Summary The DNRF LANCHART Centre plans to continue both strands of studying patterns of language variation and change and evaluations of language variation. We will increase the collaboration between research groups to analyze the link between production and perception of variation. Studies will address basic concerns in sociolinguistic theory and methodology, and will broaden the scope of the work in the direction of frameworks in general linguistics, both with respect to functional and formal approaches. We will expand the corpus so that we can contrast spoken and written language directly. This expansion will also allow us to continue the tradition of training new researchers. New students will be trained in all steps of the process from generating an idea to collecting the data, analyzing the patterns and writing up the results. Our new empirical project, the Bornholm study, draws on a range of the methods of collecting data on speech production and language attitudes developed by the center. Research plan 2013-2015 1. Introduction We build on two observations. Firstly, we have diagnosed a demand for theoretical innovations and an even larger need for theoretical integration within the vast area of spoken-language research. Disciplines have multiplied at surprising rates while the basic theoretical premises stand in need of restructuring, or even complete rethinking. Furthermore, the integration of theoretical thinking and methodological practice needs a second look. Secondly, we have built a vast data base of spoken Danish which is at once structured and searchable. This fact alone is essential for the success of our research, but we have to expand the data base so that it will function on a par with data bases for written language. 2. Studies in the theory and methodology of sociolinguistics1 2.1 Languaging and super diversity; continuing the work of the Amager group Linguistics has been quick to adopt a national basis for the study of distinct languages. In the present day and age this is not a realistic assumption to build on, hence the alternative basic concept of languaging (Jørgensen 2010). With the Amager group J. Normann Jørgensen is working to provide a theoretical and empirical basis for the study of currently ongoing change in Danish languaging practices. One focus is on the poly-centered set of norms and behaviors which seem to characterize at least metropolitan adolescents' languaging, not only in Denmark, but also elsewhere in Europe. The project has followed a cohort of adolescents for three years and has now started following a cohort of school beginners in order to carry out research among the informants for ten years paralleling the earlier success of the Køge longitudinal project. This project will yield insight into changing diffusion patterns which could not have been imagined ten years ago, such as the spread of norms and practices through social internet media and computer games. The theoretical 1 The papers mentioned in italics throughout are the 10 papers delivered to the evaluation panel. They are listed below. challenges facing such a task are also being tackled, cf paper 5. The research in the period will involve new data collection as well as data analysis of trajectories of development for both the previous cohort and the cohort being followed from 2010 and onwards. Special attention will be given to integrating results from the same individual in different contexts (in and out of school, in and out of family) and different modes (speech and the various modes of writing) (Jørgensen, Madsen, Spindler Møller, Ag and Stæhr with Karrebæk and Wedel Schøning). 2.2 Revisiting central tenets of the Labovian paradigm The Labovian paradigm from the outset rested on a number of central interlocking tenets. Most of these have gone through various phases of revision as dissension has been voiced. We have ourselves contributed to these debates in papers 4 and 8. The consequences of leaving central tenets of the paradigm will be further explored 2013-15, integrating knowledge from other disciplines without losing sight of the specificity of language. The first step is to scrutinize the division of the linguistic sciences between (some version of) formalism and (some version of) functionalism in order to search deeper for the place of sociolinguistics. The next step is to build alternative theories based on the empirical findings (Gregersen, Karoli Christensen, Madsen and the team of researchers as such). 