1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 Dietary nitrate supplementation improves reaction time in type 2 diabetes: development and application of a novel nitrate-depleted beetroot juice placebo Mark Gilchrist Paul G. Winyard Jon Fulford Christine Anning Angela Shore Nigel Benjamin NIHR Exeter Clinical Research Facility and Institute of Biomedical and Clinical Science, University of Exeter Medical School (previously Peninsula College of Medicine and Dentistry), University of Exeter, Exeter EX2 5AX, UK Manuscript including references: 6839 Abstract: 300 words Number of tables and figures: 5 Corresponding author: Mark Gilchrist Clinical Lecturer in Renal Medicine NIHR Exeter Clinical Research Facility, University of Exeter Medical School, Royal Devon & Exeter Foundation NHS Trust, Barrack Road, EX2 5AX 01392 403059 m.gilchrist@exeter.ac.uk 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 1 44 Abstract 45 Background 46 In this substudy of the effect of dietary nitrate on blood pressure, endothelial function, 47 and insulin sensitivity in type 2 diabetes, we report the development of a novel nitrate 48 depleted beetroot juice for use clinical trials and determine if dietary nitrate 49 supplementation improved cognitive function in patients with type 2 diabetes mellitus. 50 Methods 51 Beetroot juice was treated with the anion exchange resin Purolite A520e. UV-vis- 52 spectrophotometry, and a blind taste test were performed along with determination of 53 sugar content, measurement of ascorbate and dehydroascorbate, the ionic composition 54 of juice and Proton NMR. Subsequently, 27 patients, age 67.2 +/-4.9 years, (18 male) 55 were recruited for a double blind, randomised, placebo-controlled crossover trial. 56 Participants were randomised to begin in either order beetroot juice (nitrate content 57 7.5mmol per 250mls) or placebo (nitrate depleted beetroot juice nitrate content 58 0.002mmol per 250mls). At the end of each 2 week supplementation period cognitive 59 function was assessed using E-prime, E-Studio software with 5 separate tests being 60 performed. The tests utilised in the present study have been adapted from the 61 Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB) 62 Results 63 The differences in the UV-vis spectra were comparable to the natural variation found in 64 differing cultivars. There were no discernable differences in taste, sugar content, or 65 Proton NMR. Ascorbate and dehydroascorbate were undetectable in either juice. After2 66 weeks of beetroot juiceSimple reaction time was significantly quicker in the active arm 2 67 at 327±40ms versus 341.8±52.7ms in the placebo arm, mean difference 13.9±25.6ms 68 (95% CI 3.8 to 24.0 ms), p=0.009. No other measures of cognitive function differed 69 between treatment arms. 70 Conclusion 71 We have developed an effective placebo beetroot juice for use in trials of 72 supplementation of dietary nitrate. Two weeks supplementation of the diet with 7.5 73 mmoles of nitrate per day caused a significant improvement in simple reaction time in 74 individuals with T2DM. 75 3 76 Introduction 77 78 Individuals with Type 2 Diabetes have measurable deficits in cognitive function when 79 compared to age and sex matched populations[1]. The risk of developing dementia 80 may be twice as high in individuals with diabetes compared with the background 81 population[2]. This accelerated decline in cognitive function may have several potential 82 mediators: hyperglycaemia, dyslipidaemia, genetic factors, and microangiopathy [3]. 83 84 Improvements in glycaemic control have produced mixed results. One small trial in 85 elderly subjects with type 2 diabetes, two weeks intensive management of glycaemic 86 control resulted in a non-significant trend to improvement in reaction time and significant 87 improvements in other cognitive measures[4]. Ryan et al’ [5] found an improvement in 88 working memory following significant reductions in fasting plasma glucose levels 89 following a 24 week intervention with either glyburide or rosiglitazone but no differences 90 in reaction time, spatial memory or rapid processing tests. In the largest study of the 91 effect of glycaemic control on cognitive function a subset of the participants from the 92 ACCORD trial underwent cognitive assessment and MRI scanning to determine total 93 brain volume at baseline, at 20 months and 40 months [6]. While there was a significant 94 reduction in the amount of total brain volume lost at 40 months in the intensive 95 glycaemic control group compared with the standard glycaemic control group there was 96 no difference between groups in cognitive performance. 97 4 98 Vascular dysfunction is thought to be an aetiopathogenic factor in Alzheimer’s disease 99 as well as vascular dementia [7]. The plasma nitrite concentration is becoming 100 established as a marker of endothelial nitric oxide (NO) production and therefore 101 indicative of vascular health [8]. Thus plasma nitrite may be a sensitive marker for risk 102 of cognitive decline. The potential relationship between plasma nitrate and nitrite and 103 cognitive function has been little studied. Two groups have found plasma NOx (total 104 nitrate plus nitrite) was reduced in patients with dementia (Alzheimer’s, vascular, and 105 mixed) [9] or Alzheimer’s dementia compared with healthy controls [10]. Another group 106 [11] found a small but statistically significant increase in plasma NOx in patients with 107 Alzheimer’s dementia compared to healthy controls 108 109 Endothelial function determined by brachial artery flow mediated dilation is impaired in 110 individuals with Alzheimer’s disease[7]. This impairment in endothelial function is 111 present in younger individuals with vascular risk factors and is associated with poorer 112 cognitive performance in the absence of overt clinically detectable cognitive impairment 113 [12]. 