Running head: INHIBITION OF RETURN IN SACCADIC SEQUENCES Just Passing Through? Inhibition of Return in Saccadic Sequences W. Joseph MacInnes12, Hannah M. Krüger13 and Amelia R. Hunt1 1. University of Aberdeen School of Psychology William Guild Building Kings College Old Aberdeen AB24 3FX 2. Higher School of Economics (HSE) Faculty of Psychology Moscow, Russian Federation 3. Centre Attention and Vision Laboratoire Psychologie de la Perception Université Paris Descartes, Paris, France Corresponding Author: Dr. W. Joseph MacInnes Assistant Professor, HSE j.macInnes@abdn.ac.uk +7 (495) 709-65-70 Dr. Amelia R. Hunt Senior Lecturer a.hunt@abdn.ac.uk +44 (0)1224 273139 Dr. Hannah M. Krüger Post Doctoral Researcher Hannah.kruger@parisdescartes.fr +33(0)1.42.86.21.99 IOR IN SACCADIC SEQUENCES 2 Abstract Responses tend to be slower to previously fixated spatial locations, an effect known as Inhibition of Return (IOR). Saccades cannot be assumed to be independent, however, and saccade sequences programmed in parallel differ from independent eye movements. We measured the speed of both saccadic and manual responses to probes appearing in previouslyfixated locations when those locations were fixated as part of either parallel or independent saccade sequences. Saccadic IOR was observed in independent but not parallel saccade sequences, while manual IOR was present in both parallel and independent sequence types. Saccadic IOR was also short-lived, and dissipated with delays of more than ~1500ms between the intermediate fixation and the probe onset. The results confirm that the characteristics of IOR depend critically on the response modality used for measuring it, with saccadic and manual responses giving rise to motor and attentional forms of IOR, respectively. Saccadic IOR is relatively short lived and is not observed at intermediate locations of parallel saccade sequences, while attentional IOR is long-lasting and consistent for all sequence types. Keywords: Visual attention, visual search, inhibition of return, saccadic sequences 2 IOR IN SACCADIC SEQUENCES 3 When a location in space is cued, responses to targets appearing in the cued location are speeded relative to control locations. This is thought to occur because attention has been drawn to the location of the cue, facilitating processing of subsequent information appearing in the same location. When the time delay between the onset of the cue and the onset of the target exceeds 200-300ms, however, the effect of the cue reverses, with slower responses to the cued location relative to un-cued locations, and this effect has been termed Inhibition of Return (IOR, Posner & Cohen, 1984). It has been argued that IOR reflects a mechanism that inhibits attention from returning to locations in order to facilitate shifts of attention to novel locations (see Klein, 2000 for a review). IOR is typically tested with simple cues to the left and right, and a return back to fixation prior to probe onset. However, it is rarely the case that in free visual search a single saccade is enough to detect a target and sequences of two or more saccades are common. These sequences of saccades can be broadly categorised into two different types: those that are planned independently and serially, and those that are planned in parallel. In an independent sequence, each saccade is programmed and executed to a discrete, single target. In a parallel sequence, multiple fixations are steps on a path to a single target. Intuitively it may seem that the best strategy in visual search would be to operate entirely with independent saccade sequences, so that each saccade goal is individually fixated and inspected. However, parallel programming of saccades (e.g. Becker & Juergens, 1979) is common in reading (Morrison, 1984) and also comprise a substantial subset of visual search saccade sequences (Findlay, Brown & Gilchrist, 2001; McPeek, Skavenski & Nakayama, 2000). Parallel sequences differ from independent sequences in a number of respects. Parallel saccade sequences have longer primary saccadic latency than saccadic latencies in an independent sequence and this latency is positively correlated with the number of future targets (Inhoff, 1986; Zingale & Kowler, 1987, but see Crawford, 1991). Furthermore, 3 IOR IN SACCADIC SEQUENCES 4 subjects have difficulty changing saccadic vectors to targets in the middle of parallel sequences (De Vries, Hooge &Verstraten, 2014) and longer intermediate saccadic latencies result when they do (McPeek & Keller, 2001). Parallel sequences of saccades have also been shown to interact with saccadic compression. Compression is the mislocalisation of a spatial probe towards the saccade target when the probe is flashed in the brief time interval around the saccade onset (Ross, Morrone & Burr, 1997). Lavergne, Dore- Mazars, Lappe, Lemoine & Vergilino-Perez (2012) demonstrate that localization prior to the initial saccade in parallel sequences is influenced by both saccade targets. Attention has been shown to be automatically and obligatorily allocated to the goal of an impending single saccade (e.g. Hoffman & Subramanian, 1995; Deubel, Bridgeman & Schneider, 1996). This finding has been taken as evidence in favour of a coupling of attention and goal selection, and/or a coupling of attention and saccade planning. Consistent with attention being linked to saccade planning, perceptual benefits associated with attention have been observed at all saccade targets in a sequence of saccades, not just at the final location in the sequence (e.g. Baldauf & Deubel, 2008; Gersch, Schnitzer, Sanghvi, Doscher& Kowler, 2006; Godijn & Theeuwes, 2003) but only when sequence targets are marked by features such as colour (Gersch, Kowler, Schnitzer & Dosher, 2009). For example, Godijn and Theeuwes (2003) used a letter identification task prior to a parallel sequence of saccades. Letter discrimination was better at the first fixation location than the second, but both showed better discrimination than control locations. Other findings show improved contrast detection thresholds at the current fixation and at an intermediate saccade target (Gersch, Kowler, & Dosher, 2004; Gersch et al., 2006) but not the final target location. A difference in the timing of attention probes could explain the discrepancy here; attention may be allocated to all target locations prior to execution, but during the sequence, only the current fixation and the next location in the sequence are attended. 