2.3 Variation as a theme in linguistics at large Variation stands out as a nodal point in the development of linguistic theory. Formalism, functionalism, and sociolinguistics have all grappled with the problem. In a paper tentatively called Three approaches to the ‘same’ linguistic problem (cf. the problem addressed in paper 7) we (Gregersen, Juel Jensen and Christensen) contrast our findings with conclusions reached on the basis of informants’ own judgments of grammaticality. The fact that variation abounds in language use poses theoretical problems for traditional conceptions of both grammar and the language system, mainly with regard to the status of linguistic categories. Corpus data very rarely support categorical descriptions of form-meaning pairs: most often, some percentage of the data go against introspectively based hypotheses. The research project on epistemicity and semantic variation (Christensen) seeks to answer precisely these questions. Expanding on this theme we plan to place the various kinds of linguistic behavior we have disclosed, in an integrated production-and-comprehension model that allows for core elements and outliers so that frequency of use will have theoretical consequences for categorization – here seen as a continuous and ongoing process (Christensen, Pharao, Gregersen, Juel Jensen). 2.4 Studies in sociolinguistic methodology Most studies of phonetic patterning use either auditory coding or acoustic measurements. In the LANCHART project we have used both. A comparative study has been prepared by Gert Foget Hansen and Nicolai Pharao and will be completed in 2014. A related theme is that of the possibly historical nature of auditory coding. We shall compare the codes from 1989 with the LANCHART codes. We may identify exactly which items were coded as belonging to which variant in a small part of the corpus. This makes it possible to contrast two kinds of coding done at two different points in time in order to find out if the coders’ own speech community is a factor (Pharao and Gregersen with Tyler Kendall, University of Oregon, and Charlotte Vaughn, Northwestern University). This study would amount to a real time trend study of sociolinguistic perception. Randi Skovbjerg Sørensen in her Ph D project focuses on the role of the interviewer in producing ‘good’ or ‘bad’ data for sociolinguistics. This study will be integrated with the findings of paper 8 to form a coherent model of where variation comes from in sociolinguistic data to supersede the Hymes SPEAKING model. Recent results from the LARM project suggest the prospects of performing a real time panel study of sociolinguistic perception. Jacob Thøgersen now has acoustic evidence suggesting that members of different generations would perceive the variants of (a) differently, and that listeners’ perceptions may change over time. We shall make the original coders recode both old recordings and the LANCHART re-recordings. This will reveal how perception may change in real time (Pharao, Thøgersen and Gregersen with Normann Jørgensen and Kjeld Kristensen as well as Kendall and Vaughn and Foget Hansen). 2.5 Semantic variation Enlarging the data base will make truly comparative work on lexical variation, constructional variation (Geeraerts), discourse variation, pragmatic variation, and interactional variation possible on a much grander scale. The basic issue is that statistical analyses of semantics demand huge corpora. Tanya Karoli Christensen has developed promising approaches to our set of data integrating the DCA in studies of the semantic field of epistemicity and this line of research will be expanded during the next three years (Christensen, Jensen, Gregersen). LANCHART has already contributed to the heated debates on the distribution of “main clause phenomena” (most prominently V2) and variation with respect to subclause features such as complementizers (cf. paper 7 and 319 and 320).We have advanced the hypothesis that V2 serves as a foregrounding device in spoken Danish. We will pursue this and other hypotheses concerning the distribution and semantics of word order further: 1) by extending the analyses to cover adverbial clauses and relative clauses, and 2) by seeking further support for the hypothesis of word order semantics in correlations of the distribution of V2 with the use of conjunctions, modal particles and interjections (Jensen and Christensen). The Dictionary of Spoken Danish (ODT) will be continued, and hopefully expanded through external grants, for the entire final period (Carsten Hansen, Martin H. Hansen). 2.6 Studies in experimental sociophonetics We have already established a strong platform in this expansive field (cf. e.g. paper 9). Further expansion will be guided by a focus on how social meanings are ascribed to variants (Kristiansen, Maegaard, Pharao, Spindler Møller, Juel Jensen). The picture seems to be different and more multiplex for older established variants, like the short (a) variants in Danish, than for newer variants, like e.g. the [s+], with sharply delimited but different social meanings. The linguistic context matters for how individual variants are perceived. We shall expand the study of (s) in order to explore how registers are formed. Adapting methods from experimental social psychology and including grammatical variables will not only shed light on the variables themselves, but will also provide us with a window to how variation at different linguistic levels go together in language users’ perceptions. In order to study how on-going sound change affects representation of speech in the mind, a collaboration has already started between Nicolai Pharao and Charlotte Vaughn at Northwestern University, USA, where the roles of social salience and homophony will be explored. Recent studies including Labov et al. 2011 give methods to study how the frequency of use observed at the group level affects the perception of individual speakers, who themselves differ in the frequency with which they use new variants. We will adapt them to confront one obvious ‘paradox’ of the discipline, which consists in attaching great importance to differences in frequency of occurrence without knowing whether these frequency limits actually correspond to any psychological reality (Kristiansen, Maegaard, Pharao, Spindler Møller, Juel Jensen). 