RUNNING HEAD: Student’s Academic Expectations 1 Sociology Report: Student’s Academic Expectations in Regards to Howard Gardner’s Theory of Multiple Intelligences Vincent Palmeri John Jay College of Criminal Justice Sociology Report 2 Abstract Sociology Report 3 Introduction Harvard graduate and Professor of Education, Howard Gardner is credited for pioneering the Theory of Multiple Intelligences. “Gardner proposed seven different domains of intelligence, each of which operates more or less independently. That is, a person can be high or low in any intelligence, regardless of his or her level on the other six domains” (Mettetal, 1997, p. 115). Different studies and researchers suggest that there may be more than seven intelligences (Barrington, 2004; Stanford, 2003); however, the seven being discussed for this experiment are commonly agreed upon. The seven distinct intelligences discussed in Howard Gardner’s theory include “Spatial”, “Musical”, “Logical-Mathematical”, “Interpersonal”, “Intrapersonal”, “Bodily-Kinesthetic”, and “Linguistic” intelligences. Spatial intelligence relates to thinking in physical space and Musical intelligence correlates to rhythmic thought. The Bodily-Kinesthetic intelligence reflects hand on thought processes and requires bodily movement. The Interpersonal intelligence initiates learning through social interaction while Intrapersonal is the opposite and requires individual thought. Logical and linguistic intelligences are most commonly associated with learning for academic purposes. The logical intelligence requires mathematical thought processes to learn and the linguistic intelligence utilizes lectures and complex vocabulary to initiate thought (Gardner and Hatch, 1989, p.6). Since Gardner’s Theory was first introduced it has been widely hypothesized as to its implications on educational systems around the world. It is clear that Gardner’s theory; if properly implemented, could cause vast reforms to educational systems worldwide, increase student diversity, and provide educational opportunities to those who have previously not met traditional academic standards. It is commonly agreed upon that a majority of teaching methods in place today, exclude many of the multiple intelligences discussed in Gardner’s Theory, and focus on one or two of those intelligences. According to Gwendolyn Mettetal (1997, p115), “Verbal and logical-mathematical intelligences correspond closely to the traditional intelligences measured by intelligence tests”. This claim acts as evidence that intelligence is being measured with a focus on these two specific intelligences, as opposed to including the remaining five intelligences. Pokey Stanford’s analysis of the contrast between a multiple intelligence classroom, and a traditional classroom agrees with Mettetal’s statement on traditional intelligence measurements. Stanford states The teacher's role in a Multiple Intelligence classroom contrasts sharply with that of a teacher in a traditional classroom. In the traditional classroom, the teacher lectures while standing at the front of the classroom, writes on the board, questions students about the assigned readings or handouts, and waits as students finish written work. In comparison, in the Multiple Intelligence classroom, the teacher continually shifts method of presentation from linguistic to spatial to musical, and so on, often combining intelligences in creative ways (Stanford, 2003, p.81-82). Stanford’s analysis of a “Multiple Intelligence Classroom” requires continual transition between intelligences initiated by the teacher. In an experiment conducted at an elementary school in Farmington Indiana, students and teachers were assigned the task of initiating the use of multiple intelligences. An abrupt switch from a “traditional” class to class approach to a block learning period approach was the foundation for transforming into an institution that develops multiple intelligences (Mettetal, 1997). The block based system was designed to allow for interpersonal interaction between students during a “flow time” period. The period allowed for students to participate in multiple activities designed to develop all of the seven intelligences. This “flow time” also allowed for teachers to incorporate new teaching methods by consulting with other teachers. Although incorporating new teaching methods was welcomed it was not required of the teachers (Mettetal, 1997). To test the efficiency of the new educational system students, faculty and parents were interviewed and surveyed. Overall the participants had positive opinions of the new educational system; however, there were some concerns regarding the non-traditional approach (Mettetal 1997). In cases similar to the aforementioned situation, Multiple Intelligence Theory is already being implemented; however, some feel that the theory is still widely misunderstood. In a study conducted by Professor Ernie Barrington of the University of Auckland in New Zealand, fellow lecturers attended a workshop focused on the Theory of Multiple Intelligences and were later asked to complete a brief questionnaire (Barrington, 2004). A total of four workshops were held in which forty nine lecturers from Hong Kong or New Zealand attended. After each workshop each lecturer was mailed a questionnaire containing five questions in which twenty five lecturers completed (Barrington, 2004). The questionnaire contained these five questions: How much did you know about Multiple Intelligence Theory?” “Do you think MI is relevant to tertiary teaching? Do you think the quality of learning would improve if MI Theory were employed? Did you make any changes to your teaching as a result of the workshop? Do you have any other comments to make (Barrington, 2004, p.429) The results of Barrington’s questionnaire proved that a majority of lecturers felt the Multiple Intelligence Theory had a place in higher education; however, only three of the twenty five felt they knew a lot about the theory previous to the workshop. After more than three decades since Howard Gardner’s Theory of Multiple Intelligences was first publicized in Gardner’s book Frames of Mind: the theory of multiple intelligences (1983), Gardner’s theory still has not been fully implemented into current educational institutions. The experiment that will be discussed in this paper is designed to test for traces of Gardner’s Theory in a current college level English classroom based on the expectations of seven students. The experiment is designed to test the expectations of seven students on an assignment given the impression that the assignment is to be graded by the Professor. The hypothesis for this experiment is that the majority of the participants will take a Linguistic approach to completing the assignment based on societal norms created in an academic setting. Methodology For this experiments seven students in a 200 level English course at John Jay College of Criminal Justice were asked to complete a task. The seven participants were separated into three groups with two proctors overseeing each group. Proctors were fellow classmates with the participants. The task the participants were assigned was to describe their experiences in the concurrent English class. The proctors were assigned to give examples of