4 IOR IN SACCADIC SEQUENCES 5 If indeed all locations in a saccade sequence are attended equivalently, one might expect IOR to be observed at intermediate locations even for pre-planned sequences of eye movements. On the other hand, an interesting inference that can be drawn from the above is that, even though intermediate locations along a planned path are attended, it would be difficult to change the planned vectors once the parallel sequence of saccades has begun. Presumably, even if a target were detected at an intermediate location in a sequence of saccades, the eyes would normally complete the parallel sequence before returning to the previously-visited location. For inhibitory tags to be functional, intermediate fixations along a planned path should not be inhibited, so that they can be revisited if a target is detected there en route. An important factor in determining whether a previously-fixated location is revisited may be whether or not that location was an intermediate location in a parallel sequence. In order to test our hypothesis, we contrasted two tasks. Participants were instructed with a verbal cue to move their eyes to two locations on the display and respond to a target that could appear in one of two locations: either the intermediate saccade sequence location or a control location that was equidistant but not previously fixated. In the parallel condition the locations of both targets in the sequence were visible before the first eye movement, allowing for pre-planning of both saccades in the sequence. In the independent condition the final saccade target was only revealed after the participant initiated a saccade to the intermediate location, forcing participants to plan and execute the two saccades serially. If IOR is sensitive to the context in which a location is fixated, we expect IOR to be observed at the intermediate location when saccades in a sequence were independent. When sequences are parallel, however, we expect the magnitude of IOR to be reduced or eliminated. 5 IOR IN SACCADIC SEQUENCES 6 Experiment 1: Saccadic Responses Method Participants Ten participants with normal or corrected to normal vision (6 female, average age 25) were recruited from students and staff at Aberdeen University and were paid £5 per hour for their time. All three experiments of the study were approved by University of Aberdeen ethics committee and all participants provided informed consent. Apparatus Experiments were run on a Macintosh PC with 3Ghz quad core processor and 6GB ram running OS 10.6.4 and a 20” CRT monitor at 1024 x 768 resolution and 120 Hz. Eye position was monitored with an Eyelink 1000 (SR research, Mississauga, Canada). The experiment was controlled through Matlab R2009b using Psych toolbox version 3.0.8 (Brainard, 1997). Stimuli and Procedure Each participant contributed 264 trials over two blocks during the 50 minute experiment. The first 15 trials per block were excluded as practice and a further 10% were catch trials where no probe was presented leaving 210 trials available for analysis. The participant’s task was to make two saccades and then make a third, speeded saccade to a visual probe. Trials were initiated by the participant with a space bar press which triggered an Eyelink drift correction. Participants were then presented with a central fixation dot surrounded by six marked locations (disks with a 1˚ diameter) equally spaced as if on a circle of 4˚ radius from the fixation (see Figure 1 for layout and timing). In each trial, three of six locations were randomly chosen with the caveat that no two would be adjacent. These three locations were randomly assigned as final saccade location, intermediate saccade location and 6 IOR IN SACCADIC SEQUENCES 7 Figure 1. Spatial and temporal properties of the experiment design. The timeline at the top shows the sequence and timing for all experiment conditions. The layout for (A) parallel and (B) independent sequences were different prior to the onset of the first saccade but identical from that point on. Both had equivalent onsets (50% black mask) in mid saccade. Manual and saccadic experiments were identical except for the modality of the response. control location. Each of these locations was further randomly assigned one of the colours red, green or blue. In parallel sequence trials, all three of the assigned colours were visible at the start of the trial. In independent sequence trials only the colour for the intermediate location was visible. Background was uniform grey and all non-coloured locations remained black throughout the experiment. Randomization meant all six locations and three colours 7 IOR IN SACCADIC SEQUENCES 8 were equally likely as the intermediate saccade location, with the final saccade location being clockwise or counter-clockwise by two locations. The control location was always equidistant in the opposite direction. Each participant did one block of parallel saccade sequences and one block of independent saccade sequence, with order alternating between subjects. All trials began with a 500 ms delay followed by verbal cues announcing the colours of the first and second saccade location, for example “blue”, “red”. Participants were instructed not to begin their first saccade until the central fixation was removed. The central fixation was removed 1500 ms into the trial and participants were given an error tone for moving early. During independent sequences, each trial started with the colour of the initial saccade target visible, but the colour of all other potential saccade targets was hidden. Our methods required that these locations reveal their colour after the onset of the first saccade so that planning and execution of the saccade to the second location could begin only after the first saccade had finished. We timed this colour change to coincide with the eye movement to minimize the possibility that it would attract attention, but to further equate the conditions we also used partial masks in both parallel and independent conditions, as follows. In independent trials, upon detection of a saccade toward the first cued location, colours for both hidden potential locations were revealed to show their target colour, but with 50% of the surface covered by a black mask. In both parallel and independent trials, any location whose colour was initially visible also received a black mask covering 50% of the surface. The end result was all three locations in both parallel and independent conditions receiving a 50% surface change in luminance in mid saccade, and the final locations being a 50% mask/colour blend. The use of these masks meant that any local transients due to colour onsets were matched across parallel and independent conditions. 