He Describes the Forest, She Describes the Trees: The Effect of Gender on Communicative Abstraction Exploring gender differences in communication, we suggest that men and women vary in the extent to which they communicate abstractly and that this divergence has important downstream consequences. In particular, we argue that women speak more concretely than men in interpersonal contexts and find that this influences their success in entrepreneurial pitch competitions. Our findingsboth add to our understanding of basic differences in how men and women use language, and establish the organizational relevance of such differences. Gender and Abstraction Prior research suggests several key differences in how men and women use language (for a review, see Mulac, Bradac, & Gibbons, 2001). For example, while men tend to use language to convey information, women are increasingly likely to use language for social purposes(e.g., Brownlow, Rosamon, & Parker, 2003). Women and men also differ in particular word (Newman et al., 2008) and phrase (Lakoff, 1975) usages; for example, women are more likely to use hedges and ‘tag questions’ than are men (Lakoff, 1975; Mulac&Lundell, 1986). We draw on Construal Level Theory (CLT; Trope &Liberman, 2010) to argue that women’s experience of interpersonal closeness results in them adopting a more concrete communication style relative to men. CLTdistinguishes between abstract and concrete ways of representing and communicating information. Abstract messages are relatively decontextualized, focusing on a message’s gist. Concrete messages are contextualized, specific, and local, focusing on more incidental features. Prior findings suggest people use more abstract messages to communicate with a distant audience (Joshi & Wakslak, 2014; Joshi, Wakslak, Raj, & Trope, in press); this is because communicating with a distant audience requires an individual to be both relevant and relatable, making de-contextualized, abstract messages particularly effective. Historically, women’s social roles have largely placed them in small group contexts, leading them to develop a tendency to seek intimacy and social connection (Eagly, Wood, &Diekmann, 2000). Indeed, women continue to be more relational than men (Cross, Morris, & Gore, 2003), experiencing greater feelings of connection andcloseness with those with whom they interact. Given that closeness to an He Describes the Forest, She Describes the Trees: The Effect of Gender on Communicative Abstraction audience leads to concrete communication, this suggests that women will tend to adopt a more concrete communication style than men. Such differences may have important downstream consequences. Speakers who communicate concretely tend to be perceived as less powerful and having less status than those who communicate abstractly (Wakslak, Smith, & Han, 2014; Reyt, Wiesenfeld, & Trope, 2015). A tendency for women to speak concretely may therefore hinder their relative success in contexts that require people to portray themselves as confident and powerful, such as an entrepreneurial pitch competition. To summarize, women’s proclivity to feel close to others may cause women to speak more concretely than men, even in contexts such as entrepreneurial pitches that would benefit from speech abstraction. We examine this idea in the three studies described below. Study 1: We began by conducting a meta-analysis of 11 prior studies we have conducted on linguistic abstraction in communication (Joshi & Wakslak, 2014; Joshi et al, in press). These studies examined the effects of psychological distance on linguistic abstraction, finding that people communicate abstractly with distant others and concretely with near others. We expect women to communicate less abstractly than men in contexts where they feel closely connected to their audience. Because women experience greater interpersonal intimacy in relational settings, we expected women to speak more concretely than men in psychologically near communication contexts, but not necessarily in psychologically distant ones. Results revealed a main effect of gender on linguistic abstraction, with men speaking more abstractly than women, d=.19, 95% CI=[.05, .33]. The gender effect was strongwhen participants were communicating with a close other, d=.40; 95% CI=[.20,.59], but disappeared when the audience was psychologically distant, d=.03; 95% CI= [-.17,.21]. Results thus suggest a gender effect on communicative abstraction in contexts involving proximal communication. Study 2:Next, to support our relational argument, we wanted to show that men and women differ only in the extent to which they communicate abstractly, but not in the extent to which they mentally represent information abstractly more generally. One hundred participants completed two measures: in an initial prescreening session they completed a modified version of the Behavior Identification Form, a He Describes the Forest, She Describes the Trees: The Effect of Gender on Communicative Abstraction commonly used measure of abstract