114 115 High levels of vegetable consumption and in particular green leafy vegetables appear to 116 slow the rate of age related cognitive decline[13]. These beneficial effects are often 117 attributed to a variety of anti-oxidants or vitamins. However in a group with 118 cardiovascular disease or risk factors for cardiovascular disease supplementation of 119 vitamin E, C, or beta-carotene did not alter the rate of slowing of cognitive function over 5 120 time [14]. It has been postulated that it may be the vascular actions of inorganic nitrate 121 in green leafy vegetables which mediate this protective effect [15; 16]. 122 123 Inorganic nitrate from the diet is involved in a complex cycle with nitrite and nitric oxide. 124 Briefly, nitrate is rapidly and completely absorbed from the stomach and small intestine 125 [17]. From the circulation, it is then concentrated in the salivary glands and secreted 126 into the mouth. Bacteria residing in crypts on the dorsum of the tongue use nitrate 127 rather than oxygen as an alternative electron acceptor [18]. In the process nitrate is 128 reduced to nitrite, which is again swallowed. Some of this nitrite will further be reduced 129 to nitric oxide (NO) in the acidic environment of the stomach with important localised 130 effects[19]. Some nitrite will be absorbed into the circulation where, via a number of 131 mechanisms, it is reduced to NO in the vasculature[20]. This may modulate vascular 132 tone and a number of cell signalling pathways. NO generated in this way and from the 133 nitric oxide synthase family of enzymes which utilise L-arginine and O2 as substrates is 134 rapidly oxidised to nitrate by oxyhaemoglobin. This nitrate is thought to act as a stable 135 storage reservoir of NO’s bioactivity [15]. 136 137 Dietary nitrate supplementation has previously been shown to lower blood pressure 138 [21; 22], improve endothelial function [21], improve exercise tolerance[23], enhance 139 muscle contractile efficiency and force production [24; 25], modulate gastric blood flow 140 [26], protect against ischaemia reperfusion injury[27], inhibit platelet aggregation [21], 141 and potentially play a key role in host defence [28]. For review see [29]. There is some 142 evidence that dietary nitrate supplementation may alter cerebral blood flow [30]. 6 143 Presley et al’s study [30] suggested that acute dietary nitrate supplementation increased 144 blood flow in cerebral white matter, notably in the dorso-lateral prefrontal cortex and 145 anterior cingulate cortex, areas associated with executive function. There was no 146 change in overall cerebral blood flow. In the central nervous system neuronal activity is 147 tightly coupled to local blood flow. This association is the cornerstone of investigational 148 imaging techniques such as fMRI [31]. Nitric oxide, whether derived from eNOS or 149 nNOS, is one of the key mediators of this relationship [32]. In a rat model, Piknova and 150 colleagues [31] showed inorganic nitrite can restore neurovascular coupling where it 151 has been disrupted by inhibitors of nNOS. Further evidence that dietary nitrate 152 supplementation may be neuro-protective comes from a series of in vitro experiments 153 by Gladwin’s group [33] which have demonstrated that neuroglobin may function as a 154 redox sensitive nitrite reductase which may protect neurons during oxidative stress. 155 156 We tested the hypothesis that supplementation of the diet with inorganic nitrate via 157 sequential reduction to nitrite and NO would augment cognitive performance in subjects 158 with T2DM. 159 7 160 Materials and Methods 161 162 The data presented are from a sub-study of a trial examining the effect of dietary nitrate 163 on blood pressure, endothelial function and insulin sensitivity in type 2 diabetes, where 164 additional details on methods can be found[34]. 165 166 This randomised double blind placebo controlled crossover trial was approved by the 167 Devon and Torbay Research Ethics Committee (study no. 09/H0202/43). All studies 168 were conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. 169 170 Participants were identified from the Exeter 10000 (EXTEND) bio-resource. This is a 171 large cohort of well characterised individuals who have consented to being contacted 172 about biomedical research projects. Twenty seven participants (9F:18M) with T2DM as 173 defined by WHO, of at least five years duration were recruited. 174 175 Using a double blind, randomised placebo-control crossover design, participants were 176 randomised to begin in either order beetroot juice (nitrate content 7.5mmol per 250mls) 177 or placebo (nitrate depleted beetroot juice nitrate content 0.002mmol per 250mls). 178 Subjects were instructed to consume one 250ml bottle per day, for 14 days with 179 cognitive function testing occurring at the end of the supplementation period. Subjects 180 then entered a four week washout period before entering the opposing arm of the study. 181 Throughout the study patients were asked to maintain their normal diet apart from the 182 juices given and not to change any other lifestyle factors. Participants continued their 183 usual antihypertensive medication and their usual hypoglycaemic medications. 8 184 Placebo Production 185 The placebo juice was produced by passing beetroot juice through a column containing 186 the anion exchange resin Purolite a520e which exchanges nitrate for chloride. As it is 187 widely used in the treatment of water for human consumption, it was considered that the 188 resin may offer a methodology suitable for the depletion of nitrate in beetroot juice 189 Nitrate concentration was determined by ozone chemiluminescence as per for plasma 190 samples. 191 192 UV-vis spectrophotometry 193 Relative absorbance in the UV-vis light spectrum was determined 194 spectrophotometrically using a Varian Cary 300 spectrophotometer. Samples were 195 diluted 1/10 for analysis. 196 197 Taste test 198 A convenience sample of 10 members of staff from our institute agreed to participate in 199 a blind taste test. They were randomly assigned to consume both juices in either order 200 with a one week gap to simulate the conditions of the larger trial. Subjects were asked 201 to determine the order of the juices they received. 