8 IOR IN SACCADIC SEQUENCES 9 A random time (500 to 1300 ms) after landing at the second cued location, a probe (a white cross of .66˚ in the centre of the location) was presented at either the intermediate location or the alternate control location with equal probability. Ten percent of trials were catch trials and had no probe. Participants were instructed to respond as quickly and accurately as possible to the probe location by moving their eyes to that location. Responses before the probe or on a catch trial received an error tone as feedback. All potential locations were circles extending 1.0˚ diameter and any saccade within 1.5˚ radius of its centre was considered a ‘hit’ of that location. Saccadic onset events were defined by the Eyelink default threshold settings of 40˚/s velocity and 8000˚/s2 acceleration. Results and Discussion Trials were excluded from analysis if the participant blinked (2.1% of trials), or was not able to maintain fixation (that is, remain within 1.5˚ of screen centre) prior to removal of fixation (12.5%). One participant was removed from the analysis for having over 50% exclusions on these bases. Participants did not respond to any catch trials. Other exclusions were trials with saccadic reaction times (SRT) of less than 100ms (0.2%), or greater than three standard deviations from the mean (1.8%), and trials during which any fixation landed within 1.5˚ of the alternate location (1.6%) or failed to reach a target in a single saccade (1.8%). Data were collapsed across the six locations, three location colours and two saccade directions from the intermediate location (clockwise and counter clockwise). Because we were interested in the time course of IOR, we included the fixation-probe onset asynchrony (FPOA) as a factor in the analysis. We measured probe onset time from the fixation onset of the intermediate location because this best represents the interval from the time the inhibition 9 IOR IN SACCADIC SEQUENCES 10 would have been applied to that location to the time when the probe appeared, analogous to the cue-target interval in more conventional cue-target paradigms. Saccadic reaction time (SRT) was defined as the saccade onset latency timed from the onset of the probe and was analyzed with a linear mixed effects (lmer) model in the R statistical package. Linear mixed effects modelling allows for any combination of random and fixed effects in a single model. In addition, they provide an intuitive approach to continuous variables in the analysis and figures which provides a richer representation of the data as compared to median splits (Bayaan, Davidson & Bates, 2008). IOR (intermediate, control) and Sequence (parallel, independent) were included as fixed effects and Participant and FPOA (a continuous factor) as random effects. The significance of each effect was determined by comparing the model with and without main effects or interactions to see if its inclusion improved the model as measured by a Chi squared (2) test. We observed a main effect of IOR (2 (1) = 26.2, p < .001) with slower SRT to intermediate locations than to control locations. This classic IOR effect interacted separately with Sequence (2 (1) = 4.0, p < .05) and FPOA (2 (1) = 10.5, p < .01). Planned comparison confirmed what is evident in Figure 2a, that the interaction of Sequence and IOR was caused by a significant IOR effect (32ms) for probes appearing in the intermediate location in independent sequences (t (9) = 3.5, p < .01). IOR in the intermediate location in parallel sequences was not significant in a paired t-test (t (9) = 1.4). IOR was also observed at intermediate locations for early probes (less than 1500 ms after landing at the intermediate saccade location; 32 ms IOR (t (9) = 3.9, p < .01) but not for late probes (those presented more than 1500 ms after landing; 10 ms IOR t (9) = 1.7, p < .13)) and this difference disappeared at the latest times as seen in Figure 2. No three way interaction was observed (F < 1), suggesting the data are better explained by the separate two-way interactions when the model includes the variances from all factors. 10 IOR IN SACCADIC SEQUENCES 11 Figure 2. Saccadic Reaction times for E1 showing separate interactions of a) IOR and sequence type and b) IOR and fixation-probe onset asynchrony. Error bars represent Fischer’s Least Significant Difference. Fixation durations at the intermediate location were measured to see if longer dwell times in independent sequences could explain why we only observed IOR in the independent condition. If independent sequences had longer dwell times, then perhaps they were better attended, leading to the inhibition. However, there was no difference in total dwell time at the intermediate location between parallel and independent sequences (t (9) < 1.0). Participants would occasionally make a number of small fixational saccades at this intermediate location (mean of 1.3) before progressing to the second location, so we compared the length of the first fixation duration, the number of fixational saccades (< 1˚), and the total dwell time at the intermediate location. There were no differences in any of these measures between parallel and independent sequences (all t (9) < 1.0). The inhibitory cost at intermediate locations for short probe onsets also remained significant when only trials where the intermediate location had a single fixation were included in the analysis (i.e. even when no additional fixational saccades were made) (t (9) = 3.45, p < .01). The failure to find differences between serial and parallel conditions in terms of fixation behaviour at the intermediate location may initially seem to contradict previous reports described in the introduction, in which the shorter-duration fixations at intermediate 11 IOR IN SACCADIC SEQUENCES 12 locations are taken as a hallmark of saccade planning (e.g. McPeek et al., 2000; Findlay et al., 2001). However, it is important to note that in our experiment the participant had to wait for 1500ms after the first target was revealed before beginning the sequence of saccades in both conditions, which could have minimized later differences in the temporal dynamics of the fixation behaviour. Another relevant difference in our experiment relative to previous ones is that here the intermediate location was explicitly pre-defined as a saccade target, and was not an erroneous saccade to a distractor as it was in the previous studies mentioned above, which could have caused participants to fixate it for longer. The fact that we found clear differences in IOR between the serial and parallel conditions is in itself reasonable evidence that participants did, in fact, plan their saccade path differently under the two conditions. These results demonstrate that saccadic IOR is absent or masked at intermediate fixations along