ ‘‘What did your students learn about torque today?,’’ and ‘‘Why do you structure your labs the way you do?’’ The third and/or fourth round of interviews included questions designed to provide member checks on data analysis. Data analysis was presented to the teachers as a representation of their beliefs and further comments and clarifications were elicited. All interviews were audiotaped and transcribed in full. A second source of data was informal interviews conducted before, during, and after classroom observation visits. The researchers asked the teachers what they planned to do in the lesson that day, what were their purposes for the lessons, and how they thought students responded to the lesson. The informal interviews were conversational in nature. The researchers recorded data arising from the informal interviews in their field notes during the site visits. To investigate actual classroom practices, observations were conducted by the researchers from fall of 1999 through spring of 2001. Researchers attended 4–10 classroom sessions of either 55- or 90-min duration. During the first year of the study, 9 or 10 observations were made of each 944 WALLACE AND KANG teacher. Our data analysis, as well as the literature (van Driel et al., 2001), indicated that teachers’ belief sets as enacted in the classroom were similar to their espoused beliefs during interviews. Therefore, the number of classroom observations during the second year was reduced to four or five. During each observation, the researchers recorded extensive field notes and the class lesson was videotaped. Observations were made during instruction that the teachers self-identified as having an inquiry component or orientation, including demonstrations and laboratory activities. The videotape data were transcribed in full. A fourth data source included lesson plan documents and handout materials for students created by the participants in preparation for teaching what they identified as inquiry-based lessons. Approximately five sets of documents were collected for each teacher participant. The documents were photocopied. Finally, in some cases we had access to teachers’ written reflec- tions. Ellen provided a document of several pages in length that she had written in preparation for National Board Certification. It contained reflections on her inquiry-based teaching. The teachers also kept brief learning logs during the summer workshops and these were used for additional insight into their thinking. Data Analysis All interview audiotapes were transcribed in full. Four to eight videotapes for each teacher were transcribed in full; as coding proceeded we determined that our data analysis was saturated and additional video transcriptions would not reveal new data patterns. Interview transcripts, video transcripts, journal entries, and field notes were typed into computer word-processing files. Four iterations of coding took place. Prior to coding, the data corpus was unitized into segments representing a single idea, usually one to three sentences. Then, the unitized data were first descriptively coded, using coding categories suggested by the interview questions and adding emergent categories. These descriptive categories included nature of school science, goals of laboratory, teaching strategies, student characteristics, student learning, assessment, and inquiry. After descriptive coding, the first round of interpretive coding took place. The two researchers each took the data for the three teachers from the first year of the study and created interpretive codes within each descriptive category independently. These codes interpreted the teachers’ meanings within each category. For example, the first author created the following codes for participant, Tom, for the inquiry category: imagination, deep thinking, posing questions, deve- loping lab techniques, challenge students, more than one answer, and autonomous thinking. Next, the two researchers met and compared their interpretive coding lists, adding, deleting, and combining codes until a consensus coding scheme was achieved. The third coding iteration (second round of interpretive coding) then took place as the two researchers split and independently recoded data from the first set of three teachers, according to the consensus coding scheme. At this time, the frequencies of codes were determined (Miles & Huberman, 1994) and the most common patterns were noted using constant comparative analysis (Glaser & Strauss, 1967). From these data, a summary was made of the most important coding categories for each participant’s beliefs. For example, Tom’s most salient beliefs about inquiry included deep thinking, challenging students, developing lab techniques, and having more than one answer. Once each participant’s beliefs about each topic were characterized, we went back to the data to select excerpts that exemplified each main category as a further check on validity. The final steps in this round of analysis were the coding, frequency analysis, and constant comparison of the data from the three teachers in the second year of the study. In the fourth and final coding iteration, we dropped one of the original descriptive categories that did not seem directly relevant to the research questions and the emerging belief profiles, EXPERIENCED SECONDARY SCIENCE TEACHERS’ BELIEFS ABOUT INQUIRY 945 assessment. Additionally, the categories for student learning and nature of school science were collapsed into one category that appeared to be emerging as one of importance—nature of successful science learning. Third, we renamed the teaching strategies category to implementation of inquiry. The entire data corpus was reviewed in light of these changes, and exemplars from the data were modified as well. All patterns and themes were then synthesized into narratives of belief profiles that appear in the paper. Results The results comprise five beliefs profiles for the six science teachers in the study. Ellen’s Profile Nature of Successful Science Learning. Ellen’s beliefs about successful science learning can be described as enculturation into scientific practices. A large focus of Ellen’s work with her students was introducing them to scientific techniques, equipment, language, and ways of thinking. To Ellen, constructing an understanding of science was dependent on engaging in authentic scientific practices. Her goals were to ‘‘develop the students’ ability to use the inquiry process, to learn to communicate using scientific vocabulary, to teach the students a variety of tools in the lab to collect data, to explore the nature of science, and to advance the students’ problem solving, thinking, and reasoning skills’’ (Ellen, excerpt from written reflection on inquiry). Ellen believed that successful science learning had occurred when students were able understand a scientific problem, use the tools and techniques appropriate to investigate the problem, and engage in authentic discourse relevant to the problem. Interview, observation, and videotape data supported the theme that Ellen believed students are learning science when they can engage meaningfully in scientific practices. Her focus on tools and techniques was evidenced in her careful teaching about using mathematics to understand population size in the yeast lab, microbiological techniques in the herb lab, and field equipment and its use in the limnology lab. In the following excerpt from her written reflection on inquiry, she explained how she believed she met her goals for enculturating her students into microbiological techniques: In the second segment of the videotape, the students are implementing phase two of their investigation. At this stage, many students are comfortable with microbiological techni- ques and equipment used in this investigation. They have learned how to make their extracts using the mortar and pestles, balances, micropipettors, and graduated aliquots and cylinders. They demonstrated the correct technique to spread the herbs on the plates and inoculate them with bacteria’’ (Ellen, excerpt from written reflection on inquiry). In addition to becoming proficient with lab techniques and tools, Ellen described science learning as using scientific language and thinking skills for authentic purposes. The use of scientific thinking concurrently with language and technique formed the core of Ellen’s beliefs in enculturation into scientific practices. When asked what it means for an activity to be successful, she responded: I think when I see students using scientific vocabulary, using process skills, like it was the most natural thing in the world. Like they’re really not even thinking about it any more, like it’s just been incorporated into their problem solving skills. That’s when you have a 946 WALLACE AND KANG really successful activity. If they’re telling me ‘‘that thing over there’’ or ‘‘I don’t exactly know what we did here,’’ constantly referring back to their notes, I think we have a problem here. I think they’re telling me right there that they didn’t learn too much from that activity . .. but I found in particular in the herb lab that they were using many terms, like they would use any other common term—like they would use ‘‘pencil.’’ They were coming up with ‘‘where are my aliquots’’ or ‘‘there’s something wrong with my agar.’’ I think that has to do with success, when a kid can explain it back to you or, and this is a big one, when they can take what they learned and apply it in a similar situation. (Ellen interview, 4 /27 /01) Purposes of Laboratory and Inquiry. Ellen believed that the purpose of doing laboratory in science class was to develop problem-solving skills, including understanding the nature of a scientific problem, setting goals or subquestions to solve the problem, thinking through a plan, and interpreting data. In an interview, Ellen explained how her thinking about the purposes of laboratory has changed significantly over the past few years. The following excerpts illustrate how Ellen rejected her previous beliefs about the value of laboratory for teaching and reinforcing science content: Researcher: What would you say are the main purposes for doing laboratory activities in science? Ellen: If you’d have asked me that question probably two years ago, I would probably tell you that it was to reinforce content. Probably since I’ve started my master’s I’ve really changed my outlook on the purpose of labs. And really what I want them to do is to be able to think. To be able to develop a plan and follow through and then be able to do something with that data. Do they just look at the data and go, ‘‘Okay, I’ve got my data, now what?’’ or do they, can they, look at that data and make meaningful conclusions from that data? . . . [several interchanges later] They should know, before they start any activity, what is it they are trying to achieve. And, like I said with some of the other [cookbook] labs, I don’t think they do know that, they are just following the steps, and being very careful. Most of them will be very careful to follow the steps, but they really don’t know why they are doing it. So I guess you could say my goal is to get out of it, to understand, first of all, understanding their problem and the working towards a solution to that goal. (Ellen, interview, 9 /8 /00) Ellen now believes that the value of cookbook labs is very limited. She strives to inculcate in her students a chain of reasoning including, deep understanding of a scientific question or problem, designing laboratory methods to solve the problem, collecting meaningful data, and interpreting and inferring from the data. Her purposes for laboratory were closely related to her conception of successful science learning, which highlights thinking and problem solving. Because her purpose was centered on scientific thinking, she is inclined to incorporate inquiry- based labs into her teaching repertoire. Implementation of Inquiry. Ellen’s implementation of inquiry-based science was to conduct five or six inquiry labs during the two-semester biology course that she teaches. During the study year, Ellen implemented inquiry-based labs on the following topics: yeast population change, photosynthesis, osmosis, herbal antibiotics; and lake ecosystems. Each of these labs took approximately one week of class time (55-min periods). Ellen viewed the content goals of the lab EXPERIENCED SECONDARY SCIENCE TEACHERS’ BELIEFS ABOUT INQUIRY 947 as being teacher-directed. Although her primary belief in successful science learning was enculturation into scientific thinking, she also subscribed to the school cultural belief of the need to prepare students for high stakes exams. As she expressed, ‘‘To me, let’s get rid of the content and let’s really work on problems in depth, that’s what I would like to do anyway. But because of where I am and what I teach, I don’t have the opportunity to do that’’ (Ellen, interview, 4/ 18/01). Ellen eased the tension in these conflicting belief sets by designing inquiry-based labs that embedded content-driven goals of her teaching, ‘‘For the most part, it’s my goal, this is what I’d like them to learn about . . . . I’d say I do more of a guided inquiry for the most part, not all the time, but for the most part with a specific teacher-centered goal in mind.’’ Therefore, Ellen set the inquiry problem for the students, but encouraged them to develop subquestions. For example, in the herbal antibiotics lab, she posed the problem, ‘‘How do herbal medicines affect bacteria?’’ (Ellen, document data) but left the students to choose the specific variables they wanted to test. Tom and Rose’s Profile Nature of Successful Science Learning. Tom and Rose have similar belief sets concerning the nature of learning, purposes of laboratory and inquiry, and the implementation of inquiry- based teaching. Because of the similarity of their profiles, their data have been combined into one section. Both teachers held a primary belief that successful science learning is developing a deep understanding of scientific concepts. They also held a secondary belief that successful science learning involves thinking and problem solving. For example, Rose illustrated her beliefs about the importance of content and conceptual understanding as follows: What is important is for them to understand the concepts and be able to build on that. So, I think of ways to make it easier for them to understand . . . . Specifically, if I do a lab, I want the children to find out the basis for the lab. If they understand the basis for the lab, then it’s easier for them to go through the lab. I want them to understand the lab, to understand the basis. (Rose, interview, 8/31/00) Tom and Rose’s primary belief that successful science learning is conceptual understanding was very closely related to their purposes for laboratory. Tom asserted that science was ‘‘hands- on’’ topic, ‘You can’t teach science just off the blackboard. You have to get in there and see the relationships before fully understanding. You have to get in there and see it and do it, and see what will work’’ (Tom, interview, 6/23/99). Both Tom and Rose taught concepts prior to doing the laboratory, so their students would have an optimal opportunity to understand the conceptual basis of the lab, ‘‘The day before I do the lab I hit the concepts as hard as I can’’ (Tom, interview, 6/ 21/ 99). Rose similarly stated, ‘‘I usually do labs after we have done the theory, we have covered the concept and now we want to go to the lab and go through the practical aspects’’ (Rose interview, 8/ 31/00). They were concerned that students could relate concepts from lecture and discussion to the concepts in the lab. Both Rose and Tom were interested in using inquiry-based methods to satisfy their secondary goal of science teaching; helping students become independent thinkers. Their desire to teach for conceptual understanding and to promote independent scientific thinking may be viewed as competing belief sets, because it is difficult to give students the freedom to develop their own labs, yet teach for canonical science understandings simultaneously. Both Rose and Tom expressed their interest in using inquiry to foster autonomous thinking. Early in the study year, Rose expressed her desire to try more inquiry: ‘‘I realize that a lot of the labs can be modified into 948 WALLACE AND KANG inquiry-based labs. And it makes the children think more and participate more in labs and even in the classroom. So, I am going to try and modify as many labs as I can as I go to inquiry-based labs, so the students will get more