202 203 Sugar content 204 The predominant carbohydrate in beetroot juice is sucrose [35]( Sucrose concentration 205 was determined by Armanda Pinhoe, James White Drinks, Ashbocking UK using a 206 Bellingham and Stanley 45-03 refractometer and expressed as oBx (degrees Brix 9 207 equals g solute per 100ml). Refractometery is widely used to determine sugar content of 208 food stuffs, including beetroot juice [35; 36] 209 210 Measurement of ascorbate and dehydroascorbate 211 Ascorbate and dehydroascorbate content were measured by reverse phase HPLC 212 performed on a Dionex DX500 HPLC system (Dionex, Sunnyvale, USA). The limit of 213 detection for this assay is 20μM. 214 215 Ionic composition of juice 216 The sodium, potassium, calcium, magnesium and chloride ion concentrations of the two 217 juices were determined using ion specific electrodes (Roche modular ISE unit). 218 219 220 221 Proton NMR 222 AV600 spectrometer (Bruker corporation, Ma, USA) at Queen Mary University of 223 London. Before analysis samples were subjected to three cycles of freeze drying and 224 re-suspension in D2O. Spectra were analysed using ACD/labs ACD/NMR Processor 225 Academic Edition (Advanced Chemistry Development, Canada). Proton NMR spectra of the placebo and active juices were obtained using a Bruker 226 227 228 All human studies took part in a temperature controlled (21.5-22.5oC) laboratory with the 229 subjects in the seated position. Patients arrived at 09.00 having completed an overnight 230 fast. The last bottle of beetroot juice was consumed with the patient’s evening meal 10 231 (typically between 18.00 and 20.00) the day prior to the study. No oral hypoglycaemic 232 agents or insulin were taken on the morning of the study. Any other regular medications 233 including antihypertensives were taken as usual. 234 235 Upon arrival at the laboratory blood samples were collected from the participants in 236 Lithium-Heparin tubes (7.5mL Monovette Lithium Heparin, Sarstedt Ltd., Leicester, UK). 237 Samples were centrifuged immediately in a pre-chilled centrifuge (4oC) at 3600 rpm for 238 10 minutes. The plasma was immediately separated into 1ml aliquots, flash frozen in 239 liquid nitrogen before transfer to a -80oC freezer. 240 241 Following blood sampling the cognitive tests were undertaken. The subject was seated 242 comfortably. Care was taken to ensure that the keyboard was within easy reach and 243 the monitor was at a distance that permitted the subject to see the screen comfortably. 244 If the subject normally used glasses they were permitted to do so for the study. 245 Subjects who were unfamiliar with a standard QWERTY keyboard were given a trial run 246 of a modified version of the study tests at the initial screening visit in order to familiarize 247 them with the keyboard. The only keys used are the “space bar” and number keys 1-4. 248 249 All cognitive tests were undertaken using E-prime, E-Studio software (Version 1.2, 250 Psychology Software Tools, Inc, Sharpsburg, PA) with five separate tests being 251 performed. The tests utilised in the present study have been adapted from the 252 Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB) [37] which have 253 been shown to be sensitive to subtle cognitive deficits in neurological and psychiatric 11 254 disorders [38]. The subject followed the on-screen instructions, moving through them at 255 their own pace. Verbal clarification of the instructions was given by the investigator if 256 requested by the participant. The cognitive tasks undertaken were as follows: 257 Reaction time 258 The subject was presented with a black screen. A white square appeared at random 259 locations on the screen at variable time intervals. The subject was instructed to press 260 the “space bar” as quickly as they could on seeing the white square. The time interval 261 between the square appearing and the subject’s acknowledgement was recorded 262 automatically. Thirty two events were analysed per visit. 263 264 Decision reaction time 265 The subject was presented with a black screen. White arrows pointing left or right 266 appeared on screen in random locations. Only one arrow was present on the screen at 267 any time. The sequence of left and right was random. The subject was instructed to 268 press the number “1” if the arrow pointed left and “2” if it pointed right. Accuracy and 269 time to depression of the key were recorded. Forty events were analysed per visit. 270 Rapid processing 271 The subject was presented with a three digit sequence, for example 4, 7, and 2. They 272 are asked to remember that sequence. Numbers between 1 and 9 appear on the 273 screen and were replaced by another number 300 milliseconds later. The subject was 274 instructed to depress the space bar when the previously described sequence of 275 numbers appeared, completely, in the order described, with no other numbers in 12 276 between. The percentage of correctly identified occurrences was recorded. Three sets 277 of seven occurrences of the number pattern were analysed per visit. 278 Shape memory 279 The subject was presented with a square composed of four smaller squares coloured 280 red, blue, yellow and green. Every large square comprised one red, one blue, one 281 yellow and one green square. The positioning of the squares within the square 282 changed from trial to trial. Three seconds after it first appeared the large square was 283 then hidden from view. There followed a variable time interval of between 1 and 5 284 seconds before the subject was asked to identify which of the four options presented 285 matches the square seen previously. Accuracy and time to decision were recorded. 286 Twenty five trials were analysed per visit. 287 Spatial memory 288 Subjects were presented with a black screen. A white square would appear on the 289 screen in a random location. The white box would appear in that location for 2.5 290 seconds. This would disappear and another white box would appear in another random 291 location. This would happen once more such that a total of three boxes had appeared. 292 293 A screen inviting the subjects to move to the next screen by pressing the space bar 294 appeared. The subject was then presented with a black screen containing two squares 295 of the same size as those seen previously. One square was located in the same 296 position as one of the squares seen previously. The other was located immediately 297 adjacent. The squares are labelled “1” and “2”. The subject has to determine whether 298 square “1” or square “2” is in the position of one previously. 