2.7 The Gender in Danish and Dutch Youngsters project (GIDDY) Frans Gregersen and Leonie Cornips have collaborated on analyzing the acquisition and use of gender in Danish and Dutch by youngsters. This resulted in a joint paper for the 19th Sociolinguistics Symposium in Berlin 2012 focusing on what both authors call the ‘vulnerability of gender as a category’. During the collaboration Gregersen and Cornips have worked out an inventory of available data on Dutch focusing on gender. In a number of cases data for Danish is missing. This is especially the case for experimental data on the acquisition of gender by Danish mono- and bilingual children. In the period 2013-15 we shall remedy this lack, making a whole new series of comparisons possible (Gregersen, Cornips, Boeg Thomsen). 2.8 Studies in Language Attitudes, Ideologies and Patterns of Language Use The LANCHART study includes data on informants’ linguistic self-evaluation. Analyses of these self-evaluations, together with the informants’ comments about them in the interviews (coded for already, in the majority of interviews), will allow for further inquiries into the devious and delicate relationship between subconsciously offered attitudes, consciously offered attitudes and language production and comprehension (Kristiansen, Gregersen, Pharao, Scheuer). People’s opinions on varieties of Danish are important in their own right: They form part of the growing field of Folk linguistics (Preston). It is obvious that lay people have tremendous problems understanding us when we pronounce the death of Danish dialects. So what do lay people think of as dialects? What dialects are they able to distinguish with reasonable certainty and what dialect borders are most important to them? This will be addressed in a perceptual dialectology project (Monka, Scheuer, Kristiansen). The SLICE project will be integrated into the LANCHART centre work. Jacob Thøgersen studies the role of radio news in the Danish speech community. The corpus makes it possible for us to answer questions about the media’s possible lagging behind or running ahead of the development in the speech community at large (Thøgersen, Pharao, Kristiansen). Finally, we have a large amount of data which lend themselves to discourse analysis of language variation. Such discourse studies of prevalent ideas about variation may be a valuable supplement to the more quantitative approach described above (Monka, Scheuer, Kristiansen, Thøgersen). 3. Enlarging the data base of spoken language research 3.1 Transcribing old data, harvesting new We shall enlarge the data base by transcribing, according to the well tested practices we have developed, substantial parts of the residual of recordings (cf. paper 1). Transcribing strategically selected parts of the material along with some new recordings from Bornholm (cf. below) will help us reach an important limit set to work with written language data of around 15-20 million words (Gad, Gregersen). On the other hand, we need to include new types of data. The obvious place to look for them is the new social media. Andreas Stæhr has pointed the way to go. By making agreements with informants to harvest large amounts of their internet production we will be able to supplement our speech production data with written data from the new sphere of intimacy (Stæhr, Thøgersen). 3.2 The LANCHART Centre as a national Centre for knowledge about modern spoken Danish The LANCHART Centre has as its vision for the coming years to develop into a national centre of knowledge of spoken Danish. The vision has to be implemented in the context of services to the educations at the universities in Denmark enabling them to use the data base for all kinds of searches, all kinds of analyses, cf. on the web applications in sections 3.3.2 and 3.3.3 below. However, funding from the DNRF will not be used for this purpose (Gregersen, Barner-Rasmussen, Bøll, Solvang). 