a parallel sequence of saccades. We observed robust IOR when the conditions were the same except the second saccade in the sequence could not be planned in advance, providing clear evidence that parallel-planning was the key factor in reducing IOR. The IOR we observed was short-lived and was no longer significant at late onsets. Experiment 2: Manual Responses IOR can be measured using saccadic responses, as it was in Experiment 1, but it is also frequently measured using manual responses (i.e., pressing a button to indicate that the target has appeared). IOR has different characteristics depending on which response modality is used (Hunt & Kingstone, 2003; Taylor & Klein, 2000). IOR for saccadic responses has been modelled in terms of short-term cue-related changes in the responses of sensory and motor neurons in the superficial and intermediate layers of the superior colliculus (SC), a subcortical structure associated with the generation of eye movements (e.g. Satel, Wang, Trappenberg & Klein, 2011). Consistent with this, saccadic IOR (but not manual IOR) 12 IOR IN SACCADIC SEQUENCES 13 interacts with the effects of fixation offset (Hunt & Kingstone, 2003), which is also thought to arise from activity in the SC. In contrast, IOR as measured by manual responses has been associated with an “attentional” form of IOR, that is, an impediment in shifts of attention to previously-visited locations, with a locus in cortical areas linked with attention and perception (Klein, 2000 review). Consistent with this, manual IOR (but not saccadic IOR) interacts with target luminance (Hunt and Kingstone, 2003; Reuter-Lorenz, Jha and Rosenquist, 1996). Thus multiple sources of IOR may exist in parallel, and response modality is one way of dissociating them. Given the dissociation between saccadic and manual IOR, it is reasonable to ask whether the results from Experiment 1 would extend to manual responses. Since attention is allocated in parallel to all saccade targets prior to execution (Baldauf & Deubel, 2008), attentional inhibition, as measured by manual responses to probes, may exist for both sequence types. Attentional IOR has also been shown to be long lasting (3200ms for manual responses; Samual & Kat, 2003 and at least 1500ms for saccadic responses; Klein, 2000) and have late onset depending on the complexity of the task (Lupianez, Milliken, Solano, Weaver & Tipper, 2001). We therefore examined the effect of parallel planning on IOR for manual responses in this experiment, using a variety of saccade target intervals (STI) ranging from 500 to 1700 ms. Method Participants Twenty-two participants (15 female, average age 23) participated in this experiment and were reimbursed £5 for their time. 13 IOR IN SACCADIC SEQUENCES 14 Apparatus, Stimuli and Procedures Experiment 2 was identical to Experiment 1 except that participants were instructed to make a button press upon target detection. In addition, the first group (N = 12) of our participants were run with probe onset times that matched Experiment 1 (500-1300ms). There were no significant effects of FPOA on manual IOR, but we were concerned that the later onset of manual responses relative to saccadic responses could be responsible for the difference between the two experiments. We therefore decided to change the range of probe onsets to 900-1700ms for the remaining participants (N=10). This also had the benefit of providing a better match for manual IOR in similar designs (e.g., Taylor and Klein (2000) measured IOR at 1000 ms probe onset). FPOA had no effect in this group of participants either, so in the subsequent analyses, data from both groups of participants were combined to maximize power, with FPOA as a continuous variable as measured from the onset of the fixation at the intermediate location. Results & Discussion Trials were excluded if the participant blinked (2.1% of trials) or was not able to maintain fixation (that is, remain within 1.5˚ of screen centre) prior to its removal (16%). Catch trials were also excluded from analysis and responses were made to 0.1% of these. Other exclusions were manual reaction times (MRT) of less than 100ms (0.1%), or more than three standard deviations from the mean (1.0%), trials during which any fixation landed within 1.5˚ of the alternate location (3.0%) or failed to reach a target (1.5%).) 14 IOR IN SACCADIC SEQUENCES 15 Figure 3. Manual reaction times for E2. Only the main effect of IOR was significant and it did not interact with any other factors. The graph is split by planning condition for comparison to SRT data only. Error bars represent Fischer’s Least Significant Difference. A mixed-effects model was conducted with subject and FPOA as random effects and IOR (intermediate, control) and Sequence (parallel, independent) as fixed effects. The model was optimized as described above and showed a main effect of IOR (2 (1) = 10.1, p < .001) with a 13 ms cost to probes at the intermediate location. We observed a main effect of FPOA ((2 (1) = 4.1, p < .05) with late onsets having slower response times, and a significant effect of Sequence (2 (1) = 4.3, p < .05) with responses following independent sequences 7 ms faster than parallel. IOR did not interact with any factors (all F < 1.0). These results suggest that IOR, as measured by manual responses to probes, is observed at intermediate locations along the saccade path irrespective of whether or not the sequence of saccades was preplanned in parallel or had to be planned and executed serially. 15 IOR IN SACCADIC SEQUENCES 16 Experiment 3: Mixed Responses The above experiments taken together suggest a different effect of saccade planning for IOR measured by saccadic and manual responses. A different time course for IOR effects as measured by the two response modalities was also evident. In a final experiment, we sought to replicate these two effects within the same study, and within the same group of participants, to allow for a more direct and powerful comparison of IOR across modality. Method Participants Eleven participants (6 female, average age 21) participated in this experiment and were reimbursed £5 for their time. Apparatus, Stimuli and Procedures Experiment 3 was identical to earlier experiments except each participant contributed four blocks, two each of manual and saccadic responses, to allow for a within-subject comparison of response modality. Probe onset time was either 750 or 1150 ms after the end of the second saccade. Results & Discussion Trials were excluded if the participant blinked (3.9%) or was not able to maintain fixation (1.5˚) prior to its removal (7.6%). Catch trials were also excluded from analysis and responses were made to 0.1% of these. Other exclusions were manual reaction times (MRT) of less than 100ms (0.2%) or more than three standard deviations from the mean (0.9%), and trials during which any fixation landed within 1.5˚ of the alternate location (2.0%) or failed to reach a target or probe location in a single saccade (9.1%). 