13 299 300 They indicated this by pressing the corresponding number key. Five sets of three are 301 performed. Accuracy and time to answer were recorded. 302 Plasma nitrate and nitrite analysis 303 304 Plasma nitrate and nitrite concentrations were determined using a Sievers nitric oxide 305 analyser (Sievers NOA 280, Analytix Ltd, Durham, UK), and both the methods and 306 results for these determinations have been described previously[34]. 307 308 Statistical analysis 309 Data were tested for normality. Paired t tests were applied to data where the differences 310 in the parameter of interest between the active and the placebo conditions were 311 normally distributed with data presented as mean±SD. Wilcoxon signed rank test was 312 used for nonparametric data with data presented as median and IQR. Statistical 313 significance was accepted at P<0.05. 314 315 Results 316 Placebo production 317 Following treatment with Purolite a520e the nitrate concentration of the juice was 0.002 318 mM. The nitrate concentration of the juice in the active arm of the study was 30.75mM. 319 14 320 321 Taste test 322 Five subjects correctly guessed the order and five could not discern a difference or 323 guessed the wrong order. Kappa statistic for agreement was 0, that is no agreement, 324 with p=1. This is essential to the validity of the placebo. 325 326 Ascorbate and dehydroascorbate 327 Both the untreated and the placebo juice had ascorbate and dehydroascorbate 328 concentrations below the limits of detection for the HPLC assay (<20μM). Mean plasma 329 ascorbate concentrations in European populations are approximately 50µM[39]. Thus 330 there is unlikely to be any physiologically relevant change in ascorbate concentration 331 from consumption of beetroot juice in the present study. 332 333 The sucrose concentration determined by ascertaining Brix levels was unchanged pre 334 and post resin treatment at 10oBx. There appears to be no substantial change in the 335 concentrations of sodium, potassium or magnesium ions (table 1). The calcium ion 336 concentration appears to undergo a modest fall and the chloride concentration more 337 than doubles which is expected given the fact that nitrate concentration is lowered by 338 anion exchange 339 340 341 342 15 343 UV-Vis Spectroscopy 344 345 λmax was 487 nm of with an absorbance of 0.570 for the active and 0.416 for the placebo 346 were noted, thus the absorbance of the placebo juice is 73% of that of the active juice. 347 These peaks correspond to the absorbance spectra of betaxanthins known to be 348 present in beetroot juice[40]. Juice obtained from 2 cultivars that are used in 349 commercially available beetroot juice shots were also analysed with a peak absorbance 350 of 0.556 in the Gesar cultivar and 0.449 in the Bayer cultivar. It should be noted that. 351 λmax was at 489 nm for Gesar and 485 nm and Bayer. Thus there is a subtle variation in 352 colour between cultivars of untreated juice that is similar in nature to any variation 353 between resin treated juice and untreated juice (Figure 1) 354 355 Proton NMR 356 The spectra for active and placebo juices appear remarkably similar (Figures 2 and 3). 357 The peaks are likely be predominantly arising from protons related to sucrose, the most 358 abundant sugar in beetroot juice [35]. 359 360 Both spectra show several well resolved doublet resonances in the region 4.8-5.5 ppm 361 which correspond to the α anomeric proton of sugars [41]. Consistent with this the 362 doublets have small coupling constants ofc J1, 2 <4Hz. The β anomeric protons of 363 sugars have larger coupling constants (J1,2~9Hz) and appear at higher fields in the 364 region 4.1-4.7 ppm [41]. Some of the signals in this region may also be due to low field 365 ring proton resonances in sugar. 16 366 367 368 All 27 subjects, 18 (66.7%) males and 9 (33.3%) females, mean age 67.2±4.9 years, 369 successfully completed the study. Though previous studies have shown a blood 370 pressure lowering effect with dietary nitrate supplementation, no such effect was evident 371 in the current cohort. Full details of participant characteristics are in table two, 372 published previously[34]. 373 374 The 30 point mini mental state examination (MMSE) was performed on enrolment to 375 screen for significant cognitive impairment. MMSE score was 29.2±1.2 in the study 376 cohort. 377 378 HbA1c did not change between treatment arms at 7.61±1.12% in the placebo arm and 379 7.52±0.93 in the active arm. Subjects who monitored capillary blood glucose at home 380 reported no change. Fasting capillary blood glucose was not different between the two 381 study arms at 8.15±2.03 mmol/l in the placebo arm and 7.87±1.79 mmol/l in the active 382 arm, (mean difference 0.26±1.77 mmol/l, p=0.45, 95%CI -0.4764 to 1.044) 383 384 385 Reaction time was significantly quicker in the active arm at 327±40ms compared with 386 341.8±52.7ms in the placebo arm, mean difference 13.9±25.6 (95% CI 3.8 to 24.0 ms) 387 p=0.009 (paired t-test). This remained significant after applying the Bonferroni 388 correction for the use of multiple tests with statistical significance accepted at <0.01.No 17 389 other measure produced a significant difference between supplementation arms (table 390 3). There were no significant differences between visit 1 and visit 2 confirming the 391 absence of a training effect. 392 393 394 Plasma Nitrate and Nitrite 395 396 Two weeks of ingestion of nitrate-rich beetroot juice increased median plasma nitrate 397 and nitrite concentrations compared to placebo. As described previously for this cohort 398 of subjects, the median plasma nitrate concentration rose from 31 µM (interquartile 399 range: 19.8-41.6) in the placebo arm to 150 µM (IQR: 122.7-200.0) in the active arm 400 (p<0.001). The median plasma nitrite concentration rose from 232 nM (IQR 200-265) 401 in the placebo arm to 390 nM (median, IQR; 312-537) in the active arm, p<0.001[34]. 402 403 Neither change in plasma nitrite concentration nor absolute nitrite concentration were 404 correlated with reaction time (data not shown). 