3.3 Preparing the LANCHART for embedding The DNRF has invested in the LANCHART project. Since humanistic studies are labor and wage intensive most of this investment has now been exchanged for research results. But the LANCHART is also a corpus and an infra-structure which has to form the basis for research into spoken Danish in all of the foreseeable future. The embedding agreement securing the support structure of the centre until at least 2025, is ideal for this purpose since it makes it possible to keep the centre abreast of developments outside the centre such as infra structures for the humanities in general. The ideal outcome, one that is clearly within view, is that the LANCHART corpus is presented as a series of personalized, secure, human operable interfaces for ‘simple’ searches as well as a number of documented software services enabling secure and personalized low level access to the LANCHART data architecture for more advanced needs. 3.3.1 New Data Layer: The data architecture that was originally established has not scaled well enough and response times for searches remain an issue. A new architecture more suited for fast text and text pattern matching searches is being planned. 3.3.2 New ‘Business’ and new Front-end layer: The current architecture does not readily enough lend itself to facilitating large scale access for external processes, e.g. High Performance Computercalculations over the entire annotated orthography. An important vision of the future is that the corpus hardware and software stack enable a much more explorative approach by the researchers. It should be easy to create new windows into the data, and new representations of the data for output in order to facilitate a Rapid Application Development-for-Research path. 3.3.3 New Security Layer and new External Access Layer (Internationalization): A method for allowing accredited researchers access to the corpus in its various representations and modalities must be established while honoring the terms of confidentiality. A significant part of this task consists in establishing a secure layer around the search engine with the ability to serve any number of sub-corpora to accredited users. 3.4 Research plan for digital humanities The technological aspect of the LANCHART project constitutes a small research effort in its own right, orthogonal, but deeply integral, to the linguistic areas of inquiry. Prospects seem particularly good within the Digital Humanities disciplines of architecting large scale humanistic data/computing infrastructures, database design and optimization for TextGrid style data. The need for innovations in this area is great, since neither standard full-text search platforms nor out-of-the-box data mining solutions are immediately adaptable to LANCHART’s needs due to the special time-line structure of linguistic annotations and to the fact that traditional linguist programming platforms like PERL do not scale to LANCHART corpus dimensions. With the scheduled products LANCHART will be ideally placed to be a major player in developing Danish humanities research infra-structures to function as integrated hubs in the general European web. As members of both CLARIN and LARM/Dariah we have a unique experience to build on (BarnerRasmussen). 4. New empirical project: Bornholm Bornholm is a small island in the Baltic Sea. There are several reasons why Bornholm is of great interest to sociolinguists. Firstly, Bornholm has remained uncharted territory for dialectologists and sociolinguists since the 70s when Baumann Larsen and Geist revealed a fundamental structure of Rønne (the main city) versus the rest. There are even metalinguistic terms for this: People in Rønne are claimed in general to speak ‘Rønnefint’ (‘Rønne posh’) or to speak ‘thinly’. Secondly, the orginal Bornholm dialect is conspicuously different from other Danish dialects (especially in terms of prosody). Thirdly, the linguistic awareness of Bornholm speakers is presumably higher than at any other LANCHART site studied. Yet very many Bornholmers tend to lose their prosodic characteristics when they leave the island. Malene Monka’s forthcoming dissertation on movers and stayers will be relevant for the understanding of this process. Fourthly, Bornholm is an extreme case of centre and periphery. In winter, it has its own weather forecast. In summer, for the very same reason, tourists vastly outnumber residents. Bornholm has 42.000 residents but 300.000 visitors in June July and August taken as a whole. Finally, Bornholm is the missing link in Danish dialect studies (the Copenhagen department studies the islands except Bornholm and the Jutland department studies Jutland). Pedersen 2009 has shown that there are many distinct characteristics in the original dialect, but they are all of them lost now – we think. The standardization on Bornholm exemplifies a language change which will be an ideal meeting point for dialectologists, syntacticians, and students of linguistic landscaping and discourse construction of local identities. 