Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 Mediated Humanitarian Knowledge; Audiences’ reactions and moral actions PUBLIC KNOWLEDGE, REACTIONS AND MORAL ACTIONS IN RESPONSE TO HUMANITARIAN ISSUES Summary Findings of Focus Groups Conducted in the UK in 2011 – Interim Report 2 Bruna Seu, Dept. of Psychosocial Studies, BIRKBECK, University of London, UK. Page 1 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 Acknowledgements This report presents findings from the three-year research project entitled: Mediated Humanitarian Knowledge; Audiences’ Responses and Moral Actions (‘KARMA’), launched in 2010 by Dr Bruna Seu and colleagues, Dr Shani Orgad and Professor Stan Cohen (LSE). This project was kindly funded by the Leverhulme Trust: reference grant number F/07 112/Y. We would like to express our deepest gratitude to all the non-profit agencies and participants who took part in this study, without whose generous cooperation and time the research would have been impossible. We are also very grateful to the researchers – Dr. Frances Flanagan, Dr. Mastoureh Fathi, Dr. Rachel Cohen, Dr. Rodolfo Leyva - who have assisted with data collection and project management. . More information can be found on the project website: http://www.bbk.ac.uk/psychosocial/our-research/research-projects/mediated-humanitarianknowledge This report may be cited as: Seu, I.B. (2014) Public knowledge, reactions and moral actions in response to humanitarian issues. Contact details: Bruna Seu, Dept. of Psychosocial Studies, BIRKBECK, University of London, 30 Russell Square, London, WC1B 5DT. Email: b.seu@bbk.ac.uk Copyright © KARMA Project: Mediated Humanitarian Knowledge; Audiences’ Responses and Moral Actions Disclaimer The views discussed in this report do not necessarily represent the views of the author, but are the expression, as objective as possible, of the public’s opinions and observations communicated during the focus group discussions. This notwithstanding, any analysis involves a certain amount of interpretation and particularly with qualitative data, is never totally objective. While every effort has been made by the author to ensure that the contents of this report are factually correct, neither the Leverhulme Trust nor the author accept responsibility for the accuracy or completeness of the contents of this Interim Report, and shall not be liable for any loss or damage that may be caused directly or indirectly through the use of, or reliance on, the contents of this report. Page 2 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 Table of Contents Summary Statement-Introduction: Executive Summary: Key Messages: Main Findings: 1. Knowledge: 1.1. Moral Principles 1.2. Why Help? 1.2.1. Other Focused Motives 1.2.2. Hierarchies of Care 1.3. Socio-Cultural Context 2. Reactions: 2.1. Emotional Reactions 2.1.1. Other-Concerned 2.1.2. Agency-Centred Reactions 2.1.3. Self-Centred Reactions 2.2. Cognitive Reactions: The Problem of Suffering and its Solutions 2.2.1. The Nature of the Problem 2.2.2. Natural Vs. Man-Made Humanitarian Issues 2.2.3. The Causes of the Problem 2.2.4. The Nature of the Solution 3. Actions: 3.1. Monetary Donations 3.1.2 Child Sponsorship 3.1.3 Direct Debits 3.2. Other Forms of Monetary Donations 3.2.1. Material Contributions 3.2.2. Time 3.2.3. Fundraising Events and Signing Petitions 3.3. Blocks to Acting 3.3.1. Moral boundaries – Whom We Should Help 3.3.2. Why We Shouldn’t Help Distant Sufferers 3.3. Structural Reasons 4 5 5 5 7 7 9 10 10 11 13 13 13 13 14 14 15 16 17 18 20 21 21 22 23 24 24 25 25 25 28 31 Appendix A: KARMA’s Research Aims, Methodology and Ethics 32 Appendix B: Table of Participant Demographics 36 Appendix C: Interview Schedule for Focus Group Participants 43 Page 3 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 Summary Statement-Introduction Building on the findings presented in the first report (see Appendix A for the KARMA study’s full scope of research questions and project information), this second report specifically addresses the components of the KARMA study concerned with exploring: The moral responses and reactions evoked in audiences by humanitarian appeals, and how these correspond to their ideological, emotional and biographical underpinnings. (Please note that audience’s biographical underpinnings will be more fully examined in Report 3) How the responses evoked by humanitarian appeals relate to audiences’ routine thinking, emotions and actions that constitute their ‘everyday morality. In what follows, the key messages and findings related to the above will be summarised and then unpacked in more detail and empirically substantiated with excerpts from the focus group interviews with participants. Correspondingly, while most of the themes identified in this report were prompted by the researchers’ questions (see Appendix C for the semistructured interview schedule) others emerged spontaneously during group discussions. However, rather than a truth finding operation, this report presents an attempt to map out the landscape of meanings and understandings that members of the UK public employ to make sense of, empathise with and respond to humanitarian issues. To this end, this report is therefore parsimoniously partitioned according to the following three key components identified in the research questions: 1. Knowledge: This section explores participants’ knowledge and criteria for when, why and whom to help in times of need. 2. Reactions: This section explores participants’ emotional and cognitive reactions to NGO campaigns and broader humanitarian causes. 3. Actions: This section explores participants’ most and least preferred types of helping and chartable practices, and their underlying rationales. Page 4 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 Executive Summary Key Messages: The study provided clear evidence of marked and widespread public fatigue in response to humanitarian communications. At the same time, participants to the study responded sympathetically and empathetically to distant sufferers. This suggests that the fatigue might be primarily due to a crisis in the relationship between the public and NGOs rather than between the public and distant sufferers. Despite a widespread emotional responsiveness to distant suffering, several blocks to action were identified, thus suggesting that mobilising public empathy is a necessary but not sufficient condition for action. The 3 most important blocks to action were: a) Participants’ perceived duties and obligations towards distant sufferers, including moral principles prioritising blood ties or local communities b) Distrust in NGOs, their use of resources and effectiveness of their interventions in the light of public socio-cultural understandings of humanitarian issues and their causes. c) Participants’ lived experience and their varying capacities for managing distressing knowledge1 The study shows that although physical and social distance can be an obstacle to public capacity to fully relate to distant sufferers, it is the ‘human’ and emotional distance from them that the public resists. Monetary transactions are perceived to increase the human distance between the public and the sufferers. Main Findings: The KARMA research found that the British public is overall generous, actively engaged in caring for others in their community, and sympathetic to the plight of distant sufferers. However, blocks that prevent this capacity for care from turning to action to alleviate distant suffering were also identified. 1 All participants reported having strong emotional reactions to information regarding humanitarian and development issues and overwhelmingly responded with empathy. Overall people accepted and expected to feel saddened and shocked by the knowledge of distant suffering, but some found the communications excessively traumatic and counterproductive. However, as well as expressing concern for the suffering Other, the overwhelming majority of participants voiced strong negative emotions towards NGOs for their perceived manipulative intentions. This third factor will be discussed in Report 3 Page 5 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 The vast majority of participants to the study had been involved in or had contributed to humanitarian or charitable causes at some point in their lives. However, the study shows that the public perceive and respond markedly differently to humanitarian emergencies caused by natural disasters and man-made humanitarian crises. Similar differences were also identified in public attitudes and responses to discrete emergencies and on-going development problems. Although most participants have donated to NGOs to help distant sufferers, the study shows an increasing cynicism towards and distrust of NGOs and their actions towards both sufferers and the British public. These manifested through a strong resistance towards a long term commitment and support of particular NGOs and a strong dislike of direct debits, in favour of one-off donations or, when possible, direct action that would bypass NGOs. Page 6 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 SECTION 1: KNOWLEDGE This section looks at public’s knowledge, understood in this context as the information, beliefs and attitudes informing and contextualising their responses (both reactions and actions) to humanitarian information, as well as their criteria for when, how and whom to help. 1.1. Moral principles: Pure altruism, that is helping simply to make someone else feel better expecting nothing in return, was an important and frequently mentioned principle underpinning people’s helping behaviour. Some considered helping others an intrinsic and innate part of being human. Most believed that the British people are very generous, more so than other countries: Doug: […] I just think that probably we are just a caring nation. The vast majority of participants highly valued being part of a community involved in a ‘give and take’. For these participants the desire to help was based on gratitude and reciprocal support in the community, with some participants applying this criterion beyond their immediate community In terms of abstract moral principles guiding their helping others, participants rarely differentiated between helping a distant sufferer and somebody nearby or emotionally close to them. Throughout, participants expressed a wish for an experience of helping involving embodied, face to face contact or based on shared humanity. However, in practice, this lack of differentiation did not necessarily translate into a Universalist application of such principles. Although the data unquestionably demonstrated the integration of the normative to help others into participants’ individual and national identity, participants’ willingness and experienced duty to help others stopped at a variety of points of distance between the helping self and the needy other. There was a shared belief across all the group that helping is intrinsically and implicitly good. Conversely, selfishness and not helping when one can, was considered negative and frowned upon by society and some considered helping others an intrinsic and innate part of being human. Some of the principles for helping had a broad ‘everyday’ quality and multipurpose application (e.g.: ‘every little helps’ ) while other principles were based on participants’ lived experience, particularly gratitude for help received (e.g. children or relatives’ treatment in hospital). Overall, in these cases, the giving back pertained to local institutions and community and gratitude was expressed through reciprocal help and support in the community. Page 7 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 Wanting to give something back often extended beyond gratitude for specific help received and stemmed instead from a more general appreciation of having been fortunate in life and having been cared for by society. For others, the wish to ‘give something back’, seemed to stem from an appreciation of the disparity in global life conditions and an expectation to somehow redress the disparity. Adrian: It’s like when it’s in the news and very much at the forefront of everyone’s minds, it’s almost expected of you to do something about it, us as, you know, prosperous, healthy fortunate people living in the world. It would be impertinent of us not to do anything about it. However, it was felt by some that this sense of care for others was not equally applied to the British themselves. This was particularly felt because of recession and the current economic crisis Cathy No, no, yes, we are the British and I think on a lot of things we get left behind in this... you know, the way the country is at the moment the British come last in a lot of things. Participants also made use of absolute notions of moral obligations when explaining why they helped, also in the case of distant suffering. Jerry: The rich should help out the poor Latifa: Yes, I was going to say, you feel like it's the right thing to do, don't you? Among the broader motivations for altruistic practices, the following were mentioned most often: Christian caring, social responsibility, conscience and empathy Imogen: I don't know whether it’s duty, though… You make your choices in life, but it’s not a… it may be a question of duty for some people, but I don't feel it’s duty. It’s my choice.