405 406 Recent data have shown a possible difference in platelet activation in response to 407 inorganic nitrate supplementation between males and females[42] . In the present 408 study females had a significantly higher plasma nitrite concentration in the placebo arm 409 at 263 nM (IQR 229-351) than males, plasma nitrite concentration 225 nM (IQR 184- 410 257), p=0.045. There was no significant difference between the sexes in nitrite 411 concentration after supplementation. Plasma nitrate concentration did not differ pre or 412 post supplementation between males and females. In line with this there were no 18 413 significant differences between the sexes in performance in cognitive tests following 414 nitrate supplementation (data not shown). 415 Discussion 416 417 The placebo described in the present trial has been used in multiple trials reported 418 previously, both by our group and by others. In terms of taste test, critical to its validity 419 as a placebo for use in blinded trials, we have demonstrated that it is indistinguishable 420 from the high nitrate juice. There are small differences in the ionic compositions of the 421 two juices but set in the context of typical daily intakes are unlikely to be of physiological 422 relevance. There is some loss of pigment in the production of the placebo, however in 423 the beetroot shot and placebo produced commercially this is not apparent to the naked 424 eye. Furthermore the UV-vis spectroscopy data suggest that the extent of the 425 difference between the juice and its placebo is not dissimilar to the difference that can 426 be observed between two commercially available cultivars. 427 428 This is the first trial showing dietary nitrate supplementation may improve cognitive 429 function. With the present data it is not possible to elucidate the mechanism 430 underpinning the improvement in simple reaction time. Two broad considerations that 431 must be made are whether it is due to a genuine improvement in cognition or whether 432 cognition remains unaffected but the reaction time decreases due to more rapid muscle 433 contraction [25], or indeed both. Certainly there is growing evidence for improvements 434 in physical performance following dietary nitrate supplementation [23; 24; 25]. 19 435 Regardless of the mechanism this finding may have important implications in a large 436 section of the elderly population. 437 438 439 People with T2DM have slower reaction times than age matched healthy controls to a 440 variety of stimuli [43; 44]. Reaction time appears to be predictive of overall cognitive 441 performance with ageing[45]. In an elderly population, a slower reaction time is 442 associated with an increased risk of falls[46]. As falls are associated with increased 443 mortality in elderly populations the improvement in reaction time finding may have direct 444 clinical implications[47]. 445 446 In an intervention trial where subjects with T2DM were given a balance training program 447 a statistically significant improvement in simple reaction time of 7ms was achieved [43]. 448 This was associated with a significant reduction in the risk of falling as determined by 449 the long-form physiological profile assessment. Thus the 13.9ms improvement we 450 observed may be clinically significant. It seems likely in Morrison’s trial that the 451 improvement in reaction time was due to an improvement in physical or musculoskeletal 452 performance rather than having a cognitive component. 453 454 A number of trials investigating the effects of dietary nitrate supplementation assess the 455 outcome measures at around the time of the peak in plasma nitrite which typically 456 occurs at around 3 hours post nitrate supplementation[21; 29]. It is worthy of note that 457 the improvement in reaction time occurred more than 12 hours following the last dose of 20 458 nitrate. This may, if anything, lead to an underestimate of the effect size particularly if 459 cerebral blood flow is a key mediator[30]. 460 461 Dietary intervention with ginseng panax has been studied to determine whether this can 462 improve cognitive function. In a trial in young healthy volunteers no change in simple 463 reaction time was seen[48]. Interestingly there was a 13.98ms difference in choice 464 reaction time between ginseng and placebo in contrast to the lack of difference in our 465 cohort. It should be noted that the Cochrane group has found no consistent effect of 466 ginseng on cognition [49]. 467 468 If the fall in simple reaction time seen in our study represents a true enhancement of 469 cognitive processing speed possible underlying mechanisms include improvements in 470 cerebral perfusion [30], better neurovascular coupling [31], or other as yet unidentified 471 improvements in cellular efficiency analogous to the improved but incompletely 472 understood increase in muscle contractile efficiency seen during exercise following 473 dietary nitrate supplementation [24; 25] 474 475 Nitrite’s potential role as a vasodilator in the systemic circulation is well described [50]. 476 Presley et al’s study would suggest that dietary nitrate supplementation can alter 477 cerebral blood flow [30]. This was however a small study and multiple dietary 478 constituents other than nitrate were altered. Work from our own group, however found 479 no such alteration in cerebral blood flow in a group of healthy older subjects, mean age 480 64±2.7 [51]. Conahey and colleagues work in new born lambs showed that while nitrite 21 481 crossed the blood brain barrier, CSF cGMP concentration did not change despite a 482 doubling of plasma cGMP concentration [52]. This would suggest systemic changes in 483 nitrite do not affect the tone of cerebral vasculature. 484 485 In the spatial and shape memory trials where reaction time was measured in addition to 486 the primary parameter of task accuracy the lack of difference between treatment arms 487 may be due to the greater variance in response times brought about by the increased 488 difficulty of the task. A larger study would be required to elucidate whether reaction time 489 improved on these measures. 