16 IOR IN SACCADIC SEQUENCES 17 First, to replicate the analyses of previous experiments, a mixed-effects model was conducted separately for manual and saccadic responses with subject and probe onset time (as measured from the intermediate location) as random effects and IOR (intermediate, control) and Sequence (parallel, independent) as fixed effects. Probe onset time was triggered at either 750ms and 1150ms after completion the saccade to the second location, but as in the previous two experiments, our fixation-probe onset asynchrony (FPOA) variable for the analysis was the duration between the start of the fixation on the intermediate location and the onset of the probe. FPOA was therefore treated as a continuous variable in the linear effects model. For saccadic responses, there were main effects of IOR (2 (1) = 4.4, p < .05) with responses to intermediate locations 13 ms slower than control, and FPOA (2 (1) = 10.7, p < .01) with reaction times slowed for late probe onsets (note that this is opposite to the results from Experiment 1, but consistent with the results from Experiment 2). FPOA interacted with Sequence (2 (1) = 11.5, p < .001) and there was a three-way interaction between FPOA, IOR and Sequence (2 (1) = 6.2, p < .05). To analyse this further, we examined the FPOA by saccadic IOR interaction for independent and parallel sequences separately: For independent sequences, IOR was significant for early target onsets t (10) = 3.2, p < .01), but not later target onsets (t (10) = 0.5) reflecting the presence of IOR for early but not late probe onsets. No IOR was observed for parallel sequences (early target onsets t (10) = 0.4), late target onsets (t (10) = 2.0) with the latter being a non significant trend toward cuing, not inhibition. 17 IOR IN SACCADIC SEQUENCES 18 Figure 4. Graphs depicting IOR for increasing fixation-probe onset asynchrony (FPOA) following the intermediate saccade (X axis) for manual/saccadic responses and parallel/ independent sequences. IOR was observed across all conditions for manual responses (A); there was no interaction with sequence type or FPOA and the manual data is split by sequence type for comparison only. For saccadic responses, IOR was not observed for parallel sequences (B) and observed only for early onsets in independent sequences (C). Error bar shading represents 95% confidence interval for the linear approximation. For manual responses, we observed slower responses to late onsets (2 (1) = 12.4, p < .001) and slower responses in independent sequences (2 (1) = 22.7, p < .001). Consistent with Experiment 2, IOR was significant (2 (1) = 7.2, p < .01), and did not interact with either FPOA (F=1.9), Sequence type (F < 1.0) or the two together (F < 1.0). A mixed model of the full data set (E3 manual and saccadic responses) was run as a direct test for an IOR interaction with response modality. The main effect of IOR remained significant (2 (1) = 9.5, p < .01) and the expected four way interaction (IOR, Sequence, 18 IOR IN SACCADIC SEQUENCES 19 FPOA and Modality) was also significant (2 (1) = 6.6, p < .01), confirming the pattern described above for the two response modalities: saccadic IOR is only observed for independent sequences and early probes, but manual IOR is observed across sequence types and probe onset times. General Discussion The results demonstrate that saccades to a previously-fixated location are inhibited only when that location was part of a series of independently planned and executed saccades, and only when that location is probed soon after the saccade sequence has finished. Locations fixated as part of a parallel sequence of saccades, or locations probed more than 1500ms after the intermediate saccade, were not significantly inhibited. For manual responses to probes, we observed robust IOR for intermediate fixations following both parallel and independent sequences of saccades, and at all time intervals following the sequence. Below we discuss the implications for each response modality separately and then in combination. Saccadic IOR In Experiment 1, when sequences of two saccades were programmed and executed serially (the “independent” condition), saccadic responses to intermediate locations along the saccade path were slowed relative to novel locations, confirming the presence of IOR. However, IOR was not observed for intermediate locations along the saccade path when the two saccades in the sequence could be planned in advance. Furthermore, saccadic IOR was only present when probes appeared relatively soon after the end of the saccade sequence (less than 900ms), but not if the probe appeared late. It is important to note that the conditions under which we observed saccadic IOR generally match the basic parameters of a typical cueing experiment, in which a peripheral cue is followed by a delay (typically 500-1000ms) 19 IOR IN SACCADIC SEQUENCES 20 or a central reorienting event, and then a probe appears in either the cued location or the opposite location. Therefore our results are consistent with the literature on the whole, but demonstrate some important boundary conditions on saccadic IOR effects. Although we did not test earlier than 500ms after the end of the second saccade in the sequence, the relatively early detection of saccadic inhibition is consistent with previous research (Hooge & Frens, 2000). The fact that saccadic IOR diminished over time, and was no longer significant by around 1500ms after the initial fixation on the intermediate location, matches other reports in the literature (e.g. Fecteau and Munoz, 2005; Pertzov, Zohary & Avidan, 2010), and is also consistent with a recent model of saccadic IOR as reflecting shortterm depression of sensory sensitivity within the intermediate layers of the SC (Satel, Wang, Trappenberg & Klein, 2011). The transient nature of saccadic IOR that we observe suggests that inhibition may only persist to two or three subsequent eye movements, although other forms of IOR could affect responses over a longer timeframe. One possible interpretation of the slower responses to intermediate locations in the independent sequence of saccades is that it may not reflect IOR at all, but rather saccadic momentum (Smith & Henderson, 2009), that is, a tendency to continue forward rather than a delay in shifting backward. This seems unlikely, given that the intermediate location