cognition. Then, in a study session several weeks later they completed a modified communication version of this same measure (i.e., instead of asking them to identify the presented behaviors, we asked them to describe the behaviors to another student (a psychologically close audience)). As expected, women (M=4.70, SD=1.59) and men (M=4.49, SD=1.75) did not differ in the extent to which they mentally represented information abstractly, as measured by their responses to the prescreen. However, womencommunicated the same behaviors more concretely (4.20, SD=1.81) than did men (M=4.86; SD= 1.51), F(1,99)=5.77, p=.02. Study 3:In our final study, we wanted to examine the implications of gender differences in communication on women’s likelihood of success in entrepreneurial pitch competitions. A wide gender gap exists in entrepreneurship, with men being more likely than women to engage in innovative entrepreneurial activity and find success in obtaining venture capital (Fairlie& Marion, 2012, Ahl, 2006). We suggest that because speech abstraction is associated with perceptions of power and status (Wakslak et al, 2014; Reyt et al., 2015), women may be penalized in interpersonal contexts that require them to present themselves as confident and powerful such as entrepreneurial pitch competitions. We obtained entrepreneurial pitches from a student pitch competition at a large business school in the United States and coded these speeches for abstraction using the linguistic categorization model (Semin& Fielder, 1991). We also obtained ratings given by venture capitalists in these competitions on three dimensions: ‘general assessment,’‘profitability,’ and ‘communication skills.’ As expected, men (M=2.96, SD=.27) presented their entrepreneurial ideas more abstractly than women (M=2.84, SD=.25), t=2.38, p=.02. Furthermore, speech abstraction predicted overall assessment of the entrepreneur, B=.73, SEB=.26, t=2.84, p=.005. Finally, we conducted mediation analyses to examine a potential indirect effect of gender on entrepreneur assessment through speech abstraction: results revealed this significant mediated pathway (bootstrapped 95% CI: LL=-.02, UL=-.23). These findings suggest a) that gender differences in communication persist even in high-stakes communication contexts and b) that these differences have important consequences for critical organizational outcomes. He Describes the Forest, She Describes the Trees: The Effect of Gender on Communicative Abstraction References Ahl, H. (2006). Why research on women entrepreneurs needs new directions. Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice, 30(5), 595-621. Brownlow, S., Rosamon, J. A., & Parker, J. A. (2003).Gender-linked linguistic behavior in television interviews.Sex Roles, 49, 121–132. Brush, C. G., De Bruin, A., & Welter, F. (2009).A gender-aware framework for women's entrepreneurship. International Journal of Gender and Entrepreneurship, 1(1), 8-24. Cross, S. E., Gore, J. S., & Morris, M. L. (2003). The relational-interdependent self-construal, selfconcept consistency, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(5), 933. Eagly, A. H., Wood, W., &Diekman, A. B. (2000). Social role theory of sex differences and similarities: A current appraisal. The Developmental Social Psychology of Gender, 123-174. Fairlie, R., & Marion, J. (2012). Affirmative action programs and business ownership among minorities and women. Small Business Economics, 39(2), 319-339. Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (1996). The Ambivalent Sexism Inventory: Differentiating hostile and benevolent sexism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(3), 491. Joshi, P.& Wakslak, C. J. (2014). Communicating with the crowd: Speakers use abstract messages when addressing larger audiences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 351-362. Joshi, P., Wakslak, C.J., Raj, M., & Trope, Y. (in press). Communicating with distant others: The functional use of abstraction. Social Psychological and Personality Science. Lakoff, R. (1975). Language and woman’s place.New York: Harper Colophon Books. Mulac, A., Bradac, J. J., & Gibbons, P. (2001). Empirical support for the gender-as-culture hypothesis: An intercultural analysis of male/female language differences.Human Communication Research, 27, 121–152. Mulac, A., &Lundell, T. L. (1986). Linguistic contributors to the gender-linked language effect.Journal of Language & Social Psychology, 5, 81–101. Reyt, J.N., Wiesenfeld, B., & Trope, Y. (2015) The social implications of construal level for advice taking and status conferral. Unpublished manuscript, McGill Univeristy. Semin, G. R., & Fiedler, K. (1991). The linguistic category model, its bases, applications and range. European Review of Social Psychology, 2(1), 1-30. Trope, Y., &Liberman, N. (2010).Construal-level theory of psychological distance. Psychological Review, 117(2), 440. Wakslak, C.J., Smith, P.K., & Han, A. (2014). Using abstract language signals power. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107, 41-55.