490 491 The effect of dietary nitrate supplementation on cognitive function in healthy individuals 492 is not completely understood. Kelly’s study used three measures of cognitive function 493 (serial subtractions, rapid visual information processing, number recall) in an acute 494 supplementation trial with no difference found between active and placebo arms[51]. 495 For both healthy subjects and patients with diabetes the hypothesis that dietary nitrate 496 may be behind the retardation of progression of cognitive decline with aging seen in 497 studies reporting a protective effect from green leafy vegetables [13] needs to be tested 498 with a longer term prospective intervention study. 499 500 It could be argued that two weeks of an intervention is insufficient time in which to see 501 an improvement in cognitive performance. If cognitive changes were to arise as a result 502 of improvements in cellular efficiency analogous to the improved but incompletely 503 understood increase in muscle contractile efficiency or changes in blood flow then two 22 504 weeks is more than sufficient to procure these effects. Furthermore, in a small parallel 505 groups study two weeks intensive management of glycaemic control resulted in a non 506 significant trend to improvement in reaction time and significant improvements in other 507 cognitive measures in elderly subjects with type 2 diabetes[4]. 508 509 510 The assessment of cognitive function in the current study was not exhaustive. Testing 511 other cognitive domains may have produced different results. For example, in Ryan et 512 al’s [5] study where an improvement in working memory was demonstrated following 513 significant reductions in fasting plasma glucose levels following a 24 week intervention 514 with either glyburide or rosiglitazone there were no differences in reaction time, spatial 515 memory or rapid processing tests all of which were tested with a similar protocol to the 516 present study. 517 518 Though this is the largest study examining the effect of inorganic nitrate 519 supplementation in a group at high risk of developing cognitive impairment it is the 520 nevertheless small. As mentioned above for those tests with larger intra-subject 521 variance larger trials would be required. It is also likely that the study is underpowered 522 to find correlations between changes in plasma nitrite concentration and changes in 523 cognitive function tests. However the present study will inform power calculations for 524 future studies. 525 23 526 Determination of dietary nitrate’s possible effect on cognitive function in multiple 527 domains, acutely and in the longer term is worthy of further consideration and should be 528 the subject of further investigation. 529 530 531 532 533 Acknowledgements 534 given in this paper are those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of 535 NIHR, the NHS or the Department of Health. We thank all the study volunteers. The 536 authors wish to thank Dr Tim McDonald for assistance with analysis of the ionic 537 composition of the juices, Dr Michael Page and Prof Nick Smirnoff for assistance with 538 the ascorbate/dehydroascorbate analysis, and Dr Steve Simpson for assistance with the 539 proton NMR. We are grateful to James White Drinks Ltd. for the donation of juices 540 used in the study and in particular Armanda Pinhoe at James White Drinks and David 541 Upson of Stoke Farm Orchards for assistance in the production of the placebo juice. This project was supported by the NIHR Exeter Clinical Research Facility. The views 542 543 544 545 546 547 548 549 550 24 551 552 553 554 555 556 557 558 559 560 561 562 563 564 565 566 567 568 569 570 571 572 573 574 575 576 577 578 579 580 581 582 583 584 585 586 587 588 589 590 591 592 593 594 595 596 References [1] S. Euser, N. Sattar, J. Witteman, E. Bollen, E. Sijbrands, A. Hofman, I. Perry, M. Breteler, R. Westendorp, A Prospective Analysis of Elevated Fasting Glucose Levels and Cognitive Function in Older People: Results From PROSPER and the Rotterdam Study, Diabetes 59 (2010) 1601. [2] G.J. Biessels, S. Staekenborg, E. Brunner, C. Brayne, P. Scheltens, Risk of dementia in diabetes mellitus: a systematic review, The Lancet Neurology 5 (2006) 64-74. [3] Y.D. Reijmer, E. van den Berg, C. Ruis, L. Jaap Kappelle, G.J. Biessels, Cognitive dysfunction in patients with type 2 diabetes, Diabetes/Metabolism Research and Reviews 26 (2010) 507-519. [4] M. Naor, H.J. Steingrüber, K. Westhoff, Y. Schottenfeld-Naor, A.F. Gries, Cognitive function in elderly non-insulin-dependent diabetic patients before and after inpatient treatment for metabolic control, Journal of Diabetes and its Complications 11 (1997) 4046. [5] C.M. Ryan, M.I. Freed, J.A. Rood, A.R. Cobitz, B.R. Waterhouse, M.W.J. Strachan, Improving Metabolic Control Leads to Better Working Memory in Adults With Type 2 Diabetes, Diabetes Care 29 (2006) 345-351. [6] L.J. Launer, M.E. Miller, J.D. Williamson, R.M. Lazar, H.C. Gerstein, A.M. Murray, M. Sullivan, K.R. Horowitz, J. Ding, S. Marcovina, L.C. Lovato, J. Lovato, K.L. Margolis, P. O'Connor, E.W. Lipkin, J. Hirsch, L. Coker, J. Maldjian, J.L. Sunshine, C. Truwit, C. Davatzikos, R.N. Bryan, Effects of intensive glucose lowering on brain structure and function in people with type 2 diabetes (ACCORD MIND): a randomised open-label substudy, The Lancet Neurology 10 (2011) 969-977. [7] D.S. Dede, B. Yavuz, B.B. Yavuz, M. Cankurtaran, M. Halil, Z. Ulger, E.S. Cankurtaran, K. Aytemir, G. Kabakci, S. Ariogul, Assessment of Endothelial Function in Alzheimer's Disease: Is Alzheimer's Disease a Vascular Disease?, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 55 (2007) 1613-1617. [8] P. Kleinbongard, A. Dejam, T. Lauer, T. Jax, S. Kerber, P. Gharini, J. Balzer, R.B. Zotz, R.E. Scharf, R. Willers, A.N. Schechter, M. Feelisch, M. Kelm, Plasma nitrite concentrations reflect the degree of endothelial dysfunction in humans, Free Radical Biology and Medicine 40 (2006) 295-302. [9] L. Corzo, R. Zas, S. Rodríguez, L. Fernández-Novoa, R. Cacabelos, Decreased levels of serum nitric oxide in different forms of dementia, Neuroscience Letters 420 (2007) 263267. [10] M.L. Selley, Increased concentrations of homocysteine and asymmetric dimethylarginine and decreased concentrations of nitric oxide in the plasma of patients with Alzheimer’s disease, Neurobiology of Aging 24 (2003) 903-907. [11] H. Vural, B. Sirin, N. Yilmaz, I. Eren, N. Delibas, The role of arginine-nitric oxide pathway in patients with Alzheimer disease, Biological Trace Element Research 129 (2009) 58-64. [12] P.J. Smith, J.A. Blumenthal, M.A. Babyak, A. Hinderliter, A. Sherwood, Association of vascular health and neurocognitive performance in overweight adults with high blood pressure, Journal of Clinical & Experimental Neuropsychology: Official Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 33 (2011) 559-66. [13] J.H. Kang, A. Ascherio, F. Grodstein, Fruit and vegetable consumption and cognitive decline in aging women, Annals of Neurology 57 (2005) 713-720. 