[…] That’s not the way I think (that we are all children of God) […] I just feel I’ve got a social responsibility. I don't feel duty. […] It comes from a sense of the world around you and empathy. Bonnie: And it’s a question of conscience, you know, if you don't make that little effort to give where you're inspired to give, then it’s your conscience. You know you're not doing right. However, in practice, the lack of differentiation between who deserves help and who doesn’t did not necessarily translate into a universal application of such principles. Although the data unquestionably demonstrated the integration of the normative to help others into participants’ individual and national identity, participants’ willingness and experienced duty to help others stopped at a variety of points of distance between the helping self and the needy other. Page 8 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 1.2. Why Help? Conversely, in other cases several participants’ altruistic practices were motivated by more self-centred concerns, i.e., by what the helper gets from helping. When focusing on themselves, the two most frequently given reasons for helping were notions of karmic retribution and that helping makes people feel good. For example: Fawzia: Yeah, I think that's my good deed for the day, something good will happen to me. Meg: The reason why I give, it makes me feel good about myself. Sometimes karmic ideas of ‘what goes around comes around’ were mixed with principles of reciprocity. Alistair: So, if someone is suffering today, maybe they will be there for me one day if I am suffering or my family member is suffering, because times change. Andrew: Yes, I agree. It’s karma, isn't it? Participants also commented that they liked helping, that being able to help others gave them a sense of achievement, and some explained that helping made them feel more hopeful. Some participants also expressed ‘cheap participation’ motives, whereby giving enabled participants to temporarily silence humanitarian agencies and disconnect from humanitarian issues with a clear conscience. Isla: Yes, that's in your sitting room and you can just turn the TV off and block it out completely. You're almost saying Q< I can give £20 but just don't bother me with anything you know? That's how I feel that it is. 1.2.1. Other Focused Motives: When focusing on the sufferer, many participants helped because they vicariously identified with the sufferer and recognised the shared vulnerability. Others because they felt guilty. Polly: “Then my son has an eating disorder so now I’m also involved with an eating disorder charity and I'm on a local voluntary group for eating disorders. And I also have involvement with other charities but with other things that have, you know, touched me, have affected me over the years. Mental health issues in particular, because I seem to have, with my son’s eating disorder, I drifted onto the mental health side of it, so I now do unpaid work. I’m vicechairman of the local Mental Health Forum and I sit on the local health boards, Planning & Implementation Board, and, you know, lots of different things like that”. Page 9 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 Marianne: Yes. There has been certain occasions when things have happened on the television that I’ve given to it, you know, I’ve phoned in and pledged and all that kind of thing. And part of that is that there’s a part of me that thinks that if I was involved in a disaster, I’d like to think that somebody would help me.[…] I’d like to think that somebody was there for me. 1.2.2. Hierarchies of Care: Participants made use of a range of principles to prioritise those who should be helped first or at all. These everyday principles ranged from a prioritisation of blood ties and parochialism, pragmatic criteria and reciprocity to hierarchies of deservingness. Correspondingly, although there seemed to be considerable agreement on the general moral principles discussed above, there was no uniformity in how they were applied. Across the groups, participants discussed how they prioritised giving help, with the beneficiary positioned at different degrees of closeness/distance from the self. The following hierarchy of care emerged, in order of increasing distance of the beneficiary from the self. 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. ‘Me and mine’ – ‘the closest to you’ One’s own children and immediate family Extended family Local community Regional community (respectively for those coming from England, Scotland and Wales) Taxpayers living in Britain Britain – ‘my people’ ‘Whoever is wronged inside or outside the UK’ ‘The world is my family’, ‘I’m a citizen of the world’ How participants positioned themselves on this spectrum was uneven, with the overall majority positioning themselves at levels 1-4 of prioritisation. 1.3. Socio-Cultural Context: Additionally to moral principles and hierarchies of care, participants mentioned several sociohistorical factors when discussing their understanding and attitudes towards helping, both locally and to distant sufferers. Many participants made reference to feelings of distrust or to a climate of distrust. Many spoke of distrusting the British government and the Media and the way news are manipulated for political purposes. Page 10 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 Harold : […] And it’s a bit like immigration, where people say the country’s overrun with immigrants, and it isn’t really; it’s just that, you know, the vested interests like to make you feel that they are. Alistair: Yes, media and charities, and also politics as well I think. They are all connected in one way. I think politicians sometimes don’t want the media to show certain things because it may cost the government a lot of money themselves to get involved, because when there is a disaster in the world, charities do get involved, governments do get involved, everyone gets involved, the United Nations, everyone. So, I think it will cost more to help them, in a way, and that’s why no one wants to hear about it. Some spoke of the impact of a climate of distrust on the community and how it affects people’s capacity to give and receive help. Keanu: You could trust everybody in them days. […] You could trust your neighbours. You could trust the… you know, you could trust people, but nowadays you can't trust people now. If you went… If I went to a woman my age, really, and asked her, help her carry her shopping bag, she’d tell me to bugger off. Yet years ago, if you had seen an old lady and you was in your 20s, and you see a woman struggling with her shopping bag, you'd say, I'll give you a hand up with that bag, and she’d say, oh, yes. Thank you very much. But if you went to say that now, Tesco, she thinks you're going to steal it. Others advocated distrust, particularly when giving to needy people overseas. Earl : My brother and his wife donated for years and years and years for a family in Africa, and just think that they just scammed them forever; they never saw anything. Every time... they went back every five years and every time they went back they’d not done anything with it. Corruption was also a recurrent theme mentioned by people, particularly when discussing overseas aid Ken: Yes. So we know how terrible it is for these people. But I think you've got to get to the heart of the trouble. So I've got a friend who does charity work. She's been fundraising last week, and she does every year. It all goes to the plight of Africa where they're building a school. The point is, she stands out in this rain, selling the bows and the everything else, yet you’ve got the people above them, who are stashing away millions and millions of pounds, and not helping the people. And yet we're expected to try and help them. So you’ve got to get to their governments... Interviewer: Is that what you mean by people above? The government? Ken: Yes. Government. I'm talking about Mugabe, and people like him. Some people believed in universal traits that make people behave unethically Hank: Everyone’s out to make a buck, that’s why they sell arms to the poor countries. So long as they’ve got the money to buy it, they’ll sell it. Whether it’s ethical or they need it is another point. It’s… you know, if I was selling it, and I didn’t sell it, this guy’d sell it, next to me, if he Page 11 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 could make money out of it. And I think that that’s the problem, it’s… it’s like that all over the world, with everything. Bruna So how… you mean we’re selfish, is that what you mean? Hank Well, yes. Hank Everyone’s out to make a buck. Equally frequent was the mention of recession and its effects on people’s willingness to give. Neville: You’ve got... in our age now that the recession’s kicked in and there’s not a lot of money for anyone anyway sooner or later the English pound is going to go to nothing. We’re going to have to go to the Euro potentially. I mean we’ve got to start thinking about ourselves and how we’re going to make money just like they did back in the days of the Wall Street crash and all those things. Many also expressed cynicism in relation to humanitarian aid and the government’s dealings. Hamish: I mean, our government’s just offered X million pounds to Pakistan, towards their education and their children. Yet, we’re short in schools, hospital; teachers are getting made redundant… It’s all political; the only reason he’s given that money is because of the War on Terror, and we need to keep Pakistan on board, a nuclear-armed country. Similar cynicism and suspicion were expressed towards humanitarian agencies Penny: I think I always have been cynical about it, about foreign aid, […] I do, and I think having had experience of charities, I know how much these people are paid to sit in these offices and to supposedly run these charities, these campaigns that, you know, take these terrible pictures and depict these awful things, you know, which I’m not saying they haven't happened. Obviously, these are facts, you know, these tragedies have happened, these people are living like this, but it’s that whole – I resent being manipulated, I suppose. Page 12 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 SECTION 2: REACTIONS This section explores participants; emotional and cognitive reactions to NGO campaigns and broader humanitarian causes. 2.1. Emotional Reactions: All participants reported having strong emotional reactions to information regarding humanitarian and development issues. Overall people accepted and expected to feel saddened and shocked by the information, but some found the communications excessively traumatic and counterproductive. Many of these emotions expressed concern for the sufferer, but also for the impact of the information on themselves. Most participants expressed strong negative emotions towards NGOs for their perceived manipulative intentions. 