4.1 Data collection We shall develop a three tiered data collection technique as follows: (1) First, we collect subconsciously offered attitudes; this is dependent upon no prior knowledge of our working in the community. (2) Next we collect data on speech production. We shall conduct sociolinguistic interviews according to the previously followed LANCHART design for data collection i.e. with informants distributed across three generations, two sexes and two social classes. We will recruit local interviewers and interviewers among students who have grown up on Bornholm and contrast such interviews with insiders with interviews carried out by outsiders. With a smaller part of this group we shall further collect data from a limited number of different situations, some of them selfrecorded, some of them monitored by a field worker as unobtrusively as possible. (3) In addition, the smaller group of informants will give judgments in a specially designed questionnaire honing in on construction types we know will be frequent in their own production as well, e.g. the embedded clause problems evidenced in paper 7 and the coordination mismatches studied by Parrott (paper 10). The questionnaire will be partly based on reactions to spoken language (played by the field worker on site) and partly based on written stimuli in order to control for differences in normative reactions. Finally, these judgments will after an initial analysis be tested against a larger group of informants via the internet. It is to be hoped that such experiments will result in a sort of standard procedure for data collection in the future ensuring a new era of comparable data. As part of the N’CLAVE collaboration an initial field trip to Bornholm has been scheduled for November-December 2012 where we focus on the oldest dialect speakers, thus contributing to our knowledge of the present day status of this too little studied dialect. The study of the speech community will be supplemented by a study of linguistic landscaping and linguistic practices during the summer holidays when visitors will have to be catered for in spoken and written communication. Such multilingual practices are a necessity for survival for individuals as well as the community. In this sense we think of Bornholm as a Danish Martha’s Vineyard and it would be obvious to repeat some of Labov’s early approaches to variation inside the community and in communication with outsiders in this Danish context. The extensive tourism during the summer months also leads to a commodification of “Bornholmness”. What tourists are interested in when they visit the island is the “authentic” Bornholm, visually, linguistically, culturally. In our study we will analyze tourist merchandise (souvenirs), brochures, websites, signs, and other types of linguistic landscaping, and carry out interviews with both tourists and hosts about these themes. Methods from linguistic landscaping are already deployed in the Amager project, and with the Bornholm project they will be extended to this nonurban context. In a LANCHART perspective, one important focus point is to relate these types of analyses to the overall theme of language change. How does the process of commodification relate to dialect change: Commodification, since it tends to be focused on authenticity, will cultivate the original dialect but will that slow down the process of standardization or just enhance the number of registers available for performance at selected occasions? (the LANCHART team coordinated by Marie Maegaard). 5. Internationalization The LANCHART Centre was a major player in the attempt to create a Niels Bohr professorship for professor Nikolas Coupland, Cardiff, at the Universities of Copenhagen and Roskilde. The application for a Niels Bohr professorship was not supported, however. But Professor Nik Coupland has agreed to take up a half time research professorship at the Department of Scandinavian Research. This will give us the opportunity to realize some of the initiatives during the final LANCHART period. We single out among them: The creation of annual symposia and summer schools will ensure that we do not lose contact with the growth layers of young researchers that we have hitherto seen at such events. On the model of the CTR we plan to invite candidates for a Marie Curie post doc grant for a workshop in Copenhagen. This will be the obvious next step of keeping contact with the best PhDs from the summer schools. The LANCHART Centre has hosted a number of guests and will continue to do so, cf. the budget. Visitors come to Copenhagen in order to discuss general methodological questions. The methodological focus will be considerably strengthened in the coming period (cf. above). Hence we hope that the combined efforts of Nik Coupland’s visiting professorship, the SLICE project and a new focus will serve to place the LANCHART Centre even more firmly at the center of European and international sociolinguistic research. 