DIETARY NITRATE SUPPLEMENTATION IMPROVES REACTION

advertisement
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
Dietary nitrate supplementation improves reaction time in type 2 diabetes: development
and application of a novel nitrate-depleted beetroot juice placebo
Mark Gilchrist
Paul G. Winyard
Jon Fulford
Christine Anning
Angela Shore
Nigel Benjamin
NIHR Exeter Clinical Research Facility and Institute of Biomedical and Clinical
Science, University of Exeter Medical School (previously Peninsula College of
Medicine and Dentistry), University of Exeter, Exeter EX2 5AX, UK
Manuscript including references: 6839
Abstract: 300 words
Number of tables and figures: 5
Corresponding author:
Mark Gilchrist
Clinical Lecturer in Renal Medicine
NIHR Exeter Clinical Research Facility,
University of Exeter Medical School,
Royal Devon & Exeter Foundation NHS Trust,
Barrack Road,
EX2 5AX
01392 403059
m.gilchrist@exeter.ac.uk
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
1
44
Abstract
45
Background
46
In this substudy of the effect of dietary nitrate on blood pressure, endothelial function,
47
and insulin sensitivity in type 2 diabetes, we report the development of a novel nitrate
48
depleted beetroot juice for use clinical trials and determine if dietary nitrate
49
supplementation improved cognitive function in patients with type 2 diabetes mellitus.
50
Methods
51
Beetroot juice was treated with the anion exchange resin Purolite A520e. UV-vis-
52
spectrophotometry, and a blind taste test were performed along with determination of
53
sugar content, measurement of ascorbate and dehydroascorbate, the ionic composition
54
of juice and Proton NMR. Subsequently, 27 patients, age 67.2 +/-4.9 years, (18 male)
55
were recruited for a double blind, randomised, placebo-controlled crossover trial.
56
Participants were randomised to begin in either order beetroot juice (nitrate content
57
7.5mmol per 250mls) or placebo (nitrate depleted beetroot juice nitrate content
58
0.002mmol per 250mls). At the end of each 2 week supplementation period cognitive
59
function was assessed using E-prime, E-Studio software with 5 separate tests being
60
performed. The tests utilised in the present study have been adapted from the
61
Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB)
62
Results
63
The differences in the UV-vis spectra were comparable to the natural variation found in
64
differing cultivars. There were no discernable differences in taste, sugar content, or
65
Proton NMR. Ascorbate and dehydroascorbate were undetectable in either juice. After2
66
weeks of beetroot juiceSimple reaction time was significantly quicker in the active arm
2
67
at 327±40ms versus 341.8±52.7ms in the placebo arm, mean difference 13.9±25.6ms
68
(95% CI 3.8 to 24.0 ms), p=0.009. No other measures of cognitive function differed
69
between treatment arms.
70
Conclusion
71
We have developed an effective placebo beetroot juice for use in trials of
72
supplementation of dietary nitrate. Two weeks supplementation of the diet with 7.5
73
mmoles of nitrate per day caused a significant improvement in simple reaction time in
74
individuals with T2DM.
75
3
76
Introduction
77
78
Individuals with Type 2 Diabetes have measurable deficits in cognitive function when
79
compared to age and sex matched populations[1]. The risk of developing dementia
80
may be twice as high in individuals with diabetes compared with the background
81
population[2]. This accelerated decline in cognitive function may have several potential
82
mediators: hyperglycaemia, dyslipidaemia, genetic factors, and microangiopathy [3].
83
84
Improvements in glycaemic control have produced mixed results. One small trial in
85
elderly subjects with type 2 diabetes, two weeks intensive management of glycaemic
86
control resulted in a non-significant trend to improvement in reaction time and significant
87
improvements in other cognitive measures[4]. Ryan et al’ [5] found an improvement in
88
working memory following significant reductions in fasting plasma glucose levels
89
following a 24 week intervention with either glyburide or rosiglitazone but no differences
90
in reaction time, spatial memory or rapid processing tests. In the largest study of the
91
effect of glycaemic control on cognitive function a subset of the participants from the
92
ACCORD trial underwent cognitive assessment and MRI scanning to determine total
93
brain volume at baseline, at 20 months and 40 months [6]. While there was a significant
94
reduction in the amount of total brain volume lost at 40 months in the intensive
95
glycaemic control group compared with the standard glycaemic control group there was
96
no difference between groups in cognitive performance.
97
4
98
Vascular dysfunction is thought to be an aetiopathogenic factor in Alzheimer’s disease
99
as well as vascular dementia [7]. The plasma nitrite concentration is becoming
100
established as a marker of endothelial nitric oxide (NO) production and therefore
101
indicative of vascular health [8]. Thus plasma nitrite may be a sensitive marker for risk
102
of cognitive decline. The potential relationship between plasma nitrate and nitrite and
103
cognitive function has been little studied. Two groups have found plasma NOx (total
104
nitrate plus nitrite) was reduced in patients with dementia (Alzheimer’s, vascular, and
105
mixed) [9] or Alzheimer’s dementia compared with healthy controls [10]. Another group
106
[11] found a small but statistically significant increase in plasma NOx in patients with
107
Alzheimer’s dementia compared to healthy controls
108
109
Endothelial function determined by brachial artery flow mediated dilation is impaired in
110
individuals with Alzheimer’s disease[7]. This impairment in endothelial function is
111
present in younger individuals with vascular risk factors and is associated with poorer
112
cognitive performance in the absence of overt clinically detectable cognitive impairment
113
[12].