out of the labs’’ (Rose interview, 8/31/2000). Tom similarly stated, ‘‘I like the inquiry techniques or giving students more chance to use imagination, the more deeper thinking other than just following the steps or recipe’’ (Tom, interview, 6/ 23/99). Thus, for both Rose and Tom, successful science learning is made up of two elements, deep conceptual understanding, and independent scientific thinking. The teachers’ understanding of successful science learning is directly related to their purposes for conducting laboratories, discussed below. Purposes of Laboratory and Inquiry. Both Rose and Tom believed the primary purpose of laboratory to be the visualization of scientific concepts. For them, laboratory served as a visual and kinesthetic aid to connect concrete objects and events to scientific language as discussed in the lecture portion of the class. These teachers had a view of laboratory closely related to Millar’s (1998) idea of science labs as elaborate visual aids. Rose and Tom believed that students would use the information gained by their senses in laboratory to synthesize a deeper understanding of scientific concepts. Tom talked repeatedly to the researchers about the use of hands-on activities for students to ‘‘see’’ scientific ideas and relationships, ‘‘One purpose I have is to have the kids just visually do or manipulate whatever the concept we are going to talk about. I mean, I think in science, a picture is worth a thousand words.’’ Tom described how he used a solutions lab and post lab discussion to synthesize lecture notes with lab findings. He stated, ‘ [I’ll] let them reflect back on the notes I gave them yesterday. ‘That’s what happens when you crush it to increase surface area. When you heat it, you increase molecular motion,’ and see if they can tie those things back together. And take the notes they got out of class and pull it into what they were doing in lab and give them the connection . . . . They can really see it [factors on dissolving rate]. You know, I can say that the crushing increases dissolving rate. When they actually crush it for themselves, then they can see it’’ (Tom, interview, 3/23/ 00). Similarly, when asked ‘‘What are your purposes for doing laboratory?’’ Rose responded: First, to practicalize what we talked about in theory. Because sometimes you say these things in theory and they [the students] cannot visualize them. So when you do it in the lab, hands-on, the students are more able to understand what you are talking about. Like the significant figures, if I was just talking about it they probably wouldn’t get it, but because we had gone through this lab, they didn’t have any problem. (Rose, interview, 8/21/00) However, both Rose and Tom also believed that the purpose of inquiry-based instruction was to promote autonomous thinking and problem solving. Thus, they had a secondary belief in using lab to foster problem-solving skills. As Rose stated, ‘‘When you do inquiry-based labs and you start to ask questions, and somebody comes along [in their thinking] and you say, ‘Oh, fantastic, you did it and that is very good’’ (Rose, interview, 9/21/00). Tom values inquiry for promoting deep thinking in science. Tom believes that the opportunity to make mistakes, hypothesize, and manipulate equipment is an important reason to conduct inquiry: As long as students have a chance to work with it, think about it, manipulate it, and even make mistakes, get some wrong answers—I think they are learning. Even though it makes them totally lost. I’ve had kids that get totally frustrated trying to rig up a set of pulleys. They’ve never had to wire a pulley before and they can’t figure out, ‘How do I run that EXPERIENCED SECONDARY SCIENCE TEACHERS’ BELIEFS ABOUT INQUIRY 949 string through here.’ But I think when we finally get it figured out, they’ve actually learned something. They may not erect a pulley in their life again, but the thought process of thinking through the problem, that they may use again. And if I can teach my kids to think, I will be happy. (Tom, interview, 6/23/99) Implementation of Inquiry. Tom and Rose conducted many standard cookbook laboratories with their students that they did not self-identify as inquiry. Examples of these labs would be Tom’s teaching of factors that affect dissolving and Rose’s lab on the preparation and reactions of gases. These labs had step-by-step instructions for the students and little room for independent scientific thinking. The teachers considered these labs to be essential for demonstrating important concepts, so that students could visualize them. Tom and Rose implemented cookbook labs throughout their courses. Rose and Tom, however, remained interested in inquiry and implemented a few inquirybased laboratories with their students during the study year. They viewed the inquiry activities as distinct problem-solving events, lasting for one or two class periods. Their inquiry activities were designed to have students develop laboratory procedures or think through patterns in the data. Rose developed inquiry-based laboratories for the separation of mixtures and grouping ions according to their valence numbers (Rose, laboratory activities documents). The latter activity required students to sort models of ions into groups, determine common characteristics within each group, and relate each group to its place in the periodic table. Field notes revealed that students were engaged in this problem solving activity, were using scientific thinking, and were successful in recording family relationships for the ions (Rose, field notes, 9/29/00). Tom’s self-identified inquiry-based labs in physical science during the study year included an open-ended classification lab, designing bottle rockets, and designing a method to test for coefficients of friction for different materials (Tom, field notes, various). During the bottle rocket lab, Tom briefly discussed factors that could affect the height of the rocket, then gave his students complete freedom to create their rockets. Students were engaged in social negotiation of science ideas with their peers during this lab. Tom and Rose, therefore, enacted inquiry-based labs as discrete problem-solving events to promote independent thinking as a separate secondary goal of instruction. Cookbook labs were implemented to promote their primary goal of concept visualization. They resolved the competition between two competing belief sets for successful science learning by including two distinct types of labs in their repertoire, with inquiry-based labs taking a secondary role in the culture of their classrooms. Pamela’s Profile Nature of Successful Science Learning. For Pamela, successful science learning can be described as explaining everyday phenomena. She was concerned that students learn things in her classes that will help them become informed citizens. This belief was portrayed consistently in her classes through her extensive use of everyday science examples in lecture, as well as the use of everyday objects and substances in lab. She stated: I want them to go out into the world with an understanding of these topics. I think going out into the world, then, you need to know what it is, what it can do, what you can do, what to fear, and what not to fear. So I always felt this crazy desire to get as much knowledge into their heads as possible. (Pamela, interview, 3/16/2000) EXPERIENCED SECONDARY SCIENCE TEACHERS’ BELIEFS ABOUT INQUIRY 951 According to Pamela, students should have a wide range of information on why and how things work as consumers of science. Her teaching goal was to give students as much explanation as possible and to encourage them to use their science knowledge. Pamela often related to us how she felt rewarded when students are able to explain science independently: I always liked the gold penny thing. I didn’t announce we were doing an experiment with alloys. I just said, ‘‘Okay, we are going to do an experiment.’’ . . . . I never told them we alloyed. They told me that was an alloy. Sooner or later somebody figured that out. We had already covered the subject. (Pamela, interview, 3/16 /2002) Like Tom and Rose, Pamela exhibited two competing belief sets about the nature of successful science learning. While Tom and Rose had a primary belief in learning as deep conceptual understanding, Pamela had a primary belief in learning as reproducing scientists’ explanations and also had a secondary belief in learning as independent problem solving. She felt an urgent desire to communicate the scientific view, so students could use it in their everyday lives, as shown in the above quote, ‘‘I always felt this crazy desire to get as much knowledge into their heads as possible.’’ At the same time, she valued problem solving highly: She consistently defined inquiry as being able to ‘‘figure something out.’’ She stated, ‘‘They [students] love to figure things out. My whole teaching career I’ve seen that. It’s a high for them to figure things out’’ (Pamela, interview, 6/21/99). Thus, Pamela faced a tension similar to that of Tom and Rose in her teaching as she attempted to enact her competing belief sets. These competing belief sets were played out in Pamela’s classroom actions, as discussed below. Purposes of Inquiry and Laboratory. Pamela’s espoused purposes for laboratory and inquiry were very closely related to her secondary belief about the importance of independent problem solving. Pamela’s definition of inquiry is ‘‘Any activity in which they [students] have to figure something out. It might be as extensive as figuring out the procedures for a laboratory exercise, it might be simply figuring out why something happens that we observed in the classroom’’ (Pamela, interview, 11/15/ 99). Pamela believed that the purpose of demonstrations, which she defined as an inquiry-based activity, is for students to generate explanations. For example, at the culmination of a unit about phase change, she did a series of several demonstrations using dry ice to illustrate concepts such as, evaporation, freezing, and sublimation. She taught her students to make careful observations, record their observations, and then write an explanation for what it was they observed (Pamela, field notes, 9/15/99). Her goal was to have students use scientific vocabulary and cause-effect relationships to verbalize an explanation. For example, when observing dry ice in a flask, she prompted the students as follows: I want you to observe as it comes around. I want you to write what you observe inside and outside and I want you to write down why you think it is happening and identify what it is. Remember, write down what you observe. [A couple of minutes later.] Okay, what did we observe? (Pamela, video transcript, 9/15 /99) Pamela’s ideas about student participation in hands-on laboratories also illustrated her secondary purpose for laboratory, that of independent problem solving. A foundation for this belief set was her dissatisfaction with traditional cookbook labs. She stated: I’ve often felt that the typical lab was like a 1040 form, and when they’re finished they have no more idea what they’ve done than we do when we go at our income tax. And the nice 950 WALLACE AND KANG thing about inquiry is that’s not the case. They can figure out what they’re going to do, so they know what they’re going to do—it makes sense to them. (Pamela, interview, 11/15/99) Pamela believed that an important element of inquiry is that students come up with their own procedures for conducting the lab. She viewed inquiry laboratories as opportunities for student thinking about laboratory procedures. She wrote five labs during the study year that incorporated students’ own procedures for determining quantities such as volume, density, work, and mecha- nical advantage for everyday situations and objects in the classroom. Implementation of Inquiry. The data revealed that Pamela used two main types of engagement in inquiry, demonstrations with questioning, and laboratory activities in which students determined all or part of the procedures. Pamela’s beliefs about demonstrations as inquiry were shaped by research she did when obtaining her master’s degree several years ago. She was particularly impressed by a research paper indicating there was no significant difference in science achievement between students who engaged in hands-on laboratories and those who watched demonstrations, as long as there was questioning and discussion related to explaining the concept. She stated: I am influenced by a paper that I read when I was getting my master’s degree here in 1981– 1986 .. . . And what they found out was that using inquiry methods of discussion, that it didn’t matter, whether they touched it, watched it, or just talked about it. They did say that the students who did the hands-on gained some manipulative skills that the others didn’t. But the point they were making was that the inquiry discussion was the most important part. So, I like these little activities, experiments, my kids call them, to spark that kind of inquiry discussion without having to take the time for a full hands-on activity. (Pamela, interview, 6/21/99) Pamela’s belief that inquiry could take the form of demonstration was reinforced by other elements in her belief system, which represented constraints to more studentcentered forms of inquiry-based instruction. First, she believed that science instruction must be efficient, because there is so much content to cover in a short period of time. Second, her belief that her students are ‘‘immature’’ and ‘‘unsafe’’ led her to use the demonstration method over the laboratory method when any harsh chemicals or complicated procedures were involved. These beliefs worked to support Pamela’s main focus on demonstrations as a form of inquiry, especially the need to be efficient, which permeated many of her classroom actions. She described her demonstrations, ‘‘It’s very efficient, you’re doing your topic, you do your experiment, you discuss it, you move on. No set up, take down time, no socializing, no milling around’’ (Pamela, interview, 6/21/99). Her belief in efficiency was related to her primary teaching goal of transmitting canonical scientific information. She desired that each student had an opportunity to create their own understanding of the demonstration. Yet, her need for efficiency caused a tension in her teaching. Rather than let students discuss their observations and explanations at length, she typically called on a student who would have the correct explanation right away, elaborated the explanation herself, and then moved onto a different point. For example: Pamela: Okay, do you see any fog in here? Students: No. Pamela: No, so we see no fog in here, so the fog must not be coming from the dry ice. Where is the fog coming from? 952 WALLACE AND KANG Student: Water in that little jug. Pamela: I heard someone say water and that is correct. As the bubbles come up through the water, they picked up water vapor. Just the water vapor, not the food coloring and it got so cold that it condensed into fog. But you have to have water to get the fog (Video transcript, 9/15/99). While Pamela asked provoking inquiry-oriented questions, e.g., ‘‘Where is the fog coming from?’’ she seized the first correct utterance made by a student, then elaborated the concept herself, rather than let students construct and verbalize their own explanations. Thus, one major tension identified in Pamela’s belief system about inquiry was her desire to use inquiry-based questioning strategies and her need for efficiency in instruction. The urge for efficiency also mediated Pamela’s use of inquiry in the laboratory. All Pamela’s laboratory activities occurred after the main concepts had been introduced and discussed in class. For example, with the volume and density lab, students had already discussed the density formula and worked practice problems in class. Pamela felt that students would be able to conduct inquiry- based labs only when they had background knowledge, such as the density formula, to use as a resource. Second, she believed that she needed to provide enough structure and guidance so that students would complete the lab in one class period without running into major stumbling blocks. When asked how she thought inquiry-based laboratories were working she replied, ‘‘Some of them very good. Some of them sort of marginally, we need to come back and make it easier for the students to figure out what to do, to know what to do. Some of them have been a little too open inquiry, they’re going to need more guidance’’ (Pamela, interview, 11/ 15/ 99). In summary, Pamela implemented inquiry as demonstration with thought provoking ques- tions, and the design of procedures for lab activities, as she sought to satisfy her secondary goal of having students ‘‘figure things out.’’ Unlike Tom and Rose, who separated their competing belief sets through the use of different types of labs on different days for different purposes, Pamela attempted to satisfy both her competing belief sets with the same activities. This attempt at reconciliation led to her cutting student involvement and independent thinking short, as her desire to tell students what they needed to know overtook most of her activities. Jerry’s Profile Nature of Successful Science Learning. Jerry’s main goal of science teaching was to develop scientific habits of mind in his students. He felt that his students, who are among the top achievers in the school, are well equipped to learn about scientific thinking, as well as, content. Jerry referred to scientific habits of mind using many different terms, such as, ‘‘how to think’’ or ‘‘scientific philosophy.’’ He elaborated on what he meant by teaching how to think in his courses: The goal of my courses, chemistry and physics, is to teach students a little bit about how to think, to show them that we can explain how things in the world work, to give them a little bit of the scientific philosophy of raising a question, generalizing, and to try to explain that things are explainable and that the underlying rules are not really all that complex. (Jerry, interview, 2 /18 /00) One of Jerry’s primary concerns was to encourage his students to be aware of the process of science while they are learning science. He was unique among the teachers in our study in that he purposefully designed his teaching around nature of science concepts. He asserted that he has EXPERIENCED SECONDARY SCIENCE TEACHERS’ BELIEFS ABOUT INQUIRY 953 struggled to present science as both a body of knowledge and a process of developing explanations of how things work. Jerry’s view of successful science learning as learning scientific habits of mind was illustrated in his emphasis on experimental error during the teaching of labs. He used experimental error to point out the limits of scientific knowledge and emphasized to his students that lab results are not exactly the same as science theories predict. He pointed out that science theories describe what is likely to happen, rather than what really happens (Jerry, field notes, 9/ 14/ 99). For him, a science theory is a model that has limitations. He insisted on avoiding the word ‘‘theory’’ in describing science knowledge; instead, he used the word ‘‘model’’ to convey the tentativeness and limits of science knowledge in explaining how the world works (Jerry, informal interview, 10/2/ 99). Jerry elaborated on how he emphasized the tentative nature of science: My model behaves this way. If the model behaves this way, then the real world should behave the same way. So the example that I am giving is [that] Bohr’s model of the atom predicted the existence of certain lines in the ultraviolet part of the spectrum, which had never been seen before .. . . He said there should be this wavelength and people looked in that part of the spectrum and said, ‘‘Wow, you are right!’’ Therefore his model has some validity . . . .[But] it turned out that he wasn’t quite right. He got false in something .. . then we modified our theories . . . .I am trying to teach [that] scientists don’t say this is the truth. (Jerry, interview, 2/18/99) In summary, Jerry’s notion of successful science learning is for students to understand the nature of science knowledge including both tentativeness and valid explanatory power. While he felt compelled to cover the content curriculum, he identified certain parts of his course where he is able to point out the nature of science; laboratory activities are among those parts. Purposes of Laboratory and Inquiry. Jerry had two parallel beliefs sets concerning the purposes of laboratory that were connected to his beliefs about students. Jerry believed there was a large difference in both the ‘‘ability level’’ and the ‘‘maturity’’ of students in the chemistry classes compared with the physics class: In chemistry, I’ve found if I don’t give them cut-and-dried information, they can’t do the labs. They don’t work as well in lab; they are more likely to spend the time socializing and just sit there unless I tell them exactly what to do. I’ve discovered that the older students [in physics] are able to proceed more independently. (Jerry, interview, 6/23/99) He was very open about the fact that he felt the cookbook labs he used in chemistry taught little more than reading directions and some basic laboratory manipulative skills. He justified his actions of conducting traditional labs in chemistry with his belief that chemistry students could not work independently. He, therefore, felt that lab was expendable in chemistry, ‘‘Like at the end of the year, when I’m busy saying I need to cover this and this and this, um, I’m going to cut down the number of labs that I do, because that’s what I find as being expendable.’’ (Jerry, interview, 6/23/99) Both interview and observation data provided evidence for a second strongly held and seemingly contradictory belief set about the purpose of lab in physics. In physics, Jerry used lab to achieve the following teaching goals: (1) promote thinking and problem solving in science; (2) foster students to ‘‘discover’’ physics phenomenon by observing patterns and inducing relation- ships; and (3) provide opportunities to confront misconceptions by considering evidence that is counterintuitive. In this sense, Jerry used what science educators might recognize as a conceptual 954 WALLACE AND KANG change model of teaching. Jerry used inquiry-based laboratories to foster the discovery of physics phenomenon, ‘‘I don’t tell them what the answer should be. They do not know what the answer should be’’ (Jerry, interview, 6/23/99). An example of allowing students to discover physics phenomenon was a laboratory on torque that asked students to investigate by balancing several weights on a beam. Rather than giving students the equation to verify, Jerry provided the opportunity for students to derive the equation from their own authentic data in the laboratory. Jerry discussed this lab in an interview: Researcher: What do you think the students learned about torque? Jerry: Most of the students by the end of the period had the equation. They did not know it [before the lab]. That’s why I wanted to give this lab before they’ve heard of torque, so they can derive the equation for themselves. I don’t want them to read. This is a simple equation. Most, I think everybody got it. It was really nice to see [students] B and T, I did not expect them to get it. [Some students came up with the standard form of the equation] but many groups came up with the form of ‘‘d1/d2 ¼ f 2/f 1,’’ which indicates they were coming up with it themselves not using the data table, so it’s nice to see independent thought. (Jerry, interview, 2/19/00) A defining characteristic of inquiry for Jerry was that the results of the laboratory be unexpected, ‘‘Frequently, I try to have the lab be, I want to say counterintuitive, but it’s that they don’t expect what’s going to happen. Thus, in a pendulum lab, the students think a heavy pendulum will swing faster. I don’t tell them ahead of time, I let them take their measurements and try and interpret the numbers (Jerry, interview, 6/ 23/99). Therefore, Jerry believed that it was possible for students to construct science concepts and principles first hand through inquiry, although he practiced this only with his physics students. His emphasis on successful science learning as scientific habits of mind is exemplified through laboratory in both chemistry and physics in his explicit teaching of science as models as valid, but subject to change and error. The difference was that in physics, students sometimes derived the models for themselves, while in chemistry, they verified well known models. Implementation of Inquiry. Jerry implemented inquiry-based learning only in physics during the study period. He did several labs in physics that he described as ‘‘structured inquiry’’ and several that he described as ‘‘guided inquiry.’’ He wrote in his reflection log: In physics, I do many low level inquiry labs. I sketch the outlines of what I want the students to do, they do not know the answer. Sometimes they think they know the answer, the lab is a discrepant event. They are frequently required to use their data to come to a mathematical conclusion (and a qualitative one as well). Sometimes the labs are directed, they are told what and how to do, other labs are more open-ended, they are given the tools and told what the goals is. (Jerry, reflection log, 6/23 /99) As an example of a structured inquiry, Jerry conducted a lab early in the study year on speed and distance in physics. During this lab, students were given simple directions about dropping a ruler and throwing a baseball, while measuring time variables. They then used these measurements to calculate and interpret outcomes, such as the time the ball was thrown in the air or the maximum height reached. Jerry considered this lab to be structured inquiry, because the students needed to take their own data, and use their own ideas about mathematical formulations to arrive at their EXPERIENCED SECONDARY SCIENCE TEACHERS’ BELIEFS ABOUT INQUIRY 955 conclusions. In the more open-ended guided inquiry labs, Jerry demonstrated a phenomenon to the students, such as a swinging pendulum or balance (used in the torque lab noted above). He then supplied the students with materials and asked them to use trial and error to determine variables and their relationships that affected the outcomes. Jane’s Profile Nature of Successful Science Learning. For Jane, successful science learning can described as creative thinking about science. It was important to Jane that students take scientific concepts and apply them in a creative fashion to make a product or artifact that is unique. She desired her students to gain ‘‘ownership’’ of their laboratory projects and to ‘‘think outside the box.’’ Throughout her teaching and assessment, Jane illustrated that individual thinking is valued: I want students to begin to see. I want them to think for themselves. I don’t want them to be a little parrot, parroting everything [that] everybody says. I do essay questions periodically with my exams. Um, memorization, on an objective exam, they can just memorize it. Putting it indirectly makes them begin to think, and put, you know, the ideas and concepts together. Concept mapping begins to show relationships. Open labs [inquiry] do the same thing. They have to start thinking for themselves. (Jane interview, 5/14/01) Jane believed that successful science learning involved a synthesis of important ideas or big concepts, which was related to her ideas about creativity. If students could synthesize ideas and devise creative representations for ideas, they demonstrated a deep understanding. Jane’s beliefs about successful science learning were well illustrated by her conception of a ‘‘kim-chee’’ lab. In the kim-chee lab, students use the medium of making kim-chee to demonstrate several of the biological principles and concepts they have been studying over the entire semester, such as osmosis, transport across cell membranes, photosynthesis, and respiration. She had each student group choose a concept to demonstrate, so that the collection of projects illustrated each of the major concepts studied over the first semester. The creative product for the students was their demonstration of a biological concept. She stated, ‘‘Creativeness they learn. How to think for themselves. They know that I’m willing to accept off the wall type of any experiment or that it doesn’t have to fit within the box. That they can extend outside the box and it can still be viable’’ (Jane interview, 5/ 14/01). Purposes of Laboratory and Inquiry. Both cookbook laboratories and inquiry-based laboratories had specific functions in Jane’s classroom. Jane started the school year with cookbook laboratories so that students may become accustomed to manipulating laboratory equipment and bridge over to inquiry by doing small pieces of laboratory independently. Jane referred to these experiences as a ‘‘bits and pieces’’ approach to teaching inquiry. For example, early in the semester, Jane implemented a lab with set procedures, but left the data analysis methods up to the students, ‘‘I may let them devise their own, [the students say] ‘Don’t you have a chart for us to put the data in?’ [and I reply] ‘Maybe this data needs a bar graph? Maybe this data needs a line graph Maybe it’s the pie graph? Maybe a chart would be better, but you have to come up with a chart for this.’ So that’s what I mean by bits and pieces’’ (Jane interview, 5/ 14/ 01). Thus, throughout several laboratory activities, Jane provided experiences for the students in designing laboratory procedures, data collection techniques, data analysis techniques, and drawing conclusions. A second function of cookbook laboratories was to provide a direct contrast to inquirybased laboratories. About halfway through the first semester, Jane introduced a cakebaking analogy to 956 WALLACE AND KANG the students to illustrate the distinction between cookbook and inquiry labs explicitly to the students: I liken both labs to baking a cake. In a cookbook lab, you have a recipe, for which you bake a cake. And I use this analogy with the students. And I say, ‘‘You go and you get, you know, your recipe and it tells you to get out, you know, flour, sugar, and eggs, and to cream your eggs and it gives you the directions. And it tells you what to do and what’s your result going to be. So what’s creative? What’s unusual? . . . . I tell the students, yeah, there’s a place for that. What’s different about the open lab? And then we begin to start, ‘‘I’m giving you the ingredients. Looking at these ingredients, how do you think we’re gonna do it? Why would we cream butter and sugar first, why not just add the butter after? Why do you do things a particular way? Can you vary a thing? Which ones can you vary?’’ (Jane interview, 5 /14 /01) Jane viewed the purpose of inquiry-based lab as a vehicle for students to develop their creative and independent scientific thinking. This purpose for inquiry was directly related to her notion of successful science learning as creative thinking, described above. Jane sought to have students create not only their own procedures and conclusions, but actually create their own methods of learning through science laboratory. To accomplish this, Jane allowed a great deal of choice in developing methods of data collection, so that students might choose the best way to answer their own questions. She designed a few extensive laboratories throughout the school year in which students had the opportunity to create their lab methods that matched their chain of reasoning. As she stated when asked her definition of inquiry-based labs: I like an open lab where a student is allowed, um, with some directions, is allowed to devise and develop a product of work that is his own. That hasn’t been dictated to by the teacher. An open lab is a lab where students are given the opportunity to explore ideas and concepts that they may have. Inherent in this is that the most difficult portion is that they don’t realize they can do this. And it takes a while to develop these skills or to think about using skills that they’ve never used before. (Jane, interview, 1/19/01) Thus, for Jane, the purpose of inquiry-based labs was to promote creativity and ownership in scientific problem solving. Her goal was for students to go beyond teacher direction and generate an independent product of scientific thinking. These ideas are well illustrated with her teaching of the ‘‘evolution lab,’’which is described in depth below. Implementation of Inquiry. Like Ellen, Jane preferred to conduct four to five in-depth inquiry labs over the course of the school year, each lasting 1 week or longer. Jane’s biology inquiries included the kim-chee inquiry, the meal worm inquiry, the evolution inquiry, and a required science project based on Wisconsin fast plants. Also, similar to Ellen, the content and goals of Jane’s inquiry labs were teacher-centered. Jane believed that inquiry topics should be chosen by the teacher, so that important mandated curriculum goals were achieved. Thus, neither Ellen nor Jane promoted a brand of inquiry where students chose their own inquiry topics. Jane described her method of assessment, which clearly related to a teacher-centered goal structure for inquiry, ‘‘So, I have to go back and look at the purpose for which I devised the lab at the beginning. And what was it I was looking for when I thought about this particular lab? What is that I wanted them to see, to measure, to do, to achieve? . . . . What is MY objective of this particular lab?’’ (Jane interview, 5/14/01). Jane began her evolution lab by describing to the students how paleontologists might go about developing observations of a primate skull. The goal of the lab was to ‘‘devise a model that could EXPERIENCED SECONDARY SCIENCE TEACHERS’ BELIEFS ABOUT INQUIRY 957 be used on unknown skulls by using known information (Jane, informal interview, 1/19/01). She then posed questions to guide student thinking and to draw students’ attention to salient features of the skulls. Starting with paper models (diagrams), the students were to make as many observations as possible of three skulls, then design a model, against which new unknown skulls could be compared. Her goal was to have the students develop a model to illustrate the evolutionary progression of skull features in primates. Jane allowed students to choose their own groups for the lab. Jane’s teaching technique during the lab was to circulate among the groups and ask probing questions. She often reiterated that the students needed to think for themselves. She reminded them of the major goal of the lab, ‘‘Take your time, be creative, think about how you will use the model. Next week, I will hand you a new skull and expect you to figure out where it fits into your model’’ (Jane, field notes, 1/19/01). She emphasized that students were doing scientific processes in order to reach the creative goal of the model, and that there should be a reason for each task they undertake, ‘‘Why take measurements if you don’t know why you’re doing it? Are you going to take measurements, observations, or both?’’ (field notes, 1/19/01). Over the next few days, the student groups completed and turned in their models. Several groups constructed two-dimensional continuums, similar to a time line with quantitative data, some did pictorial models, and some used qualitative data on their continuums. Discussion and Implications The findings of this study illuminate the tensions that secondary classroom teachers face when considering the implementation of inquiry. The study suggests that two major belief strands may need to be reconciled within the teachers’ practical knowledge base. The first belief strand appears to stem from school culture and centers on constraining factors that limit inquiry. Some of the results of this study match the findings of previous studies (Munby et al., 2000; Tobin & McRobbie, 1996; Yerrick et al., 1997) in that beliefs about students, efficiency, rigor, and exam preparation override inquiry implementation. In the current study, Pamela, Tom, and Rose believed they had to present canonical concepts and explanations in an efficient manner. Pamela and Jerry exhibited cultural beliefs that some students are too immature and lazy to accomplish inquiry. Jerry believed that laboratory was expendable in chemistry class when time was limited and the curriculum needed to be completed. Ellen was concerned that her gifted students be presented with a rigorous curriculum and also believed the teacher must choose laboratory problems that match the mandatory curriculum. Therefore, this study supports findings of other studies that indicate inquiry-based teaching will be difficult to implement in the current school culture. However, this study also illustrates another strand of teacher beliefs that work to promote inquiry. All six participants retained interest in and achieved some level of inquirybased activities in their teaching. Each teacher exhibited a core belief set in favor of inquiry. Pamela, Tom, and Rose believed that inquiry could foster independent thinking, deep thinking, and problem solving. These represented skills that they valued for their students. Jerry believed that physics students could gain conceptual understanding from inquiry-based investigations. Ellen believed that using inquiry-based strategies was the best way to enculturate students into scientific thinking practices, while Jane believed in inquiry for stimulating creativity in science learning. Thus, the teachers held competing belief sets. The belief sets that constrained inquiry-based teaching were more public and culturally based, while the belief sets that promoted inquiry were more private and based on the individual teacher’s notion of successful science learning. While the culturally based beliefs of exam preparation and efficiency in covering the curriculum exhibited a 958 WALLACE AND KANG powerful influence, the teachers also had learning goals for their students that stood in contrast to the culturally supported goals. The private and individual goals of the teachers for their students represented aspects of learning that are not explicitly named in state or district mandated curriculum including deep thinking, independent thinking, creativity, learning the techniques and language of science, and deriving important ideas from patterns in data. Because these goals were not officially sanctioned, the teachers may struggle to reconcile their own learning goals for their students with the culturally mandated learning goals. We believe that this causes a substantial difficulty for teachers, who are placed in the position of trying to choose between what they believe to be best for their students with what society has deemed best for their students. The difficulty of resolving this tension was observed in our study through the various techniques teachers used for decision making. Jerry separated his competing belief sets about learning goals by segregating them into his two courses. In physics, he used an inquiry-based approach often as an integral part of teaching. In chemistry, he conducted a teacher-centered class. Tom and Rose segregated their competing belief sets by conducting most activities in a manner to achieve culturally based learning goals (e.g., conceptual curriculum) and conducting a few separate activities to foster their private goals for scientific thinking. Pamela attempted to integrate her competing belief sets into the same activities, but in practice, she failed to allow students time to accomplish meaningful inquiry. Ellen and Jane may have achieved the most successful integration of their belief sets. Ellen implemented several meaningful inquirybased laboratories each year and reconciled the time it took to achieve her private learning goal of enculturation into science by staying within the content of the mandated curriculum. Jane integrated her ideas about creativity into her on-going class activities, but like Ellen, stayed within the bounds of teacher- guided inquiry. It is also interesting to note that both Ellen and Jane taught generally high achieving students, whom they may believe have the cognitive capacity to process both the learning goals of the mandated curriculum, as well as, the teacher’s private learning goals. The private beliefs about independent and deep thinking resonate with recent calls for establishing argumentation as a central form of teaching science. Driver, Newton, and Osborne (2000) state that a social constructivist view of teaching science implies an education in scientific investigative and discursive processes, and that ‘‘Consideration needs to be given to the purpose of the experiment to be carried out. What would be an appropriate design to address this question? What methods would give reliable data?’’ (Driver et al., 2000, pp. 298 –299). Once the data have been collected, alternative interpretations need to be considered. It is at this point, in particular, where students need to appreciate that scientific theories are human constructs and that they will not generate a theory, or reach a conclusion, by deduction from the data alone.’’ This view echoes that of the teachers in this study. It further implies that students can learn both concepts and the nature of science from rich problem solving activities, without necessarily expecting that students can derive theory from their hands-on investigations. This study therefore suggests that a view of inquiry as application and problem solving after concept introduction may be more viable in the secondary classroom that inquiry as induction of concepts. Exceptions to this generalization could be made for many topics that are amenable to ‘‘discovery,’’ such as those in physics. The results of this study suggest possible new emphases on inquiry in curriculum materials and professional development activities. For example, curriculum activities could suggest a driving question, but allow students the opportunity to design subquestions and investigative procedures. Professional development activities might focus on developing teachers’ core beliefs about the importance of enculturation into scientific practice and argumentation skills as a major focus of science class. The sharing of private learning goals with fellow teachers in learning communities may facilitate teachers to act on core beliefs that run in opposition to the mandated EXPERIENCED SECONDARY SCIENCE TEACHERS’ BELIEFS ABOUT INQUIRY 959 curriculum. At the policy level, school boards should make explicit the value of rich and meaningful learning goals for students by supporting curriculum standards for scientific thinking. Another major implication of this study is for future research on inquiry. The findings suggest that research is needed on student performance related to teacherdesigned inquiry-based instruction. One question raised by this study is whether modified versions of inquiry (e.g., where students do not raise their own investigation questions), such as those designed by the participants will be efficacious in developing students’ nature of science understandings, reasoning, or conceptual knowledge structures. Teachers in this study intuitively believe that inquiry-based activities will promote scientific thinking skills, but as yet they have little evidence to support this hypothesis. Research is needed on several aspects of students’ responses to inquiry in order to justify its current revered status in reform documentation. We need to know more about both the cognitive and affective influences of inquiry on science learning including motivation, science identity, creativity, conceptual growth, scientific thinking, and nature of science understandings. Studies of student response to inquiry should take place in classrooms where teachers are currently implementing inquiry within the constraints of school culture, rather than special camps, classes, or programs. References Anderson, R.D. & Helms, J.V. (2001). The ideal of standards and the reality of schools: Needed research. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 38, 3–16. Brickhouse, N.W. (1990). Teachers’ beliefs about the nature of science and their relationship to classroom practice. Journal of Teacher Education, 41, 53–62. Crawford, B.A. (2000). Embracing the essence of inquiry: New roles for science teachers. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 37, 916–937. Crawford, B.A., Krajcik, J.S., & Marx, R.W. (1999). Elements of a community of learners in a middle school science classroom. Science Education, 83, 701–723. Cronin-Jones, L.L. (1991). Science teacher beliefs and their influence on curriculum implementation: Two case studies. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 28, 235 –250. Driver, R., Newton, P., & Osborne, J. (2000). Establishing the norms of scientific argumentation in classrooms. Science Education, 84, 287–312. Duffee, L. & Aikenhead, G. (1992). Curriculum change, student evaluation, and teacher practical knowledge. Science Education, 76, 493–505. Duschl, R.A. & Wright, E. (1989). A case study of high school teachers’ decision making models for planning and teaching science. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 26, 467– 501. Erickson, F. (1986). Qualitative methods in research on teaching. In M.C. Wittrock (Ed.), Handbook of research on teaching (3rd ed.; pp. 119 –161). New York: Macmillan. Gallagher, J.J. (1991). Prospective and practicing secondary school science teachers’ beliefs about the philosophy of science. Science Education, 75, 121 –133. Gee, J.P. (1990). Social linguistics and literacies: Ideology in discourses. Bristol, PA: Taylor & Francis. Gibson, H.L. & Chase, C. (2002). Longitudinal impact of an inquiry-based science program on middle school students’ attitudes towards science. Science Education, 86, 693 – 705. Glaser, B.G. & Strauss B.G. (1967). The discovery of grounded theory: Strategies for qualitative research. New York: Aldine De Gruyter. Hand, B., Wallace, C.S., & Yang, E.M. (2004). Using the Science Writing Heuristic to enhance learning outcomes from laboratory activities in seventh grade science: 960 WALLACE AND KANG Quantitative and qualitative aspects. International Journal of Science Education, 26, 131– 149. 960 WALLACE AND KANG Haney, J.J., Czerniak, C.M., & Lumpe, A.T. (1996). Teacher beliefs and intentions regarding the implementation of science education reform strands. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 33, 971 –993. Hashweh, M.Z. (1996). Effects of science teachers’ epistemological beliefs in teaching. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 33, 47–63. Huberman, M. & Middlebrooks, S. (2000). The dilution of inquiry: A qualitative study. Qualitative Studies in Education, 13, 281–304. Keys, C.W. & Bryan, L.A. (2001). Co-constructing inquiry-based science with teachers: Essential research for lasting reform. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 38, 631–645. Meijer, P.C. & Verloop, N. (1998, April). A strategy for triangulating multiple source data: Analyzing teachers’ practical knowledge. Presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, San Diego, CA. Miles, M.B. & Huberman, A.M. (1994). Qualitative data analysis: A sourcebook of new methods (2nd ed.). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Millar, R. (1998). Rhetoric and reality: What practical work in science education is really for. In J. Wellington (Ed.), Practical work in science: Which way now? New York: Rutledge. Minstrell, J. & van Zee, E.H. (2000). Inquiring into inquiry learning and teaching in science. Washington, DC: American Association for the Advancement of Science. Munby, H., Cunningham, M., & Lock, C. (2000). School science culture: A case study of barriers to developing professional knowledge. Science Education, 84, 193 –211. National Research Council. (1996). The National Science Education Standards. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Nespor, J. (1987). The role of beliefs in the practice of teaching. Journal of Curriculum Studies, 19, 317 –328. Olson, J. (1981). Teacher influence in the classroom: A context for understanding curriculum translation. Instructional Science, 10, 258–275. Pajares, M.F. (1992). Teachers’ beliefs and educational research: Cleaning up a messy construct. Review of Educational Research, 62, 307–332. Richardson, V. (1996). The role of attitudes and beliefs in learning to teach. In J. Sikula (Ed.), Handbook of research on teacher education (pp. 102 –119). New York: Macmillan. Schwab, J.J. (1962). The teaching of science as enquiry. In The teaching of science. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Tobin, K. & McRobbie, C.J. (1996). Cultural myths as constraints to the enacted science curriculum. Science Education, 80, 223–241. van Driel, J.H., Beijaard, D., & Verloop, N. (2001). Professional development and reform in science education: The role of teachers’ practical knowledge. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 38, 137 –158. Welch, W.W., Klopfer, L.E., Aikenhead, G.S., & Robinson, J.T. (1981). The role of inquiry in science education: Analysis and recommendations. Science Education, 65, 33–50. Wildy, H. & Wallace, J. (1995). Understanding teaching or teaching for understanding: Alternative frameworks for science classrooms. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 32, 143 –156. Yerrick, R., Parke, H., & Nugent, J. (1997). Struggling to promote deeply rooted change: The ‘‘filtering effect’’ of teachers’ beliefs on understanding transformational view of teaching science. Science Education, 81, 137 –157.