2.1.1 Other-Centred Reactions: The two most frequently mentioned emotional powerlessness/hopelessness and empathy/sympathy. reactions were Powerlessness was expressed both in terms of humanitarian problems not being amenable to a solution, and participants’ sense that their contribution would not make a difference. Bruna: Do you think that as individuals we can make any difference at all? Keith: I don’t think so. It's them themselves that have to make the difference, not us. It doesn’t matter what we say, or how much money we send them or whatever it is. Candice: It [my first reaction] was that I felt sad and I felt hopeless actually. So it made me feel that it’s absolutely hopeless, what can I do? While powerlessness stopped people from contributing to humanitarian and development causes, empathic and sympathetic responses made people inclined to help. Gail: I think it's sympathy at the time it happens and that's what makes you give. Participants also experienced sadness and guilt. Gail: It's in our face, we live, eat and breathe it for days and days and days when it happens and it might be a guilt thing, I don't know, but we all tend to give to people like that. 2.1.2. Agency-Centred Emotional Reactions: Often the empathic response was accompanied by negative emotional responses in relation to the perceived manipulative intentions of the agencies. Page 13 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 Hamish: Well, I don’t think it’s right, because they’re pressurising you. It’s like a guilt trip with you. Send you pictures of, like, young kids and all; you think, no, that’s… Keanu: [the communication] made for you to pity and put your hand in your pocket and give more than what you can afford. […] UM: I feel… that's what it is, isn't it? It is a form of manipulation. UM: Manipulation. Keith: Blackmail more like. 2.1.3. Self-Centred Reactions: Participants also expressed cynicism, mistrust and resentment towards agencies for being made to read distressing material. The overall quality of this type of reaction was that the emotions evoked by the communication were unbearable Alan: I can’t stand it, it hurts me so much I just flick away, it makes me feel sick in a way Keanu: They show you all the acts and Children in Need, and then they put, next thing you see is children, babies, black babies in arms, with their parents, covered in flies, please give to these, you know. My wife looks at that, and she turns it off. Because not that she…she won't do anything... it upsets her. She won't watch it because she gets… she emotionally gets upset. Carmen: I mean I can’t watch the adverts. I mean when you do... like Comic Relief last week when they were doing the... I had to turn away because I can’t bear it. 2.2. Cognitive Reactions: The Problem Of Suffering And Its Solution: The most widespread belief in regards to humanitarian and development issues was that ‘nothing ever changes’ and that humanitarian problems are intractable, chronic and stuck. Most participants differentiated between and responded differently to humanitarian issues resulting from natural causes and those which they perceived to be man-made. A similar polarised differentiation was also used to explain different responses to humanitarian emergencies and development issues. The differentiation between long-term versus emergency humanitarian crises were mentioned across all focus groups. Whilst for natural emergencies most participants expressed a sense of responsibility and willingness to help, with man-made and on-going humanitarian problems, participants’ responses were less linear and more ambivalent. The majority of participants believed that that man-made problems were endemic to the country in need, particularly in the case of Page 14 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 African countries, that monetary or other kinds of interventions are futile and ill advised, and that the British public and government are not responsible or equipped to intervene. 2.2.1. The Nature of the Problem: The most widespread belief and sentiment expressed in regards to humanitarian issues was that ‘nothing ever changes’. Thus the sense of fatigue in relation to humanitarian communication expressed by the vast majority of participants was exasperated by a belief that humanitarian problems are chronic and stuck. Africa was mentioned by many as the symbol of what is quintessentially intractable in humanitarian issues. The use of Africa as shorthand captures these widespread perceptions of development and humanitarian problems, without the need for further explanation Milly: “ I think we’ve got used.. you know.. the Africa thing. The standard reference was to child starvation in Africa, offered by many participants in most groups as the proof that nothing ever changes. The mention of Live Aid and Band Aid was also a recurrent way of showing both how starvation in Africa is a chronic problem and the futility of monetary donations. Hugo: they’ve been starving in Africa – I’m not being funny about it – since I were a kid. And, like, we’ve had Live Aid, Band Aid, whatever it is, but they’re still starving in Africa. Added to the chronic poverty many participants viewed humanitarian issues as caused by chronic corruption. Imogen: I don't think it’s going to make any difference. Well, you know, I think everybody’s skirting around the issue which is, you know, chronic poverty and chronic corruption in a lot of countries. Especially the corruption. Additionally, as discussed in Report 1, many believed that high administration costs meant that “only a little money gets through”. Others believed that humanitarian problems are the legacy of colonialism. Kaleb: Well, I think the only comment I made, I mean, we used to have this vast empire, as England, and I think we're guiltily responsible for lots of things that went on then. 2.2.2. Natural vs. Man-Made Humanitarian Issues: Most participants differentiated between and responded differently to humanitarian issues resulting from natural causes and those which they perceived to be man-made. Differentiations were made on the basis of several factors. Page 15 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 One was the issue of responsibility and blame. Milly: I think, when I saw the Pakistan and all that, the people sitting for days on end on top of roofs and everything, that hit me more. I didn’t feel like that was a corrupt government that had left those people and they weren’t helping solve, you know. Another frequently mentioned factor was the immediate and visible impact of help and the effectiveness of monetary aid in case of natural disasters compared to man- made humanitarian issues Iris: Well, a lot of it is man-made. It’s not... I mean, if it’s a natural disaster like a flood or an earthquake, I mean we can pour money in and rebuild a town or supply food, but if it’s a war... I mean, you just don’t know where the money’s going or what the result will be. Ida: Or if the money gets through or the aid gets through. A similar polarised differentiation was also used to explain different responses to humanitarian emergencies and development issues. Overall, the former seemed to be received with relatively less resistance by the public. In emergencies, the usefulness of monetary donations is evident so is the clarity of what is needed and the visibility of what can be achieved through aid. There is also a sense of a discrete emergency with an end to the need. In contrast, in the case of poverty, the problem is perceived to be ongoing and not being ameliorated by monetary aid. These differences were mentioned across all focus groups. Milly: I think, natural disasters and on-going poverty are two different, completely different things. […] the Pakistan, you know, the people on the houses and everything waiting to be rescued. You know, that’s immediate and needs money straightaway, you know, China and everything. But you know this starvation thing? It’s gone on and on and it doesn’t get any better and no matter how much money you chuck at it, it doesn’t seem to get any better, does it? It goes on and on and we don’t seem to get any conclusion from that. It doesn’t get any better. Bridget: I think that if you give to something that is a sudden emergency and you suddenly hear that it is, you know, obviously getting much better, then you do feel it has been helping in some sort of way. But the things that are long-term, it’s just, you know, you do get this feeling of helplessness, sadly. But, oh, you see it over and over again and you think it’s just awful but... 2.2.3. The Causes of the Problem: In contrast to the overall empathic and proactive responses across focus groups to humanitarian emergencies due to natural causes, participants were divided over their understanding and responses to man-made humanitarian issues. The vast majority of Page 16 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 participants believed that the causes of the problem were endemic to particular countries, with Africa being the quintessential example of this Hugo: They’re corrupt, aren’t they? They send as much money as you want to some of those African places, they’re never… they’re not going to be… they’ll still be like this in 20 years’ time, no matter how many people give two bobs. Some believed that the problem originated from the sufferers themselves. Keanu: They still keep having babies, covered in flies. They can’t feed them. It’s awful. […] They haven’t got enough food in the country to feed them, so why keep having them? Because the’re all dying. It’s like breeding rabbits and they die. Some of these beliefs were moderated by alternative and more sympathetic views. For example Oscar, who believed that African governments are corrupted, also added: Oscar: I’d just like to say one thing about Africa, that they cannot grow food because the weather, it’s so arid there that, so you can say they can help themselves but if they can’t grow the food to eat, apart from irrigation and things like that which, what are they supposed to do? Whilst for natural emergencies most participants expressed a sense of responsibility and willingness to help, with man-made, on-going humanitarian problems, participants’ responses were less linear and more ambivalent. There was expressed reluctance to getting involved both in terms of individual responses and state interventions. Ogden: So none of these would appeal to me because a lot of it is caused by politicians, you know, and governments and somebody’s caused a problem or it’s, you know, due to civil war or whatever. Let them fight it out themselves. You know, why do we need to get involved and send money there? It’s only going to keep things going. A very small number of participants believed that the problems ‘here’ and the problems ‘out there’ are inextricably linked and that the West is directly implicated in various ways. Quaide: The issues that occur, whether they’re to do with conflict, or to do with famine, have an effect on us either immediately or at some point in the future, whether it’s through people being able to buy things that we make, or it’s going to be to do with people who are fleeing persecution, or economic migrants. So the idea that they’re separate, I find worrying, and I think the world’s a lot smaller than that. The view was also expressed that “the British Empire has got a lot to answer for”. Harold: I think it’s all… I mean, it’s all tied up in the political, ex-colonial situation, though, isn’t it, in the sense that a lot of these places, whether it be in Africa, the Indian sub-continent, it’s all… seems to be like a legacy of colonialism in the sense that they’ve kind of relied on the mother country being there for them as a backup in a lot of sense. But not only that, you’ve Page 17 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 got the arms trade whereby Britain and all the superpowers, they’ve never had any qualms about selling these places arms. Others also highlighted the complexity of the situation and while agreeing on the role of corruption in keeping people in poverty, some made reference to “exploitation by multinational companies while others believed that “the causes of poverty are almost always political. Even though the participants who believed the West was implicated in the causes of poverty and lack of development in third world countries were highly vocal, these were only a handful of people across the focus groups. 2.2.4. The Nature of the Solution: The majority of participants believed that poverty, starvation and on-going development problems had no solution and were impossible to solve. In this context the majority of participants thought that monetary donation was not a good enough or even effective solution. Only a few disagreed with this view and even those who judged important to continue financial aid to suffering countries, believed that this was simply because of lack of alternatives. As an alternative to the view that humanitarian problems such as poverty and starvation would never change, the view was offered that change results from a natural process, internal to the country. Genevieve: I think it is a natural process, really, it's up to some degree, yeah. I think it's… I don't know, sometimes the harder you push and the less you get there, you know, I suppose it's… I don't know. I think it's just a natural thing. Somebody somewhere will suddenly go, ping, light bulb turns on. For people believing in a process of ‘natural’ progression, individuals and NGOs are not agents of change and countries will ‘get there’ eventually. In contrast to this view, a few others believed that change happens as result of political pressure. Yet, for only a few this translated into a belief that that petitions and pressure from citizens can make a difference. Quincy: It’s because we, sort of, all…we allow it to happen, because we don’t ask those questions. Like, we’re all sitting here saying, well, you know, what actually happens to that? But how many of us have actually bothered to ask? I know it sounds silly, but writing to your MPs and saying, actually, where does it go to; what is happening? Because, at the end of the day, we are…if we’re all…we’re saying if these charities came together, well, what if we all came together, you know? Page 18 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 For the vast majority of other participants believing in the importance of the role of politics in change, this translated into further self-disempowerment and justification for not intervening in humanitarian causes. Hugh: Because, I mean, I think we’re all mankind, we need to help everybody, but we as individuals don’t have the ability to help everybody. […] As bad as it gets, we don’t suffer an ounce to what people all over the world suffer in these countries, through no fault of their own. But I see what we can do. I don’t see that giving £10 a month is going to help them. It’s political and it needs to start from the top; these people need to be helped from the top, from their own government. Page 19 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 SECTION 3: ACTIONS This section explores participants’ most and least preferred types of helping and chartable practices, and their underlying rationales. 