25 597 598 599 600 601 602 603 604 605 606 607 608 609 610 611 612 613 614 615 616 617 618 619 620 621 622 623 624 625 626 627 628 629 630 631 632 633 634 635 636 637 638 639 640 641 642 [14] J.H. Kang, N.R. Cook, J.E. Manson, J.E. Buring, C.M. Albert, F. Grodstein, Vitamin E, Vitamin C, Beta carotene, and cognitive function among women with or at risk of cardiovascular disease: The women's antioxidant and cardiovascular study, Circulation 119 (2009) 2772-2780. [15] J.O. Lundberg, M. Feelisch, H. Bjorne, E.A. Jansson, E. Weitzberg, Cardioprotective effects of vegetables: is nitrate the answer?, Nitric Oxide 15 (2006) 359-62. [16] M. Gilchrist, N. Benjamin, Vegetables and Diabetes: Is nitrate the answer?, British Medical Journal 341 (2010). [17] T.H. Florin, G. Neale, J.H. Cummings, The effect of dietary nitrate on nitrate and nitrite excretion in man, British Journal of Nutrition 64 (1990) 387-97. [18] C. Duncan, H. Dougall, P. Johnston, S. Green, R. Brogan, C. Leifert, L. Smith, M. Golden, N. Benjamin, Chemical generation of nitric oxide in the mouth from the enterosalivary circulation of dietary nitrate., Nature Medicine 1 (1995) 546-51. [19] N. Benjamin, F. O'Driscoll, H. Dougall, C. Duncan, L. Smith, M. Golden, H. McKenzie, Stomach NO synthesis, Nature 368 (1994) 502. [20] M. Gilchrist, A.C. Shore, N. Benjamin, Inorganic nitrate and nitrite and control of blood pressure, Cardiovascular Research 89 (2011) 492-498. [21] A.J. Webb, N. Patel, S. Loukogeorgakis, M. Okorie, Z. Aboud, S. Misra, R. Rashid, P. Miall, J. Deanfield, N. Benjamin, R. MacAllister, A.J. Hobbs, A. Ahluwalia, Acute blood pressure lowering, vasoprotective, and antiplatelet properties of dietary nitrate via bioconversion to nitrite., Hypertension 51 (2008) 784-90. [22] F.J. Larsen, B. Ekblom, K. Sahlin, J.O. Lundberg, E. Weitzberg, Effects of dietary nitrate on blood pressure in healthy volunteers, New England Journal of Medicine 355 (2006) 2792-3. [23] K.E. Lansley, P.G. Winyard, J. Fulford, A. Vanhatalo, S.J. Bailey, J.R. Blackwell, F.J. DiMenna, M. Gilchrist, N. Benjamin, A.M. Jones, Dietary nitrate supplementation reduces the O2 cost of walking and running: a placebo-controlled study, Journal of Applied Physiology 110 (2010) 591-600. [24] S.J. Bailey, J. Fulford, A. Vanhatalo, P.G. Winyard, J.R. Blackwell, F.J. DiMenna, D.P. Wilkerson, N. Benjamin, A.M. Jones, Dietary nitrate supplementation enhances muscle contractile efficiency during knee-extensor exercise in humans, Journal of Applied Physiology 109 (2010) 135-148. [25] A. Hernández, T.A. Schiffer, N. Ivarsson, A.J. Cheng, J.D. Bruton, J.O. Lundberg, E. Weitzberg, H. Westerblad, Dietary nitrate increases tetanic [Ca2+]i and contractile force in mouse fast-twitch muscle, The Journal of Physiology (2012). [26] J. Petersson, M. Carlström, O. Schreiber, M. Phillipson, G. Christoffersson, A. Jägare, S. Roos, E.Å. Jansson, A.E.G. Persson, J.O. Lundberg, L. Holm, Gastroprotective and blood pressure lowering effects of dietary nitrate are abolished by an antiseptic mouthwash, Free Radical Biology and Medicine 46 (2009) 1068-1075. [27] A. Webb, R. Bond, P. McLean, R. Uppal, N. Benjamin, A. Ahluwalia, Reduction of nitrite to nitric oxide during ischemia protects against myocardial ischemia-reperfusion damage, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 101 (2004) 13683-8. [28] C. Duncan, H. Li, R. Dykhuizen, R. Frazer, P. Johnston, G. MacKnight, L. Smith, K. Lamza, H. McKenzie, L. Batt, D. Kelly, M. Golden, N. Benjamin, C. Leifert, Protection against oral and gastrointestinal diseases: importance of dietary nitrate intake, oral nitrate 26 643 644 645 646 647 648 649 650 651 652 653 654 655 656 657 658 659 660 661 662 663 664 665 666 667 668 669 670 671 672 673 674 675 676 677 678 679 680 681 682 683 684 685 686 687 reduction and enterosalivary nitrate circulation, Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology, Part A Physiology. 118 (1997) 939-48. [29] M. Gilchrist, P.G. Winyard, N. Benjamin, Dietary nitrate - Good or bad?, Nitric Oxide 22 (2010) 104-109. [30] T.D. Presley, A.R. Morgan, E. Bechtold, W. Clodfelter, R.W. Dove, J.M. Jennings, R.A. Kraft, S. Bruce King, P.J. Laurienti, W. Jack Rejeski, J.H. Burdette, D.B. Kim-Shapiro, G.D. Miller, Acute effect of a high nitrate diet on brain perfusion in older adults, Nitric Oxide 24 (2011) 34-42. [31] B. Piknova, A. Kocharyan, A.N. Schechter, A.C. Silva, The role of nitrite in neurovascular coupling, Brain Research 1407 (2011) 62-68. [32] D. Attwell, A.M. Buchan, S. Charpak, M. Lauritzen, B.A. MacVicar, E.A. Newman, Glial and neuronal control of brain blood flow, Nature 468 (2010) 232-243. [33] M. Tiso, J. Tejero, S. Basu, I. Azarov, X. Wang, V. Simplaceanu, S. Frizzell, T. Jayaraman, L. Geary, C. Shapiro, C. Ho, S. Shiva, D.B. Kim-Shapiro, M.T. Gladwin, Human Neuroglobin Functions as a Redox-regulated Nitrite Reductase, Journal of Biological Chemistry 286 (2011) 18277-18289. [34] M. Gilchrist, P.G. Winyard, K. Aizawa, C. Anning, A. Shore, N. Benjamin, Effect of dietary nitrate on blood pressure, endothelial function, and insulin sensitivity in type 2 diabetes, Free Radical Biology and Medicine 60 (2013) 89-97. [35] L. Juszczak, M. Witczak, T. Fortuna, B. Solarz, Effect of Temperature and Soluble Solids Content on the Viscosity of Beetroot (Beta vulgaris) Juice