was a reversal of the current trajectory, and the control location against which it was compared was closer to a return vector than a forward vector. But a momentum account could predict a faster response to the control location, if there exists a gradient of facilitation that is strongest for a forward vector and weakest for a reversed vector. However, any momentum that would be reflected in this measure would be expected to be quite weak. Probes at the forward location or at two back saccadic locations could differentiate between an IOR and momentum account of these data, but our current design does not support these conditions. In any case, our independent condition, in which we also observed slower responses to the previously- 20 IOR IN SACCADIC SEQUENCES 21 visited location, matches the “one-back” location measured in previous experiments examining IOR in natural search. Saccadic momentum has been convincingly ruled out as an explanation for the inhibitory effects in these conditions (Bays & Husain, 2012). It is important to note that a lack of measured IOR does not mean the underlying mechanism is absent. Some authors have suggested that inhibitory and facilitative effects could coexist (Tipper et al., 1999; Wascher, & Tipper, 2004). In other words, the fact that IOR was not observed could be due to the expression of a facilitative effect that counteracts the inhibition. Saccadic IOR seems to be generated by the selection of a final motor goal. Several recent models of IOR suggest it is generated at multiple, independent sites, including both within the Superior Colliculus (Satel et al., 2011) and upstream from the SC (e.g. Dorris, Klein, Everling & Munoz., 2002). The Posterior Parietal Cortex (PPC) plays a role in attention (Kastner & Ungerleider, 2000, Buschman & Miller, 2007) and preparation of goaldirected movements (Snyder, Batista & Anderson, 1997). The PPC has also been found to transform object representations from retinotopic to motor coordinates (Colby & Goldberg, 1999; Buneo, Jarvis, Batista & Anderson, 2002) and it has been suggested to incorporate spatiotopic information through gain fields (Anderson & Mountcastle, 1985). Therefore, its involvement would explain findings of IOR in a spatiotopic reference frame (Krüger & Hunt, 2012; Mathot & Theeuwes, 2010; Pertzov, et al., 2010). Coordinate transformations and IOR in PPC would be based on locations and vectors involved at the time of saccadic programming, and for parallel sequences, these saccades are programmed simultaneously at the initial fixation location. PPC’s involvement in IOR is consistent with recent EEG studies that see a delayed N2PC (an event related potential (ERP) likely to originate from the PPC) for probes at valid locations (Yang, Yao, Ding, Qi & Lei,2012; but see McDonald, Hickey, Green & Whitman, 2009). 21 IOR IN SACCADIC SEQUENCES 22 One can speculate about the implications of these results for eye movements during free search. Parallel sequences of saccades are frequent (Findlay, et al., 2001; McPeek, et al., 2000), do more work from the primary fixation (Inhoff, 1986; Zingale & Kowler, 1987) and are difficult to cancel from the intermediate location(De Vries, et al., 2014). This suggests that planning for both saccades is done primarily at the initial fixation and no further saccadic planning occurs at the intermediate fixation locations. If this is the case, information that is gained while an oculomotor program is being carried out will not be able to influence subsequent saccadic selection processes until after the current sequence is completed. The lack of inhibition at these “drive-by” locations could help make search more efficient: fixation locations can be selected in parallel, executed in sequence, and still revisited efficiently if necessary. Manual IOR In Experiment 2, manual responses to probes appearing at the previously fixated location were delayed compared to novel locations, confirming the presence of IOR at intermediate locations along a sequence of saccades. Unlike for saccadic responses, IOR was observed equally in both parallel and independent sequences, and was long lasting. Manual responses have been suggested to elicit an attentional or cortical component of IOR based on their interaction with other attention effects, while saccadic responses have been associated with the superior colliculus component, based on their interaction with oculomotor effects (Hunt & Kingstone, 2003). This framework suggests that the manual responses in our experiment were more influenced by where attention has previously been focused, while the saccadic responses were influenced by saccade planning and motor factors. Our results are therefore in line with the literature that suggests attention is allocated to all saccade locations before execution of the first saccade of a sequence (Godijn & 22 IOR IN SACCADIC SEQUENCES 23 Theeuwes, 2003) and during execution at the current fixation location and the next-in-line saccade target (Gersch et al., 2004). Since all locations are attended, regardless of planning, our manual response result likely reflects an inhibition of attention to return to the previously fixated location. Additionally, unlike with saccadic IOR, we observed manual IOR at late probe onsets, which is consistent with the proposal that IOR is a long-lasting attentional inhibitory tag which facilitates visual search (Klein & MacInnes, 1999; Klein, 2000, Samuel & Kat, 2003). Coming together: Two types of IOR Consistent with other results (Taylor & Klein, 2000; Hunt & Kingstone, 2003), we observed different patterns of IOR depending on which response modality was used. Saccadic IOR was observed only for independent sequences and early probe onsets, whereas manual IOR was consistent regardless of sequence type and probe onset time. Based on the premise that attention is allocated to all the targets along the saccade path, we suggest that IOR, when measured by saccades, is locked to the saccade planning process, while IOR as measured by manual responses is generated wherever attention resides. IOR can be generated by oculomotor planning, giving rise to oculomotor IOR, and IOR can also be generated by shifts of covert attention, giving rise to attentional IOR. In both cases, for IOR to reflect its origins, a response modality must be used that is sensitive to the type of IOR generated. In the current experiment, these two components of IOR were presumably both generated at the intermediate location in the independent condition. In the planned condition, no oculomotor was applied at the intermediate location, leaving only attentional IOR. Another promising framework for understanding IOR