3.1. Monetary Donations: One-off donations to natural disasters, particularly through telethons, were the only form of monetary donations that participants responded to positively. Several factors contribute to make telethon the preferred form of monetary help: the urgency and visibility of the sufferer’s need, ease of response (both in terms of what is being asked of the donor and the ease of making that donation), visibility and effectiveness of impact of the donation, the ‘feeling good’ factor and the capacity to then ‘forget about it’. Andrew: Commitment, that’s exactly right, not investment2. But for a lot of people it’s a lot, I mean say there’s a massive flood, people need support and money and it’s a lot easier to say oh well, I'll ring this number and give now. That’s going back to what I said about Comic Relief. We have one night, I know people think it goes on for months around the year, but they have one night where they go give us your money now and throughout the night they’re saying we’ve got 25 million, 30 million. It’s a lot easier for people to go oh, it’s only a phone call, so they ring up and give their details, they pay and then they forget about it. I think a lot of people think like that and it’s a weight off their mind. They think oh, I should probably give, so then they do and then they think oh, that’s all right, they’ll be fine now. However, whilst the majority of participants were occasional givers (in particular telethons, but also to shakers and money boxes), only a small minority of participants extended their help on a regular basis beyond the borders of Britain. Their willingness to help stopped at different points of increasing distance of the beneficiary from the self. Overall, the vast majority of participants had been involved in or contributed to humanitarian or charitable causes at some point in their lives. However, they also expressed a strong dislike for monetary contributions and a marked preference for other types of contributions, particularly fundraising events, giving time and signing petitions. 2 In response to a previous comment criticising direct debits as a commitment rather than an investment Page 20 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 3.1.2 Child Sponsorship: In terms of long term giving, some of the characteristics that make telethons successful emergency appeals applied also to long term giving of child sponsorship. It provides proof of efficacy and gives feedback on what has been done with the help given. Additionally, it fosters a relationship with the beneficiary. Barbara: I mean, one of the ones that I think is the nicest, you know, Q< she can change your life forever, sponsoring a child and I know... I work at a market, and I do know a gentlemen who has actually sponsored a little boy in Sri Lanka and he goes once a year and he gets letters and he writes letters to this little boy and it’s... you know, lit up that man’s eyes, you know, because he’s never had children and he feels that he’s really helping someone. So, I mean, that’s really a hands-on, one to one thing, but that’s the only thing that really gives me hope out of this because nobody really cares about, you know, oh yes, that’s... oh that’s what happens when you start to actually, you know, [unclear] and I’m just turning the page to look at something else. I mean, it’s a sad reflection on what’s happening in life, but unfortunately a lot of these. However, there was disagreement of opinion on whether the donation went to one particular child or to the child’s community. Some thought it didn’t matter and, in fact, the latter was better for all concerned. Others interpreted this as further proof of manipulation and untrustworthiness on the part of the agency. Penny But I think they've got a pile of photos of children looking like that and you sponsor it and they’ll send you the... and they’ll just take it off the top of their... do you know what I mean? And I’m not saying that then the money doesn’t necessarily go to children, but does it go to that child? When they say you sponsor Mary who lives in Kenya? Are you though? Do you know what I mean, are you actually making a difference to that one child’s life or you making a tiny difference to a whole village? I’m not saying it’s bad but it’s that whole manipulation that they're making you feel that you're doing that for that one child, where actually I don't think, being a cynical old cow, that they are, you know. These factors make this ‘emergency’ model of communication of and response to humanitarian crises the quintessential and most successful prototype of ‘sufferer-agencypublic’ dynamic. In terms of long term giving, some of the characteristics that make telethons successful emergency appeals applied also to long term giving of child sponsorship. It provides proof of efficacy and gives feedback on what has been done with the help given. Additionally, it fosters a relationship with the beneficiary. Barbara: I mean, one of the ones that I think is the nicest, you know, Q< she can change your life forever>Q, sponsoring a child and I know... I work at a market, and I do know a gentlemen who has actually sponsored a little boy in Sri Lanka and he goes once a year and Page 21 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 he gets letters and he writes letters to this little boy and it’s... you know, lit up that man’s eyes, you know, because he’s never had children and he feels that he’s really helping someone. So, I mean, that’s really a hands-on, one to one thing, but that’s the only thing that really gives me hope out of this because nobody really cares about, you know, oh yes, that’s... oh that’s what happens when you start to actually, you know, [unclear] and I’m just turning the page to look at something else. I mean, it’s a sad reflection on what’s happening in life, but unfortunately a lot of these .. However, there was disagreement of opinion on whether the donation went to one particular child or to the child’s community. Some thought it didn’t matter and, in fact, the latter was better for all concerned. Others interpreted this as further proof of manipulation and untrustworthiness on the part of the agency. Penny: But I think they've got a pile of photos of children looking like that and you sponsor it and they’ll send you the... and they’ll just take it off the top of their... do you know what I mean? And I’m not saying that then the money doesn’t necessarily go to children, but does it go to that child? When they say you sponsor Mary who lives in Kenya? Are you though? Do you know what I mean, are you actually making a difference to that one child’s life or you making a tiny difference to a whole village? I’m not saying it’s bad but it’s that whole manipulation that they're making you feel that you're doing that for that one child, where actually I don't think, being a cynical old cow, that they are, you know. 3.1.3 Direct Debits: Direct debits were the most disliked and passionately resisted form of donations, even by people who had been consistently and actively involved in humanitarian actions such as fundraising. Primarily this was because participants felt unable to make the required commitment, either because of changes in their life circumstances, the impact of recession, or uncertainties about the future. This was particularly felt by pensioners, most of who experienced a direct debit as ‘one more bill to pay’. Meg: What puts you off is the direct debits. That’s the only thing that puts you off if you just cannot do commitment. Some participants were put off from signing up to a direct debit because they were afraid of fraud or didn’t trust NGOs. Quimby: And I’m also wary about revealing my financial details. I guess things are checked, like, but, I mean, a few months off I closed my bank account down, one of these things, because again I just don’t look 100% to banks for security, you know, in sending information to third parties. Page 22 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 Correspondingly, others didn’t give to overseas suffering because believed that a large percentage of their donation would not reach the beneficiaries but would be used by NGOs or administration costs or wasted. Hank: The thing that’s always put me off is, like, say, you donate £10 for a family in India, just, for example, running water. I would gladly give £10 if I knew it would get to them and it would help them, but you think, well, probably £8 of that’ll be administration, backhanders, government… 3.2. Other Forms of Monetary Donations: Participants unanimously disliked and resented phone calls from sales representatives working for NGOs asking existing donors to increase their direct debits. Participants also commented on their ambivalent relationship with shakers. Some felt it was a convenient and low cost way of helping to put their pennies in collection boxes, just because they were in shops. Others felt that “if somebody’s good enough to stand out there in hail, rain and snow, rattling the box in front of you, I'll gladly put my hand in my pocket, put some money it. And I do it” while others resented the face to face pressure and refused to give to a “random shaker”. The feeling and dislike for being pressurised was particularly strong in connection to cold callers and chuggers that made participants feel ‘badgered’. 3.2.1. Material Contributions: A large proportion of participants thought that material contributions, in particular food, toiletries, tents, clothing, were better alternatives to money. This was because, differently from money, the donated objects are more likely to reach people in need, they won’t end up in the wrong hands, can’t be turned into weapons, or spent in administrative costs. Otto: Like the malaria vaccine that they’re sending now to Africa, there’s millions of them sent from this country and they reckon it’s only about 2% of that’s going to get to the people because, as you said, they’re going to, there are people there taking it and selling it to other people, so it could be on the black-market. So you know, I think it’s the issue of, as Oliver said, if it’s material, if it’s shoes and socks, something like that, it’s difficult but when it’s [..], they’re all in the pot for it, aren’t they? You know, that’s sad. Renee: I just always remember the kind of thing at Live Aid in ‘85, the whole kind of like con that came out of that, because they hid so many… I don’t know you remember this, but millions upon millions was raised, and Bob Geldof gave it over to the prime minister of this country who thought it was a present and spent it on cars. Uh huh. It was a huge controversy. Page 23 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 The government thought it was a gift. We have no idea how much actually got to the people that were actually needing it, because the government themselves weren’t doing anything about it. You’re just thinking, well… Raina: They were stupid to give them the money. Renee: I know, and there’s so much like that. You know what there is, you know, if we’re actually making up little boxes and it’s got clothes and it’s got toys, and it’s got sanitary and blah blah blah, you just feel as if, well, they can’t turn that into weapons, you know. […] Rebecca: And if you paid the ₤5 a month, that would never be getting spent on... Renee: Administration fees alone... 