possible methods to complete the task; however, proctors also stated that the participants would be graded, and the participants should complete the assignment using a method, they felt would satisfy the professor. The participants were told that the assignment was to be graded to increase the potential that they would take it seriously, and to ensure a more accurate reflection of their expectations. Possible method examples given by the proctors included, but are not limited to, working together, working individually, drawing a picture, writing a song or poem, and acting. Examples were given to encourage the use of a non-traditional method. Proctors were not given a script or a unanimous question; therefore, participants in each group had different experiences. No participant information such as age and gender was recorded. The method in which the participants completed the task would reflect their expectations of how a traditional English course was taught. The method in which they completed the task would also reflect which intelligence the participant most commonly identifies with when asked to complete an academic assignment. Results For this experiment, recorded data consisted of behavior characteristics and the individual’s method of completing the task. Group one consisted of three participants in which two of the three participants wrote summaries of their experiences while one participant drew a picture. Participants one and two who wrote summaries reflected a linguistic approach to academics; however, participant one made his or her summary rhyme. This technique correlates with the Musical intelligence as well. Participant three, who chose to draw a picture; this demonstrates both the Visual-Spatial intelligence, and the Bodily-Kinesthetic intelligence. However, the participant also included written dialogue, and written dialogue reflects the Linguistic intelligence. Group two consisted of two participants; both of which wrote summaries that clearly reflect a linguistic intelligence. However, the participants consulted with one another immediately after the task was assigned. The fact that the participants consulted one another prior to beginning the task demonstrates the Interpersonal Intelligence. The interaction between the two participants caused them to agree to draw a picture. Their decision to draw a picture reflects both the Visual-Spatial intelligence and the Bodily-Kinesthetic intelligence. Although the participants agreed to draw a picture eventually they both chose to write a summary. These process, in which the participants reflected on a previous decision, and chose to pursue a different method demonstrates a high level of self-reflection; therefore, demonstrating the Intrapersonal Intelligence. Upon completion of the task, the proctors spoke with participants and asked why the chose to write a summary. The participants stated that they felt a written summary was the most common method. This statement reflects the use of the Logical-Mathematical Intelligence because selecting the most common method required logical thought. Group three consisted of two participants: participant’s six and seven. Participant seven chose to write a summary while participant six chose to write a poem. As previously mentioned a written summary clearly reflects a linguistic approach. A poem demonstrates both the Linguistic and Musical Intelligences. Similar to group two, the participants in group three chose to consult with one another upon being presented with the task. Like the participants in group two, the participants in group three agreed to draw a picture; in fact, both participants began to draw their individual pictures before deciding to write a poem and a summary respectively (visible in the addendum). Unlike group two, the participants in group three chose to remain in contact, and consulted with each other throughout the entire process. The consistent communication between participants six and seven clearly reflects the interpersonal intelligence. When asked why they chose not to draw a picture both participants agreed that it was too difficult. Discussion The results for this experiment, in relation to the hypothesis, are inconclusive. Although a majority of the participants utilized the Linguistic Intelligence to complete task, it was not the sole intelligence that they associated with. Although the results of this experiment did not prove the original hypothesis, this experiment has acted as further research for Howard Gardner’s Theory of Multiple Intelligences by providing proof of Multiple Intelligences in certain students. Understanding the academic expectations of students, in regards to the Theory of Multiple Intelligences, is important in creating a more efficient and effective educational system. If it is proven that students commonly associate traditional academics with a particular intelligence, the educational system may fail to develop the remaining intelligences. If this is the scenario then a majority of students will be unable to reach their full potential. Further research in this field could result in a more diverse academic environment, in turn, potentially creating opportunities for those who previously did not meet traditional academic standards. Although this experiment did not suffice as means to test this hypothesis, similar or a revised version of this experiment could. The lack of a universal question could have potentially harmed this experiment. Proctors were unable to present the same information to all of the participants, in turn, creating a different experience for each group. Presenting a universal question to all groups in further research would help prevent tainted results. For this experiment, participant characteristics such as age, sex and gender were not recorded. For future experiments recording such data could be beneficial by potentially noticing further correlations between participants. Including participant’s academic characteristics such as, grade point average, class standing and academic interest could be beneficial as well. These characteristics could demonstrate the participant’s level of involvement in academics. Removing peers as proctors and replacing them with older, more experienced proctors could have been a benefit to the experiment as well. Since the proctors and participants are members of similar social groups it is unsure the level of seriousness both parties presented. Introducing proctors from separate social groups could potentially alter the way participants react. References Barrington, E. (2004). Teaching to student diversity in higher education: how Multiple Intelligence Theory can help. Teaching In Higher Education, 9(4), 421-434. Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of mind: the theory of multiple intelligences. New York, NY: Basic Books. Gardner, H., & Hatch, T. (1989). Multiple Intelligences Go to School: Educational Implications of the Theory of Multiple Intelligences. Educational Researcher, Vol. 18, No. 8 (Nov., 1989), pp. 4-10 Mettetal, G. (1997). Attitudes towards a multiple intelligence curriculum. Journal Of Educational Research, 91(2), 115. Stanford, P. (2003). Multiple Intelligence for Every Classroom. Intervention In School & Clinic, 39(2), 80-85. Addendum Attached as a PDF