suggests that the form it takes depends on whether or not the oculomotor system is, or has recently been, inhibited. Based on a meticulous review of the literature, Klein and Hilchey (2012) argue that IOR reflects an 23 IOR IN SACCADIC SEQUENCES 24 inhibition of attention from the previously cued location if, and only if, the oculomotor system is inhibited. When the oculomotor system has been activated (or there is no need to inhibit it), IOR reflects a motor bias, that is, an inhibition of response in the direction of the cue. In the context of the current results, it could be argued that the oculomotor system is activated for both manual and saccadic responses, since it has just been engaged in the process of executing the sequence of saccades, which would suggest both experiments should elicit a motor form of IOR according to Klein and Hilchey’s taxonomy. However, the oculomotor system would need to be inhibited in the final stages of the manual IOR task, since the instruction is to maintain fixation and not saccade to the final probe, which could provide the necessary conditions for generating an attentional form of IOR. Alternatively, response modality per se could be a key factor (Taylor & Klein, 2000; Hunt & Kingstone, 2003) or perhaps the two forms of IOR co-exist (Kingstone & Pratt, 1999), and our results represent a combination of saccadic and attentional IOR. The finding that saccadic IOR is absent in parallel sequences provides insight into the function of IOR in visual search. Klein (1988) and Klein and MacInnes (1999) have postulated that IOR reflects a mechanism that biases saccades away from previously fixated locations and towards novel locations. While this seems to hold for a majority of saccades there has been some debate over those saccades that do return to recent locations (Hooge, et al., 2005; Smith & Henderson 2009, 2011; but see Bays & Husain, 2012). In the current study we have found that saccadic IOR is reduced at an intermediate location within a parallel saccadic sequence. Given that dwell times at intermediate saccade locations are reduced in search (McPeek& Keller., 2001) and the intermediate saccade cannot be easily cancelled (DeVries, et al., 2014; McPeek & Keller, 2001), it would not be functional for such saccades to be inhibited. It is possible that the reduction in IOR for return saccades after a parallel 24 IOR IN SACCADIC SEQUENCES 25 sequence can account for at least some of the return saccades that are observed during visual search. The time course of the two forms of IOR in the current results suggest that saccadic IOR is more likely to inhibit responses to recently fixated locations as it is short lasting, while attentional IOR could be more dominant over a longer time frame, as it persisted over the late intervals tested (900+ ms). During a natural search process, saccadic IOR could therefore be expected to prevent return saccades to locations that were already selected as the final saccade goal, but this inhibition is short lived and not applied to locations that were fixated along the way to that goal. Attentional IOR could play a role in inhibiting attention from returning to all locations of a saccade sequence, and it could prevent returns to locations fixated two or more previously. The current results therefore suggest that in natural search, refixations of previous locations would be more likely when that location was part of a parallel sequence of saccades, and the response to that location is executed soon after the sequence is completed. Given that visual search encompasses many strategies incorporating both of these patterns of sequences, it is not surprising that detecting evidence of IOR in free search garners noisy results. One final important additional implication of this result is that the intermediate location can be inhibited for attention, but not for oculomotor planning and execution. As such, our findings are consistent with other results demonstrating independence of saccade planning and attention (Klein, 1980; Hunt and Kingstone, 2003), and are inconsistent with the premotor theory of attention, which proposes that attention and saccade planning are the same process. Acknowledgements: This research was supported by BBSRC research grant BB/H01280X/1 and the James S. McDonnell Foundation (ARH). 25 IOR IN SACCADIC SEQUENCES 26 References Andersen, R.A., & Mountcastle, V.B. (1983). The influence of the angle of gaze upon the excitability of the light- sensitive neurons of the posterior parietal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 3, 532–548. Bays, P.M. & Husain, M. (2012). Active inhibition and memory promote exploration and search of natural scenes. Journal of Vision, 12(8), 1-18. Baldauf, D., & Deubel, H. (2008). Properties of attentional selection during the preparation of sequential saccades. Experimental Brain Research, 184(3), 411–425. Becker, W. & Jürgens, R.. An analysis of the saccadic system by means of double step stimuli. Vision Research 19(9), 967–983. Brainard, D.H. (1997) The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10(4),433-436. Buneo, C.A., Jarvis, M.R., Batista,A.P. & Anderson,R.A. (2002) Direct visuomotor transformations for reaching. Nature. 416, 632-636. Buschman, T.J. & Miller, E.K. (2007). Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices. Science, 315(5820), 1860-1862. Crawford, T.J. (1991). Multi-stepping saccadic sequences in humans. Acta Psychologica, 76, 11–29. Colby & Goldberg (1999). Space and attention in parietal cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 22, 319-349. De Vries, J.P, Hooge, I., & Verstraten, F. (2014). Saccades toward the target are planned as sequences rather than as single steps. Psychological Science. 25(1), 215-223. Deubel, H., Bridgeman, B. & Schneider, W.X. (1996). Postsaccadic target blanking prevents saccadic suppression of image displacement. Vision Research, 36(7), 985-996. 26 IOR IN SACCADIC SEQUENCES 27 Dodd, M., Van der Stigchel, S. & Hollingworth, A.(2009). Novelty is not always the best policy: inhibition of return and facilitation of return as a function of visual task. Psychological Science, 20(3), 333-339. Dorris, M., Klein, R., Everling, S., & Munoz, D.(2002). Contribution of the primate superior colliculus to inhibition of return. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(8), 1256-1263. Fecteau, J. H., & Munoz, D. P. (2005). Correlates of capture of attention and inhibition of return across stages of visual processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(11), 1714-1727. Findlay JM, Brown V, Gilchrist ID. (2001). Saccade target selection in visual search: the effect of information from the previous fixation. Vision Research. 