3.2.2. Time: Giving time was also perceived to be a preferable alternative to giving money, both when related to local helping and distant sufferers. This was partly because the giver can see the concrete effects of their efforts. Penny: When the Haiti thing happened, I was absolutely horrified and I really struggled to watch it on television. And the thing that upset me most was that weeks after it first happened, after these awful pictures were on the news every day, all day, this government kept saying that USA, they kept saying they were going to send aid out and they just didn’t. And they kept saying, oh, yeah, we’re going to, we’re going to, we’re going to do all this and the other and then meanwhile they're spending billions daily on arms, warfare, wars, you know. It’s crazy. But the thing it made me want to do, well, what I did do when I saw that is that I signed up with the Red Cross to actually become a volunteer to go to disaster zones. 3.2.3. Fundraising Events and Signing Petitions: Many participants had given to, participated in or organised fundraising events. Several factors contributed to making fundraising attractive: it enables people to raise more than they could afford to donate individually, it is relatively easy to organise, involves the community, and people have a good time participating. Meg: And since then we’ve been involved, I’ve been involved when the tsunami took place and I’m quite good at cooking so I cooked takeaway food. I raised £2,000. […] I could not give £2,000 but I was willing to put the hard work in, raised the money and provided the food and cooked the food to triple or quadruple the money; buy a pound of potatoes, cook them up and then, you know, 50p you spent on potatoes and you probably got £10 for them or £20. Sort of making samosas and curries and rice and stuff like that. Then, it’s like somebody else said, then another disaster happened, the Pakistan disaster and then people come to me and saying, are you going to cook again, are you going to do this? So you feel like, well, you go again because I’m Pakistani. And then it went on for six months. You do, you give everything what you’ve got, you know. I couldn’t give out money but, again, you have a bit of money and you can make more. And we did but you can’t keep doing it and doing it. We might have Page 24 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 raised £5,000 but that £5,000, if I did a direct debit of £2 a month, would probably still be ongoing for another five years or whatever. I mean, my maths isn’t very good. Signing petitions has the advantage of offering an easy form of participation, it provides feedback and proof of efficacy, and it makes people feel they are part of something. Rachel: I sign a lot of petitions online, which is something that’s really easy to do, once you’ve signed up. You know, you put your details in once, and it’s not like you have to send an email to somebody, or anything like this. And I find that really useful, because then…they then give you an update on whether whatever it was went through. So it could be, we’re sending this to Gordon Brown to do such and such. So you sign it, and then they’ll send you after two days, oh, look 50,000 people did it, and it’s gone through. Like, we’ve succeeded. So you kind of feel part of something, without having to give money away, or a lot of time, which I find really great, I love it. 3.3. Blocks to Acting: Despite their overall generosity and belief in the value of altruism and helping others in need, participants did not always help in cases of humanitarian need.. Several reasons were given for not helping, the vast majority of which referred to monetary donations. 3.3.1. Moral boundaries – Whom We Should Help: Although the overall majority of participants considered themselves to be highly generous and had helped relatives and members of their community, they were more discerning when asked to help distant sufferers. A small proportion of participants felt that the same principles should apply to near and distance suffering and that we have the resources to help both in our country and abroad. Bianca: they are all God’s children. Becky: Because I consider the world is my family. We are all one big family. Iris: I think we can help at home and abroad. […] I’m not saying we shouldn’t support, help the poor in this country, but I feel we’re rich enough to help both at home and abroad. The majority of participants believed that helping some sufferers should be prioritised over others and applied a variety of criteria to decide who should be helped, or not, and why. A large section of participants believed that charity begins at home and that we should prioritise blood ties Florence: I think if all these little children and all these countries, whenever one of them died, if that were me or brothers and sisters like there, what my view would be. Whereas if our Page 25 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 brothers and sisters were dropping dead of cancer or whatever, I'd feel I’d support them more because it would be my own, kind of thing. Everybody else around me, my life is affected by it. My own life isn't affected by one of them dying. It sounds a bit selfish but... Bruna: So that's where you draw the line, it’s things in your family. Florence: Yes, just me and mine, me and my own, kind of thing. Others applied pragmatic criteria when deciding whom to help Adrian: If we don’t look after ourselves, how can we look after other people? Nelson: Like there are all those little things you can do for yourself but you haven’t got time to so why are you going to go out of your way to do something for someone else? Reciprocity was considered very important by participants when deciding to help or not, and some applied this criterion beyond their immediate community Genevieve: We've all had it up to here. Bruna: So you feel… you don't feel it's our responsibility basically? Genevieve: No, honestly, not anymore. But I feel we jump into everyone's war, we jump into everyone's country to support them and look after them, and in the meantime…who supports us and who helps us? Do any of those countries come running over here to support to us? Deserving help was used as an important criterion to decide who to help and some groups of people were considered more deserving than others. There seemed to be agreement that children were intrinsically the most deserving of help, and some believed that British children were the most deserving of all. Genevieve: In Britain research is being curbed, hospitals are closing and personally would rather give to the children in this country that are dying, children with leukaemia, children at Great Ormond Street Hospital. To me, I would rather give my money to them. […] Yes, because unfortunately, the way the country is at the moment, the children in this country are suffering. A lot of children that are so very, very poorly, can't have life-saving operations. Maternity units are closing, premature babies are dying because there isn't and to me, I think, we must get our priorities right, and we live in this country. Others felt that beneficiaries should earn the help by working for it. Neville : If that’s what you’ve earned your money for, you’ve gone out and worked hard enough and that’s something that you’ve set your mind on, a goal, you’ve given yourself a goal, I want to get an iPod, it costs £200 I will work and save my money to get what I desire. If you can do that then you should do it happily. It’s not someone else’s choice to say, no, with that money that you’ve just raised you should go and give it to somebody else. At the end of the day why is that person telling you this, do you know what I mean? Page 26 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 Others believed that the beneficiaries deserving help where those able to work with the help given. Otto: But initially they [the Japanese] refused help whereas in Africa there’s been the bowl where you just can’t fill it. This is the sad fact of the reality I think. Africa has had billions of pounds and they’re still nowhere near, and yet in Japan, they’re going forward. They’re even building, you know, and you think... I don’t know. I don’t know the answer to be honest but, you know, what is it? There is something. People, a lot are willing to do that and others, as you said about India, no, we do it ourselves. Others preferred to help beneficiaries whose cultural behaviour they recognised and approved of. Raina: To me, it just came across as, oh, their culture, and everything about them, I just really found them to be… I don’t even think you would get that in this country, you know, the waiting in queues for water, helping people getting out, you know, yes, if anything happened, then you would think, yes, because you know your money’s going to help them. Only a small proportion of participants believed we should give to ‘Whoever needs it’, regardless of the nationality or other characteristics of those in need, causes of the need, etc. Bruna: You don’t feel that we should look after our own first, as it were? Meg: Oh, no, no. I think, where it’s needed. Basically, if a country needs water to toys in a hospital, I think the water comes first, you know, to having toys here. Some participants believed that only those who have paid taxes should receive help, others that we are already giving through taxation. Penny: And the other thing is that you're taxed. You're working hard, you're doing all these things and you're paying all these bills. You're paying tax on your earnings so that's going to the government, and supposedly that's meant to be divvied up to go to a bit of everything. So you are giving, aren't you? 3.3.2. Why We Shouldn’t Help Distant Sufferers: Participants provided a range or reasons to explain their unwillingness to help in humanitarian need. Most of these reasons, but not all, referred to monetary donations. One of the most frequent reasons for not giving was the participants’ perception that nothing ever changes. Keanu This has been going ever since I remember. It will go in the next generation and the next generation. There's no end to it. It's just something that's going to stop because Page 27 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 everybody’s giving money. It just will go on and on and on, constantly, generation after generation, because they keep multiplying. Others believed that we are not responsible for other countries, that we should solve our problems first because there aren’t enough resources to help others too. Betty: I don’t feel that I’m responsible for a child born in Africa, no. I have enough problems of my own just looking after my people. Omar: Well, the elderly, you know, you hear of care homes in trouble and so on; look after the health service and so on. You know, we’ve got our own problems; let’s worry about that. Africa will look after itself. The more money we pour in there, it’s not solving the problem. Let them look after themselves. Ogden: We can’t afford to give to everybody. Some participants believed that only those who have paid taxes should receive help, others that we are already giving through taxation. Penny: And the other thing is that you're taxed. You're working hard, you're doing all these things and you're paying all these bills. You're paying tax on your earnings so that's going to the government, and supposedly that's meant to be divvied up to go to a bit of everything. So you are giving, aren't you? Other participants didn’t give because of Government’s corruption in the suffering country. Betty: It’s not just that charity begins at home. It’s the fact that we give these people, these Governments, huge amounts of money to the detriment of ourselves... Belinda: Yes! Betty: And they squander it, they rape and pillage their own countries, they do not help their own people... Belinda: No, they don’t. Betty: Who the money is supposed to be going to. Belinda: It actually creates more poverty and makes them very rich too. Others didn’t give to overseas suffering because believed that a large percentage of their donation would not reach the beneficiaries but would be used by NGOs or administration costs or wasted. Hank: The thing that’s always put me off is, like, say, you donate £10 for a family in India, just, for example, running water. I would gladly give £10 if I knew it would get to them and it would help them, but you think, well, probably £8 of that’ll be administration, backhanders, government… Page 28 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 Betty: I’d like to know how much this cost to sponsor a child. Half the money that goes to sponsorship gets taken up with rubbish. You know, it doesn’t feed the people, it pays peoples’ wages, it pays for paper like this. You don’t need that to tell somebody there’s a poor man in Africa. We all know that. We get it on the news every single day of the week. This is absolute nonsense. Many participants commented on the impact of recession on their lives and how it discouraged them from