6. International publications The centre staff has increased its output considerably during the first 7 years of its lifetime. Intensified efforts at publishing earlier in the cycle of empirical research have been modified by our allegiance to ‘large’ papers reporting on more than one study and reporting ‘final’ results. The international integration of the LANCHART Centre as well as renewed efforts at using the collective resources of the staff as a whole has resulted in a production format that makes use of internal peer reviewers in the guise of international experts as well as colleagues from the sociolinguistic milieu at and around the centre, before submitting papers to journals or anthologies. This has proved very helpful. In the final period of the centre we will optimize the production of first class papers even further. We intend to develop the internal peer review process and to stipulate that papers should be presented twice, the first time as an idea and the second time as a near finished paper. In this way we hope to achieve even more and even better papers - faster. 7. Research training The most important experience of the first 7 years at the LANCHART Centre is that the centre can and should play an important role in the recruitment and education of new PhD students. The best way to do this is to initiate undergraduate students from the second or third years and graduate students right from the start into research practices by taking them on as student assistants. The student assistants should gradually be promoted from transcribers through proof reading jobs to coders and field workers. This model has proved very successful and the track record of this centre is unique in this respect within the Humanities, for precisely that reason. This is one of many good reasons why the budget for the new period includes a heavy budget post for student assistance, cf. also the Bornholm project above. The centre continues to be an environment where former employees come to write their MA theses and receive supervision. 8. Organization Since coding and transcription stopped in November 2011, the senior researchers have by and large had no leadership duties; the organization has focused on producing papers. This means that the former intense meeting activities where each group leader reported regularly about the progress to date thus coordinating the empirical effort, have been substituted by less formal meeting types aimed at finalizing and improving papers for journals. We will adapt the organization to suit the new period’s priorities. This means re-introducing joint meetings but only on focused themes and problems that help bringing the group as such to a new level of understanding. Also it has proved necessary to develop some consistency in the use of terms and methods. The focus on theory and methodology lends itself particularly well to a more flexible meeting plan. The new period will start already in January 2013 with a three day external future workshop focusing on priorities in the upcoming period. Starting 2013 more attention will be given to exploiting the competencies of the senior researchers to attract students for summer schools as well as visitors to the centre, and to obtain grants for PhD scholarships from external funds. In this way, senior researchers will get ready to start their own projects so that the LANCHART will breed a new generation of centres. The support structure for such grant applications is already in place. 1. Frans Gregersen, Marie Maegaard and Nicolai Pharao: Contribution to Jacques Durand, Ulrike Guth and Gjert Kristoffersen (eds.) Handbook of Corpus Phonology on: The LANCHART Corpus, Oxford University Press, forthcoming (accepted) 2. Tore Kristiansen and Nikolas Coupland (eds.): Standard Languages and Language Standards in a changing Europe, Oslo: Novus 2011. This book will be sent separately. It may be kept. 3. Marie Maegaard, Torben Juel Jensen, Tore Kristiansen and Jens Normann Jørgensen: Diffusion of Language Change: Accommodation to a Moving Target, submitted for peer review Journal of Sociolinguistics, March 2012 4. Frans Gregersen and Michael Barner-Rasmussen: The Logic of Comparability: On genres and phonetic variation in a project on language change in real time. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory, 7/1, special issue: Sociolingustics and Corpus Linguistics, edited by Tyler Kendall and Gerard van Herk, p.7-36, 2011 5. J. Normann Jørgensen, Martha Karrebæk, Lian Malai Madsen and Janus Spindler Møller: Polylanguaging in Superdiversity, Diversities 13/2, special issue Language and Superdiversities, guest edited by Jan Blommaert, Ben Rampton and Massimiliano Spotti, p.23-38, 2011 6. Gert Foget Hansen and Nicolai Pharao: Differences in formant values caused by different microphone set ups, submitted for peer review, JASA, May 2012. 7. Torben Juel Jensen and Tanya Karoli Christensen: Promoting the demoted. Submitted for peer review, Lingua, May 2012. 8. Frans Gregersen, J. Normann Jørgensen and Janus Spindler Møller: Sideways, five methodological studies of sociolinguistic interviews, submitted for peer review, Language Variation and Change, April 2012 9. Nicolai Pharao, Marie Maegaard, Janus Spindler Møller and Tore Kristiansen: Indexical meanings of [s+] among Copenhagen youth: Social perception of a phonetic variant in different linguistic contexts. Submitted for peer review, Language in Society