41(1):87-95. Gersch, T.M., Kowler, E. & Dosher, B. (2004). Dynamic allocation of visual attention during the execution of sequences of saccades. Vision Research, 44(12), 1469–1483. Gersch, T., Schnitzer, B., Sanghvi, P. Doscher, B & Kowler,K. (2006). Attentional enhancement along the path of a sequence of saccades. Journal of Vision, 6(6), 89. Gersch, T., Kowler, E., Schnitzer, B. & Dosher, B. (2009) Attention during sequences of saccades along marked and memorized paths. Vision research, 49, 1256-1266. Godijn, R. & Theeuwes, J. (2003). Parallel allocation of attention prior to the execution of saccade sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 882–896. Hoffman, J.E. & Subramanian, B. (1995). The role of visual attention in saccadic eye movements. Perception & Psychophysics, 57(6), 787-795. Hooge, I. & Frens, M. (2000) Inhibition of saccade return (ISR): spatio-temporal properties of saccade programming. Vision Research, 40, 3415-3426. Hooge, I. T., Over, E. A., van Wezel, R. J., & Frens, M. A., 2005. Inhibition of return is not a foraging facilitator in saccadic search and free viewing. Vision Research, 45, 1901-1908. 27 IOR IN SACCADIC SEQUENCES 28 Hunt, A. & Kingstone, A. (2003). Inhibition of return: dissociating attentional and oculomotor components. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 1068-1074. Inhoff, A.W. (1986). Preparing sequences of saccades under choice reaction conditions: effects of sequence length and context. Acta Psychologica, 61(3), 211-228. Kastner, S. & Ungerleider, L. G. (2000). Mechanism of visual attention in the human cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 23, 315-341. Kingstone, A. & Pratt, J. (1999). Inhibition of return is composed of attentional and oculomotor processes. Perception and Psychophysics, 61(6), 1046-1054. Klein, R.M. & Hilchey, M. Oculomotor inhibition of return.(2012) In S. Liversedge, I. Gilchrist & S. Everling.(Eds.), Oxford Handbook of Eye Movements (471-492).London, UK: Oxford University Press. Klein, R.M., & MacInnes, W.J. (1999). Inhibition of return is a foraging facilitator in visual search. Psychological Science, 10, 346–352. Klein, R.M. (1988). Inhibitory tagging system facilitates visual search. Nature, 334, 430431. Klein, R.M. (2000). Inhibition of return. Trends in Cognitive Science, 4(4), 138-147. Krueger, H. M. & Hunt, A.R. (2012). Inhibition across eye movements and object movements: the role of prediction. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. Lavergne, L., Vergilino-Perez, D., Collins, T., Orriols, E. & Doré-Mazars, K. (2008). The planning of a sequence of saccades in pro- and antisaccade tasks: influence of visual integration time and concurrent motor processing. Brain Research, 1245, 82-95. 28 IOR IN SACCADIC SEQUENCES 29 Lupianez, J. Milliken, B., Solano, C., Weaver, B. & Tipper, P. (2001). On the strategic modulation of the time course of facilitation and inhibition of return. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 54(3), 753-773. Mathot, S. & Theeuwes, J. (2010). Gradual Remapping Results in Early Retinotopic and Late Spatiotopic Inhibition of Return. Psychological Science, 21(12), 1793-1798. McDonald, J.J., Hickey,C., Green, J.J. & Whitman, J.C. (2009). Inhibition of return in the covert deployment of attention: evidence from human electrophysiology. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(4), 725-733. MacInnes, W.J., & Klein, R. M. (2003). Inhibition of return biases orienting during the search of complex scenes. The Scientific World Journal, 3, 75–86. McPeek, R.M. & Keller, E.L. (2001) Short-term priming, concurrent processing, and saccade curvature during a target selection task in the monkey. Vision Research, 41,785–800. McPeek, R.M., Skavenski, A.A.. & Nakayama, K. (2000). Concurrent processing of saccades in visual search. Vision Research, 40(18), 2499-2516. Morrison, R.E. (1984) Manipulation of stimulus onset delay in reading: Evidence for parallel programming of saccades. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10(5), 667-682. Pertzov, Y., Zohary, E. & Avidan, G. (2010) Rapid Formation of Spatiotopic Representations As Revealed by Inhibition of Return. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(26),8882-8887. Posner, M.I. & Cohen, Y. (1984) Components of visual orienting. In Attention and Performance Vol. X (Bouma, H. and Bouwhuis, D., eds), pp. 531–556, Erlbaum Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., Jha, A. P., & Rosenquist, J. N. (1996). What is inhibited in inhibition of return. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22(2), 367. 29 IOR IN SACCADIC SEQUENCES 30 Ross, J., Morrone, M.C. & Burr, D.C. (1997). Compression of visual space before saccades. Nature, 386(6625),598-601. Samuel, A.G & Kat, D. (2003). Inhibition of return: A graphical meta-analysis of its time course and an empirical test of its temporal and spatial properties. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10(4), 897-906. Satel, J., Wang, Z., Trappenberg, T. P., & Klein, R. M. (2011). Modeling inhibition of return as short-term depression of early sensory input to the superior colliculus. Vision Research, 51(9), 987-996. Smith, T. J., & Henderson, J. M. (2009). Facilitation of return during scene viewing. Visual Cognition, 17, 1083–1108. Smith, T. J. & Henderson, J. M. (2011). Looking back at Waldo: oculomotor inhibition of return does not prevent return fixations. Journal of Vision, 11(1), 1-11. Snyder, L.H., Batista, A.P. & Anderson, R.A., (1997). Coding of intention in the posterior parietal cortex. Nature, 386(6621), 167-170. Taylor, T.L. & Klein, R.M. (2000). Visual and motor effects in inhibition of return. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26 (5), 1639-1656. Thomas,L. E., Ambinder, M.S., Hsieh, B., Levinthal, B., Crowell, J.A., ... Wang, F.R. (2006). Fruitful visual search:inhibition of return in a virtual foraging task. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13 (5), 891-951. Tipper, S., Jordan, H. & Weaver, B. (1999). Scene-based and object-centered inhibition of return: Evidence for dual orienting mechanisms. Perception and Psychophysics, 61(1), 50-60. Wascher, E. & Tipper, S.P. (2004). Revealing effects of non-informative spatial cues: An EEG-study of inhibition of return. Psychophysiology, 41, 716-728. 30 IOR IN SACCADIC SEQUENCES 31 Yang, D., Yao,S., Ding, C.,, Qi, S.& Lei, Y. (2012). Electrophysiological evidence for inhibition of return effect in exogenous orienting. Experimental Brain Research, 22(3), 279-285. Zingale, C.M.& Kowler, E.(1987). Planning sequences of saccades. Vision Research, 27(8), 1327-1341. 31