giving to humanitarian causes Pearl: But then I, going back to the giving, you know, money every month, my point is I work damn hard and my partner, everything in this country has gone up horrifically, food, petrol, diesel, whatever, and I think, well, you know, obviously I’m a working class person and all that money that I'm making is for me and my family. So, you know, if the government can't do something so... Pattie: It’s a struggle for us when... Pearl: It is a struggle. Pattie: It’s a struggle at the moment. We don't have disposable income, you don't have... children say when are we going on holiday, mum?, and you think, oh, God, I don't know. Similarly, some participants believed that NGOs are targeting the wrong population and that the rich should give first. N: The trouble is, like, you see these people, like, all the time in the street come up to you with the same kinds of things and the thing that always seems to me is that it’s really bad and everything. It really is bad but you’re targeting the wrong people. Like, people in the street, just everyday people, that are just doing their salary like, I don’t know, £20,000 or £30,000 or something a year when, like, there are billionaires in the world with, like, offshore bank accounts and everything who probably could get rid of most of this just by putting a few like million or something. So to me it seems like, kind of, targeting the wrong people. Participants also were worried about making monetary donations because of the danger of fraud Bridget: I don't think there's anything that helps now; most of these forms that I’m just looking at are asking you for your credit card details. I think the old thing of where you can put a cheque in the post, you know, because you wanted to something, something hits you and you put a cheque in the post, okay, you know, that’s good. But all this, giving the credit card number again, and you think are they going to suddenly take another donation, because although it’s become very normal to give credit card numbers there’s nowhere... Some participants expressed difficulties in connecting with distant suffering. Hank: And it’s just… it’s close to your heart, whereas, like that guy said, you know, you sponsor somebody on the other side of the world, it’s not… there’s no connection there. It’s not being awful, but you tend to keep it more at home, you know. Page 29 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 Some participants reasoned that we don’t respond because we are desensitised. Alan: We see all this advertised on TV and in the paper and everything but we don’t see any changes. All they show us is suffering, suffering, suffering, suffering. So, we are kind of getting mean to that, we see these people suffering so we don’t really make an effort or take any notice of all these ads that are going around every day. We just think oh, okay, they are suffering, big deal, I'm suffering too. Others felt over-demanded and that there are too many humanitarian organisations competing with each other. Quinn: One thing that springs to mind, just when you look through all of that, seeing all these different organisations, all vying for your attention, […] if there’s so many different organisations saying they’re going to do this, in this area, you just think, who’s best to give my money to, then? Some participants felt they couldn’t engage with humanitarian issues because they are too upsetting Francine: It’s not like you don't want to know; it’s just you don't particularly want to read it. You know what it’s going to be... you know what you're going to be getting yourself in for reading it, so why put yourself through reading that. […] Like that one, this quote, it’s like this quote from the kids or there's the quotes from the women saying, so you know then it’s come from them themself. I don't want to read that. It’s just too much. Because you're like putting yourself in that position and you start to feel it yourself and it’s not fair to play with emotions like that and it’s not nice. You can't imagine what they've gone through and you don't want to imagine what they’ve gone through, so I won't read it. Only a very few participants stated that they didn’t help simply because they didn’t care. Joshua: No, I don’t even think twice about it. We’ve got our own problems, so I haven’t been working for a while. You know, that’s my concern. You know, to get myself sorted out, rather than, before looking anywhere else, and to be honest, I’d rather pay a thing for a dog thing or an animal than a human. Don’t ask me why I feel like that, because I don’t give a shit about humans really. Fawzia : I just don't really care about these people, that's just it. I think I just have to worry about... I want to say yes and then five minutes later I will completely have forgotten about it, so... Page 30 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 3.3. Structural Reasons: One of the most frequent reasons for not giving was the participants’ perception that nothing ever changes. Keanu: This has been going ever since I remember. It will go in the next generation and the next generation. There's no end to it. It's just something that's going to stop because everybody’s giving money. It just will go on and on and on, constantly, generation after generation, because they keep multiplying. Some participants believed that foreign aid damages the sufferer and that it is better not to interfere with the natural course of things Ogden: One of the dilemmas that again has been highlighted is that in some circumstances you can, I wouldn’t say do more damage than good, that’s probably not right, but you can certainly muddy the waters because you can give aid and these people become dependent on that aid and they’ll never help themselves. I mean, the classic case, again this is from the Lions so I’m afraid that will crop up, but there’s an international scheme, Water Aid, where provide water tablets, we provide pumps, we provide all sorts for people to have their own clear, clean water supply, which is great in theory. You stick that in, somewhere in Africa and of course the whole of the population gather around that one waterhole which means that, over time, all the wood around that waterhole is used up and it’s used up because they’re building fires, they’re building homes and whatever. And so you’ve upset the natural order of things because so many people, it would be no matter, whatever walking around the country, they suddenly gather at one spot and there’s no infrastructure for that. So there are these knock-on effects, albeit the initial thing is a good cause to do it. We are glad we have but it’s surprising how these things can have this domino effect. Other participants didn’t give because of Government’s corruption in the suffering country. Betty: It’s not just that charity begins at home. It’s the fact that we give these people, these Governments, huge amounts of money to the detriment of ourselves... Belinda: Yes! Betty: And they squander it, they rape and pillage their own countries, they do not help their own people... Belinda: No, they don’t. Betty: Who the money is supposed to be going to. Belinda: It actually creates more poverty and makes them very rich too. Others believed that monetary donations address the symptom not the cause Quincy: I mean, I think the argument is… the whole thing’s inefficient. If I should, anyway, if I should… just handing over the money. It’s not necessarily the answer. There’s all the sustainability, you know. You need to educate people, it’s not about sending this or sending Page 31 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 people out to other countries, but if there’s a specialism that’s required, you’d bring that across, you educate, you bring everything up to a level. Just, like, you don’t just provide water, you give them the wells and all the rest of it, and try to build it, but I don’t think that necessarily would…that’s not going to solve the problem. Some participants believed they were equally suffering and invoked stoicism. Flora: I just feel like everyone has their own worries, everyone has their own things going on at home, whether it’s been losing a family member due to cancer or what and I just think that they're bombarding you with their worries but you've got your own worries, do you know what I mean? Everyone’s got something traumatic that's going off in their life, whether it’s something big or something small, but you don't see me going out in the street because my mam is dying of cancer, bombarding everybody in the street to give to cancer. You just get on with it, don't you? You then feel strong about cancer so you'll give to cancer, but I just think they have their worries, they’ve got their worries, I’ve got my worries. I deal with it myself. You don't see me bringing out TV adverts, I’ve just lost my auntie to cancer, do you? Do you know what I mean? I just think everyone’s got their own worries, deal with it yourself. For the vast majority of those participants believing in the importance of the role of politics in change, this translated into further self-disempowerment and justification for not intervening in humanitarian cause: Hugh: Because, I mean, I think we’re all mankind, we need to help everybody, but we as individuals don’t have the ability to help everybody. […] As bad as it gets, we don’t suffer an ounce to what people all over the world suffer in these countries, through no fault of their own. But I see what we can do. I don’t see that giving £10 a month is going to help them. It’s political and it needs to start from the top; these people need to be helped from the top, from their own government. Page 32 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 Appendix A: KARMA’s Research Aims, Methodology and Ethics Background: The frequency and scale of humanitarian disasters is increasing, as reflected in their high visibility in the global media. Whilst still responding generously to humanitarian emergencies, the British public seems to be increasingly critical of and reluctant to commit to on-going support of humanitarian and international development agencies. In these highly challenging times for the humanitarian sector and humanitarianism more generally, it seems ever more urgent to understand how the public relates and responds to humanitarian crises and international development causes. The KARMA study sought to shed new light on the UK public’s understandings and reactions to humanitarian communications, including campaigns about international development issues and humanitarian appeals. To do so this study explored how people make sense of the images and narratives of distant suffering that agencies generate and how ideologies, emotions and biographical experiences shape those responses. This study also explored how agencies plan and think about their communications. The specific research questions addressed by this study are as follows: What are the public’s immediate responses to humanitarian messages and what do people do with their knowledge? What kinds of motivations and influences inform their actions? How do people justify and explain their responses? What biographical and emotional factors might facilitate or discourage moral action? How do the public's moral responses correspond (or not) with what humanitarian agencies hope for, and with their thinking about the communications they produce? Methodology: The data collection for this study was spread over the following three phases: A series of 18 demographically representative focus groups throughout the UK (total 162 people) Interviews with members of humanitarian agencies, which will allow for an investigation of the relationship between the production and reception of humanitarian messages A series of in-depth one-on-one interviews with 10 audience members. Page 33 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 Ethics: To ensure the protection of participants’ sensitive information and privacy, all of their names have been replaced with pseudonyms in all publications, and their information has been kept confidential and only accessible to members of the research project. Additionally, all participants were informed of the nature of the project and given the following information sheet and consent form. Information Sheet Small world: Attitudes towards and perceptions of what goes on around us in the world We would like you to take part in this research project. It is funded by the Leverhulme Trust and is carried out independently by the Psychosocial Studies Department, Birkbeck, in collaboration with the Media and Communications Department, LSE. You have been contacted at random by interviewers from The Field Department to help us with this project. Your participation is voluntary and much