Concentrate, International Journal of Food Properties 13 (2010) 1364-1372. [36] N. López, E. Puértolas, S. Condón, J. Raso, Á. Ignacio, Enhancement of the solid-liquid extraction of sucrose from sugar beet (Beta vulgaris) by pulsed electric fields, LWT Food Science and Technology 42 (2009) 1674-1680. [37] B.J. Sahakian, A.M. Owen, Computerized assessment in neuropsychiatry using CANTAB: Discussion paper, Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine 85 (1992) 399-402. [38] E. Joyce, S. Blumenthal, S. Wessely, Memory, attention, and executive function in chronic fatigue syndrome, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry 60 (1996) 495-503. [39] B. Olmedilla, F. Granado, S. Southon, A.J.A. Wright, I. Blanco, E. Gil-Martinez, H. Van Den Berg, B. Corridan, A.M. Roussel, M. Chopra, D.I. Thurnham, Serum concentrations of carotenoids and vitamins A, E, and C in control subjects from five European countries, British Journal of Nutrition 85 (2001) 227-238. [40] L.C.P. Gonçalves, B.M. Di Genova, F.A. Dörr, E. Pinto, E.L. Bastos, Effect of dielectric microwave heating on the color and antiradical capacity of betanin, Journal of Food Engineering 118 (2013) 49-55. [41] K.E. Falk, P.A. Jovall, P. Winyard, Proton NMR studies of a tetrasaccharide which is a receptor for uropathogenic E. coli bacteria, Acta Chemica Scandinavica Series B Organic Chemistry & Biochemistry 36 (1982) 558-60. [42] S. Velmurugan, V. Kapil, S.M. Ghosh, S. Davies, A. McKnight, Z. Aboud, R.S. Khambata, A.J. Webb, A. Poole, A. Ahluwalia, Antiplatelet effects of dietary nitrate in healthy volunteers: Involvement of cGMP and influence of sex, Free Radical Biology and Medicine 65 (2013) 1521-1532. [43] S. Morrison, S.R. Colberg, M. Mariano, H.K. Parson, A.I. Vinik, Balance Training Reduces Falls Risk in Older Individuals With Type 2 Diabetes, Diabetes Care 33 (2010) 748-750. 27 688 689 690 691 692 693 694 695 696 697 698 699 700 701 702 703 704 705 706 707 708 709 710 711 712 713 714 715 716 [44] S.J. Richerson, C.J. Robinson, J. Shum, A comparative study of reaction times between type II diabetics and non-diabetics, Biomedical Engineering Online 4 (2005) 12. [45] I.J. Deary, G. Der, Reaction time, age, and cognitive ability: Longitudinal findings from age 16 to 63 years in representative population samples, Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition 12 (2005) 187-215. [46] Y. Lajoie, S.P. Gallagher, Predicting falls within the elderly community: comparison of postural sway, reaction time, the Berg balance scale and the Activities-specific Balance Confidence (ABC) scale for comparing fallers and non-fallers, Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics 38 (2004) 11-26. [47] J. Gribbin, R. Hubbard, C. Smith, J. Gladman, S. Lewis, Incidence and mortality of falls amongst older people in primary care in the United Kingdom, Qjm 102 (2009) 477-83. [48] B.R. Sünram-Lea SI, Wesnes KA, Petrini O., The effect of acute administration of 400 mg of Panax ginseng on cognitive performance and mood in healthy young volunteers., Current Topics in Neutraceutical Research 3 (2005) 65-74. [49] J. Geng, J. Dong, H. Ni, M.S. Lee, T. Wu, K. Jiang, G. Wang, A.L. Zhou, R. Malouf, Ginseng for cognition, Cochrane database of systematic reviews (Online) 12 (2010). [50] K. Cosby, K.S. Partovi, J.H. Crawford, R.P. Patel, C.D. Reiter, S. Martyr, B.K. Yang, M.A. Waclawiw, G. Zalos, X. Xu, K.T. Huang, H. Shields, D.B. Kim-Shapiro, A.N. Schechter, R.O. Cannon, 3rd, M.T. Gladwin, Nitrite reduction to nitric oxide by deoxyhemoglobin vasodilates the human circulation., Nature Medicine 9 (2003) 1498-505. [51] J. Kelly, J. Fulford, A. Vanhatalo, J.R. Blackwell, O. French, S.J. Bailey, M. Gilchrist, P.G. Winyard, A.M. Jones, Effects of short-term dietary nitrate supplementation on blood pressure, O2 uptake kinetics, and muscle and cognitive function in older adults, American Journal of Physiology - Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology (2012). [52] G.R. Conahey, G.G. Power, A.O. Hopper, M.H. Terry, L.S. Kirby, A.B. Blood, Effect of Inhaled Nitric Oxide on Cerebrospinal Fluid and Blood Nitrite Concentrations in Newborn Lambs, Pediatr Res 64 (2008) 375-380. 717 718 719 720 721 722 723 724 28 725 Legends 726 727 Table1. Ionic composition of juice pre and post resin treatment in context of typical daily 728 intake. 729 730 Table 2:Participant characteristics reproduced with permission from [34] 731 732 Table 3: Effect of dietary nitrate supplementation on selected aspects of cognitive 733 function in subjects with T2DM. * P=0.009 734 Figure 1: UV-vis spectroscopy of A active juice with absorbance peak at 487nM of 0.570 735 and 0.416 for the placebo and B Juice from the Gesar and Bayer cultivars with 736 absorbances of 0.556 in and 0.449 respectively. 737 738 Figure 2: (A) Proton NMR spectra of active juice in the study, (B) Proton NMR spectra 739 for placebo juice used in the study. Both spectra have been normalised to the largest 740 peak at 3.7ppm. 741 742 743 744 745 746 747 29 748 Tables 749 750 Table 1. Source Placebo Typical Daily Intake Sodium 17.7 mM 16.4 mM 150mmol Potassium 76.6 mM 73.0 mM 71mmol Calcium 0.48 mM 0.38 mM 20mmol Magnesium 7.4 mM 7.7 mM 7mmol Chloride 27.3 mM 60.5 mM 150mmol 751 752 Table 2 Age (years) Mean 67.2 Std. Deviation 4.9 Duration of diabetes (years) 13.6 8.1 Mean office systolic blood pressure (mm Hg) 142.9 13.9 Mean office diastolic blood pressure (mm Hg) 81.1 9.2 BMI (Kg/m2) 30.8 3.2 HbA1c (%) 7.6 1.1 Serum Creatinine (mmol/l) 88.2 27.9 Total number of antihypertensives 2.1 1.1 Retinopathy 5 (18.5%) Neuropathy 8 (29.6%) Nephropathy 1 (3.7%) 753 30 754 Table 3: Placebo Active Reaction Time (ms) 341.8±52.7 327.9±40* Decision Reaction Time (ms) 716.6±161.3 721.3±169.4 Rapid Processing (accuracy %) Shape Memory (accuracy %) 71.8±11.1 94.8±6.9 72±13.2 95±4.5 Spatial Memory (accuracy %) 80.4±9.4 78.8±12.4 755 756 757 758 759 760 761 762 763 764 765 766 767 768 769 770 771 772 773 31 774 Figures 775 776 Figure1: 777 Active Placebo 778 Bayer Gesar 779 780 781 782 Wavelength Wavelength 783 784 785 786 787 788 789 790 791 792 793 794 795 796 32 797 798 799 Figure 2: A 800 801 802 803 804 805 806 807 B 33