appreciated. We would like you to participate in this study as we believe that you can make an important contribution to the research. If you do not wish to participate you are free to drop out at any time. If you are happy to participate, please read this information sheet and sign the consent form. We will then ask you to participate in an informal focus group discussion with eight other people like yourself. The group discussion will be lead by Dr. Bruna Seu and/or Dr. Frances Flanagan from Birkbeck, University of London. At the discussion you will be given an information pack and you will be asked to describe and discuss your responses and your thoughts. The discussion should last approximately an hour and a half and refreshments will be served. The discussion will be audio and videotaped for internal use by the researchers only, and will not be seen or heard by anyone outside this study. After the focus group discussions are completed some people may be given the opportunity to take part in a followup discussion at a future date just on a one-to-one basis with Bruna where they have the chance to express their views in greater detail. We very much hope that people will want to take up this offer and have found in the past that it is something past participants have enjoyed doing very much. If you are requested to participate in this follow-up interview, you will be given a detailed explanation of the process at that stage, a separate consent form, as well as an additional incentive payment. What are the possible disadvantages and risk of taking part? Some of the material you will look at in the focus group may be disturbing, and may stir up emotions or thoughts that you find uncomfortable in the same way as some people find some newspaper or TV coverage uncomfortable. What are the possible benefits of taking part? People enjoy these discussions very much – it’s a place where many different viewpoints are exchanged and where there are no right and wrong answers. You will receive £35 as a token of our thanks for your participation once the discussion is finished. You may also find the opportunity to discuss and reflect on the issues interesting and informative, and by giving your time in this way you will have made a contribution to an important research field. What will be done with the research? We are hoping to write a book and a number of articles based on the research. Will my responses be kept confidential? Page 34 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 All the personal information you provide to us will be kept strictly confidential at all times, and only members of the research team will have access to that information. The responses you give to our questions will be anonymised. Identifiable responses will not be provided to any other third party. Information emanating from the study will only be made public in a completely unattributable format or at the aggregate level in order to ensure that no participant will be identified. All data collection, storage and processing will comply with the principles of the Data Protection Act 1998 and the EU Directive 95/46 on Data Protection. Who can I contact with any questions about the study? If you have any questions about the study or any information you wish to add after the focus group is complete, please contact Dr Frances Flanagan on 020 7631 6678 or f.flanagan@bbk.ac.uk or Dr Bruna Seu on 020 7631 6539 or b.seu@bbk.ac.uk . Dr Flanagan and Dr Seu’s mailing address is School of Psychosocial Studies, Birkbeck, University of London, Malet St, London WC1 E7HX. Thank you in advance for all your help. Page 35 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 Participant Consent Form I have read the participant information sheet for this research project and understand the following: 1. That I am free to withdraw at any time 2. That all information I provide will be dealt with in a confidential manner 3. I agree that the researcher may contact me after the focus group Signed………………………………………………………………………………….. Print name …………………………………………………………………………… Address………………………………………………………………………………… ……………………………………………………………………………………………… ……………………………………………………………………………………………… Email address ………………………………………………………………………. Telephone Number……………………………………………………………… Date…………………………………………………………………………………….. Page 36 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 Appendix B: Table of Participant Demographics Socioeconomic grouping Sex [M/F] Occupation Ethnicity Sexuality Marital Status and age of children (if any) Albert C1 Male 19 Administrator White British Straight Single Allen B Male 25 Financial Controller Bengali Straight Co-hab Andrew B Male 21 Quantity Surveyor White British Straight Single Alex C1 Male 18 Student White British Gay Single Aaron C1 Male 25 Corporate Travel Consultant White British Straight Single Adam C1 Male 23 Media Operations Black African/White Mixed race Straight Single Anthony C1 Male 20 Retail Supervisor Black Caribbean Straight Single Alistair B Male 25 Credit Manager Black African Straight Single Adrian C1 Male 18 Student White British Gay Single Bridget C2 Female 60 Avon Lady White British Straight Widow, empty nester Bonnie C2 Female 64 Complimentary Therapist Afro-Caribbean Straight Co-hab, Empty Nester Belinda C2 Female 60 Part-Time Yoga Instructor Black British Straight Separated, 2 kids ages 18 & 22 Becky E Female 65 Retired White British Straight Separated, empty nester Barbara D Female 56 Market Trader White British Straight Married, kid age 19 Betty C2 Female 56 Cook White British Straight Single Blanca C2 Female 56 Teaching Assistant White British Straight Married, 2 kids ages 18 & 19 Billie C2 Female 60 Music Teaching Assistant White British Straight Widow, empty nester Brenda E Female 65 Retired White British Straight Married, empty nester Candice C1 Female 65 Cleaner White British Straight Co-hab Caroline C1 Female 65 Retired Teacher White British Straight Divorced Cathy C2 Female 73 Retired Appliance Fitter White British Straight Widow Cordelia C2 Female 65 Retired House Cleaner White British Straight Married Carmen C1 Female 65 Retired Antiques Dealer White British Straight Married Pseudonym Age Page 37 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 Cynthia C2 Female 68 Retired NHS Maintenance Worker White British Straight Divorced, kid age 44 Chloe C1 Female 65 Book Keeper Jewish Straight Single Christina C! Female 70 Retired Book Keeper White British Straight Married Claire C1 Female 73 House Sitter White British Straight Divorced Dwight C2 Male 35 Bricklayer Irish White Straight Single Daniel C1 Male 37 Credit Admin White British Gay Single Darren C2 Male 41 Painter and Decorator White British Straight Single, 2 kids ages 2 & 13 Damien C1 Male 45 Sergeant Major Scottish White Gay Single Dennis C1 Male 37 Bar Manager White British Gay Single Desmond C1 Male 45 Artist White British Straight Divorced Dominic C2 Male 44 Printer White British Straight Co-hab Don C2 Male 39 Train Driver White British Gay Co-hab Doug C2 Male 37 Block Paver White British Straight Married Edward C1 Male 56 Civil Servant White Gay Married Edgar C1 Male 56 Manager White Gay Married Edmund C2 Male 56 HGV Black Straight Married, empty nester Ernie C2 Male 56 Builder White Straight Married, empty nester Eamon C2 Male 60 Retired Electrician White Straight Co-hab, empty nester Eddie C1 Male 64 Retired manager, White Straight Married, empty nester Edison C1 Male 63 Retired Company Director White Straight Married, empty nester Earl C1 Male 65 Retired Police Officer, White Straight Married, empty nester Florence C2 Female 20 Student Black Straight Single Francesca C2 Female 22 Housewife Black Straight Single, kid age 2 Francine D Female 20 Domestic Cleaner White Straight Couple Fawzia E Female 25 Housewife White Straight Felicity C2 Female 25 Housewife White Straight Single, kid age 5 Couple, 2 kids ages 9&3 Page 38 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 Couple Felicia D Female Fiona D Female 22 Counter Assistant White Straight Couple, 2 kids ages 3&1 Flora C2 Female 21 Sales White Straight Single Freda E Female 21 Housewife White Straight Single, 6 months Gabrielle B Female 65 Retired Accounts Secretary Jewish Straight Married, empty nester Gail B Female 65 Retired Box Office Cashier Straight Married, empty nester Gemma C1 Female 67 Retired Nurse White British Straight Married, empty nester Genevieve B Female 66 Retired Secretary Jewish Straight Married, empty nester Georgia C1 Female 70 Retired Deli Owner White British Straight Widowed, empty nester Gaynor C1 Female 71 Retired Senior Administrator White British Straight Widowed, empty nester Geraldine B Female 66 Retired Property manager Jewish Straight Divorced, empty nester Gita B Female 65 Housewife Jewish Straight Married, empty nester Gertrude C1 Female 68 Retired Retail Manager White British Straight Divorced, empty nester Harry D Male 50 Panel beater White British Straight Married, kid age 15 Hal E Male 46 Unemployed Printer White British Straight Married, kid age 18 Hamid E Male Married, 2 kids ages 9 & 10 Hamish D Male 25 Unemployed White White British Straight 47 Unemployed Builder White British Straight 46 Taxi driver Black Caribbean Straight Co-hab, 2 kids ages 4&6 Married, empty nester D Male 52 Wagon Loader Mixed Race Straight C2 Male 48 Painter/Decorator White British Straight Habib C2 Male 46 Publican White British Straight Married, empty nester Co-hab, 3 kids ages 15, 17 & 18 Halim C2 Male 47 Joiner (self-employed) White British Straight Married, 2 kids ages 4 & 20 Hanif C2 Male 47 Painter/Decorator White British Straight Married, 3 kids ages 15, 17 & 19 Ingrid AB Female 47 Care home manager White British Straight Co-hab, kid age 9 Iris AB Female 54 Accountant White British Straight Married, empty nester Ida C1 Female Straight Divorced, kid age 16 Ianthe C1 Female Straight Divorced, kid age 18 Hank Hans 56 50 Sales Administrator Administrator White British White British Page 39 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 Married, 2 kids ages 18 & 16 C1 Female 47 Estate Agent White British Straight AB Female 53 Telecommunications Manager White British Straight Isabella AB Female 52 Lawyer White British Straight Married, 2 kids ages 13 & 19 Divorced, 2 kids ages 8 & 14 Isla C1 Female 50 property manager White British Gay Single Julian C2 Male 35 Painter/Decorator White British Straight Co-hab, kid age 2 Jonathan E Male 29 Registered Disabled White British Straight Married, kid age 4 Jack C2 Male 35 Windscreen Fitter White British Straight Married, 3 kids ages 5, 7 & 16 Jeremy D Male 26 Sales Assistant Black Straight Single Joshua D Male 34 Steel Polisher White British Straight Married John C2 Male 33 Musician White British Straight Single Jerry D Male 28 Packer White British Straight Co-hab James E Male 35 Unemployed White British Straight Co-hab Jim D Male 28 Call Centre Assistant White British Straight Single Kevin D Male 70 Gardener White Straight Married, empty nester Kieran D Male 66 Retired Groundskeeper White Straight Married, empty nester Kai D Male 72 Retired Factory Worker White Straight Married, empty nester Kaleb D Male 66 Gardener White Straight Married, empty nester Kane C2 Male 65 Painter/Decorator White Straight Married, empty nester Keanu D Male 65 Retired Postman White Straight Married, empty nester Keith C2 Male 67 Retired Tool Setter White Straight Married, empty nester Ken C2 Male 69 Retired Toolmaker White Straight Married, empty nester Keon C2 Male 65 Retired HGV Driver Italian Straight Married, 3 kids ages 37, 15 & 13 Lucy B Female 35 Teacher White Gay Co-hab Layla C1 Female 26 Administrator Black British Straight Single Lane Lara B Female 31 Teacher White Straight Married, expecting first baby C1 Female 32 Data Analyst White Straight Married, expecting first baby Imogen Indigo Page 40 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 Larissa B Female 30 Office Manager White Straight Co-hab, 2 kids ages 3 & 9 months Latifa B Female 30 Accountant White Straight Co-hab Leanne C1 Female 33 Financial Investigator White Straight Married, kid age 2 Lia C1 Female 26 Student Mixed Race Caribbean Straight Co-hab, kid age 3 Melissa B Female 26 Accountant White Straight Married Mary C1 (C2) Female 47 School Support Worker English Straight Married, 2 kids ages 17 & 15 Marianne C2 Female 46 Housewife Pakistani Straight Married Michelle C1 Female 46 Social Worker English Straight Separated, 2 kids ages 17 & 14 Michaela C1 Female 50 Sales Assistant English Straight Divorced, kid age 10 Monica C2 Female 47 Merchandiser English Straight Married Milly C1 Female 55 Sales Assistant English Straight Married, kid age 28 Meg C2 Female 43 Hospital Technician English Straight Married Marcie C2 Female 49 Theatre Technician English Straight Married. 2 kids ages 24 & 26 Nathan C1 Male 19 Call Centre Assistant English Straight Single Nigel C2 Male 18 Student English Straight Single Nick C2 Male 23 Mechanic English Straight Single Neil C1 Male 22 Carer Special Needs Pakistani Straight Single Neville C1 Male 18 Window Cleaner English Straight Single Newman C1 Male 20 Student English Gay Single Nelson C2 Male 20 Security Guard English Straight Co-hab, kid age 10 months Nathaniel C1 Male 21 Student English Straight Single Nachman C1 Male Straight Co-hab, 2 kids ages 1&3 Oliver B1 Male 56-65 Lecturer Welsh Straight Married, kids (ages not specified) Oscar C1 Male 56-65 Retired Agriculture Officer Welsh Straight Married, kid age 16 Otto C1 Male 56-65 Retired Financial Advisor Welsh Straight Married kids (ages not specified) Oberon B Male 56-65 Retired Police Inspector Welsh Straight Married kids (ages not specified) 21 Manager English Page 41 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 Octovio B Male Straight Married Ogden B Welsh Straight Married, kids (ages not specified) Omar Hostel Manager Welsh Straight Married, kids (ages not specified) 56-65 Retired Agricultural Chemist Welsh Straight Married 56-65 Retired Banker Welsh Straight Married, kids (ages not specified) 45 Housekeeper Welsh Straight Single Female 42 Unemployed Welsh Straight Single, kid age 17 D Female 43 Kitchen Assistant Welsh Straight Married, 2 kids ages 7 & 9 Pearl D Female 43 Shop Assistant English Straight Single Pam C2 Female * Shop Manager Welsh Straight Married Pattie D Female 45 not working Welsh Straight Married, 3 kids ages 16, 15 & 13 Paula C2 Female 42 classroom assistant Welsh Straight Married, kid age 7 Quincy B Male 36 Account Manager White Gay Co-hab Quinn C1 Male 44 Fire-fighter Indian Straight Married, kid age 18 Qadair B Male 41 Property Agent White Straight Married, kid age 2 Quimby B Male 36 Teacher Indian Straight Engaged Quigley C1 Male 42 Prison Officer White Straight Married, 3 kids ages 8, 12 & 15 Quennel A Male 44 Operations Director White Straight Married, 2 kids ages 16 & 18 Quasim B Male 39 Chartered Surveyor White Straight Married, 3 kids ages 8, 11, & 13 Quade B Male 36 Architect White Straight Single Quimat Rachel B Male 36 Management Development White Straight Married, kid age 1 C1 Female 26 Freelance Illustrator White British Straight Single C2 Female 29 Barmaid White British Straight Co-hab, pregnant C2 Female 35 Housewife Indian Straight Married, kids (age not specified) C1 Female 26 Student White British Straight Single C1 Female 35 Horticultural Therapist White British Straight 56-65 Retired Maths Teacher Male 56-65 Retired Museum Administrator C1 Male 56-65 Orion B Male Orlando B Male Polly C2 Female Patricia D Penny Welsh Rebecca Regina Rita Raina Single, kid age 11 Page 42 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 Renee C2 Female 33 Part-time Jeweller White British Straight Married, 2 kids ages 4 & 6 C1 Female 33 Civil Servant White British Straight Married, kid age 3 C2 Female 29 Administrator White British Straight Co-hab Rana Reba Page 43 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 Appendix C: Semi-Structured Interview Schedule I would like to get a general idea of your response to what you have seen. Any kind of comment will be really helpful; it can relate to one particular appeal or campaign you have seen now, or to more than one, or to something else you have seen or heard even though it is not covered here. This part of the discussion will be free flowing and I will say little at this stage apart from asking for clarification. Feel free to respond to each other without coming back to me at this stage. What is your reaction (thoughts, feelings) in reading this information? a. What gut reaction did you have when you read this information? b. Could you give me two or three words that would summarise your main feelings after seeing the information? c. How do you feel about the victims? d. Are there any elements to the communication which arouse a particularly strong emotional reaction in you? e. Which of these feelings makes you want to do something?/ which instead makes you want to turn away? What kind of thoughts did you have when you saw the information in your pack Could you summarise your general attitude/the way you think about these issues? Often people comment that we should look after our own first – do you agree? What are your thoughts on this - do you think we are responsible for distant strangers? Do you think we, as individuals and communities, ought to help other people, like the ones described in the leaflets, who are so distant from us? In what circumstances do you think it is appropriate for people from other countries to help people suffering in faraway places? Some people say that money won’t help, but also that giving money is not the best way to help the humanitarian cause. What is your opinion on this? Do you have any thoughts on what else people could do to help? Do you feel you can help or make a difference in any way? What do you think would make you feel you can make a difference? What do you think are the factors the prevent people from doing more about these issues? Page 44 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 If you were in a humanitarian crisis or a victim of HR abuse, what do you think you would want happen? I imagine you have seen this kind of information before, right? Let’s start with few general questions: a. What is your ordinary response to information of this kind: (prompts: what are your thoughts and feelings when you get one of these?) Remember, there isn’t a right or wrong answer; please be as honest as you can. b. Does any of these appeals produce guilt in any of you? How do you react to guilt? Does it spur you into action? c. If not guilt, what do you feel would help you to contribute or/and act? d. What are your feelings when you see appeals of this kind on a train, in a newspaper or when it comes through your letterbox? Were the feelings you experienced today different to what you experience in those circumstances? e. Does it make a difference where you see them (e.g. do you have a different response if the appeal comes through your letterbox rather than seeing it on a train?) f. What do you ordinarily do when you get it? (Prompts: do you read it? Bin it? Put it away to read it later? Ignore it? Donate? ) I would like you to go back to your information pack and go through the information more carefully. Feel free to scribble on them if something in particular strikes you and/or highlight particular words. When there is text to read, I would like you to behave as you would ordinarily: read it to the end if that’s what you would normally do or make a note of where you stopped reading and, if you can, why you stopped at that point. a. Which, if any, of these leaflets would you pay attention to if you received them through your letterbox or saw them in the street? Which ones would you read? Why? b. Do the appeals speak to you? What do you understand their message(s) to be? c. Is the message in some of the appeals clearer than in others? You might want to give different answers for different appeals. d. I want to ask you now about the amount of information contained in these appeals. Do you feel it would make a difference to have more/less information? How does it? e. Does the individual story make a difference? In what way? f. Does it make a difference if there is a visual image? Does it matter if the image is realistic rather than staged? Page 45 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 g. Does it make a difference if the situation is an emergency rather than an ongoing situation? Before I have asked you to describe your reactions in general, now I would like you to be more specific. I am going to ask you about specific actions in relations to the information in your pack: a. Which appeals you would ignore or throw away – why? b. Which ones would make you donate (this include one-off donations) – why? c. Which ones would motivate you to find out more about the issue or the organization? d. Which ones would motivate you to do something else (volunteer, join an organization, read more about independently, write a letter or anything else) – what would that be? e. Which ones would stay with you; is likely to make you think about it tomorrow/ in a week’s time? f. Does it make a difference if the problem is a natural disaster rather than something caused by other human beings? g. Some people say that it helps them to respond if the appeal mentions a clear solution. Do you feel the same? How does it work for you? h. What do you think of the solutions that the leaflets recommend? i. Is there anything that you would like to see in appeals that would stimulate you into action? j. If you decide to act (by donating, volunteering, writing a letter etc) how does that action make you feel? (possible prompts: relieved, proud, excited, empowered, as though I have been true to my values/faith/obligations, connected to others, informed) k. Do you think these organizations have given a fair representation of the issues in these leaflets? l. Are any of the organizations who have produced these leaflets familiar to you? Do their values accord with your own? m. Are there any organizations here you do not trust? n. A lot of these leaflets mention money – how do you think the money will be spent? Does it matter to you to know how the money gets spent? o. Do you trust these organizations to spend the money they are given appropriately? Page 46 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 Some people say it’s a miracle that we care and help others in the first place. What do you think makes people get involved in these kind of issues? 1. Do you think of yourself as a moral person? Why? (remember, I am not assuming you are not; I’m fascinated by how people think about these things) 2. Which of these institutions, if any, do you see as part of your moral community: family, friends, church, the state, charities, workplace, the media? 3. I am sure you currently are or have in the past taken care of others in some way (e.g. friends, partners, elderly relatives, children, neighbours, pets etc.), but do you feel personally involved in any of the issues described by the leaflet? Do you feel the appeals speak to you personally? In short, do you feel this is your business? 4. I am sure you know or have known people in the past who have been in need of care of some kind (eg. friends, partners, elderly relatives, children, neighbours). What do you do to express your concern? Why? 5. Have you ever given time or money to a charitable organisation? Which one, why, do you still do? Why not? (possible prompts: to make a difference; because my values/religion oblige me to; to meet likeminded people; to be part of something bigger than myself; to make me feel less guilty; to develop as a person) 6. Do any of your friends or family give time or money to charities? Does that influence your decision to give/not give, and who to give to? 7. What, if anything, would make you give to a charitable organisation? 8. If you have children, would you like them to give to a charity (/when they are older)? Why/why not? 9. Do you think the way that Governments deal with these issues has an impact on the way you think about them? 10. What about the media? I would like you to get into pairs and ask each other the following questions. You have about 10 mins – at the end you will tell us what your partner has said. Please do take notes if it helps. 1. Tell us about a time when you first became aware of moral issues beyond your family (e.g. cruelty to animals, homelessness, world poverty etc.) Tell us what happened (e.g. how it happened, how you felt, what you did) 2. Do you remember the first time you responded to a humanitarian/charity appeal or any other request for help to others who were suffering? Can you tell us about it? Page 47 of 48 Focus group report 2: knowledge, reactions, moral actions. Easytoread v.1 3. Where or from whom did you learn to do this/ who inspired you/ set the example for you? (parents, teachers, friend, relatives, spiritual leader, historical figure, historical event?) 4. Have you always thought/felt/behaved the way you do now, or can you identify something that made you change in respect to these issues? Again in pairs: 5. Do you remember any time in your life when you helped a stranger in need? It could be when you were a child or at any time in your life. What happened? What made you do it? 6. Can you think of an instance when you have been helped by a stranger? 7. Can you think of a time in which you were vulnerable and required assistance in order to function independently? (eg. childhood, a time when you were sick or disabled, lacking in skills or education, poor or marginalised). What happened? Did anybody help? Page 48 of 48