114
115
High levels of vegetable consumption and in particular green leafy vegetables appear to
116
slow the rate of age related cognitive decline[13]. These beneficial effects are often
117
attributed to a variety of anti-oxidants or vitamins. However in a group with
118
cardiovascular disease or risk factors for cardiovascular disease supplementation of
119
vitamin E, C, or beta-carotene did not alter the rate of slowing of cognitive function over
5
120
time [14]. It has been postulated that it may be the vascular actions of inorganic nitrate
121
in green leafy vegetables which mediate this protective effect [15; 16].
122
123
Inorganic nitrate from the diet is involved in a complex cycle with nitrite and nitric oxide.
124
Briefly, nitrate is rapidly and completely absorbed from the stomach and small intestine
125
[17]. From the circulation, it is then concentrated in the salivary glands and secreted
126
into the mouth. Bacteria residing in crypts on the dorsum of the tongue use nitrate
127
rather than oxygen as an alternative electron acceptor [18]. In the process nitrate is
128
reduced to nitrite, which is again swallowed. Some of this nitrite will further be reduced
129
to nitric oxide (NO) in the acidic environment of the stomach with important localised
130
effects[19]. Some nitrite will be absorbed into the circulation where, via a number of
131
mechanisms, it is reduced to NO in the vasculature[20]. This may modulate vascular
132
tone and a number of cell signalling pathways. NO generated in this way and from the
133
nitric oxide synthase family of enzymes which utilise L-arginine and O2 as substrates is
134
rapidly oxidised to nitrate by oxyhaemoglobin. This nitrate is thought to act as a stable
135
storage reservoir of NO’s bioactivity [15].
136
137
Dietary nitrate supplementation has previously been shown to lower blood pressure
138
[21; 22], improve endothelial function [21], improve exercise tolerance[23], enhance
139
muscle contractile efficiency and force production [24; 25], modulate gastric blood flow
140
[26], protect against ischaemia reperfusion injury[27], inhibit platelet aggregation [21],
141
and potentially play a key role in host defence [28]. For review see [29]. There is some
142
evidence that dietary nitrate supplementation may alter cerebral blood flow [30].
6
143
Presley et al’s study [30] suggested that acute dietary nitrate supplementation increased
144
blood flow in cerebral white matter, notably in the dorso-lateral prefrontal cortex and
145
anterior cingulate cortex, areas associated with executive function. There was no
146
change in overall cerebral blood flow. In the central nervous system neuronal activity is
147
tightly coupled to local blood flow. This association is the cornerstone of investigational
148
imaging techniques such as fMRI [31]. Nitric oxide, whether derived from eNOS or
149
nNOS, is one of the key mediators of this relationship [32]. In a rat model, Piknova and
150
colleagues [31] showed inorganic nitrite can restore neurovascular coupling where it
151
has been disrupted by inhibitors of nNOS. Further evidence that dietary nitrate
152
supplementation may be neuro-protective comes from a series of in vitro experiments
153
by Gladwin’s group [33] which have demonstrated that neuroglobin may function as a
154
redox sensitive nitrite reductase which may protect neurons during oxidative stress.
155
156
We tested the hypothesis that supplementation of the diet with inorganic nitrate via
157
sequential reduction to nitrite and NO would augment cognitive performance in subjects
158
with T2DM.
159
7
160
Materials and Methods
161
162
The data presented are from a sub-study of a trial examining the effect of dietary nitrate
163
on blood pressure, endothelial function and insulin sensitivity in type 2 diabetes, where
164
additional details on methods can be found[34].
165
166
This randomised double blind placebo controlled crossover trial was approved by the
167
Devon and Torbay Research Ethics Committee (study no. 09/H0202/43). All studies
168
were conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.
169
170
Participants were identified from the Exeter 10000 (EXTEND) bio-resource. This is a
171
large cohort of well characterised individuals who have consented to being contacted
172
about biomedical research projects. Twenty seven participants (9F:18M) with T2DM as
173
defined by WHO, of at least five years duration were recruited.
174
175
Using a double blind, randomised placebo-control crossover design, participants were
176
randomised to begin in either order beetroot juice (nitrate content 7.5mmol per 250mls)
177
or placebo (nitrate depleted beetroot juice nitrate content 0.002mmol per 250mls).
178
Subjects were instructed to consume one 250ml bottle per day, for 14 days with
179
cognitive function testing occurring at the end of the supplementation period. Subjects
180
then entered a four week washout period before entering the opposing arm of the study.
181
Throughout the study patients were asked to maintain their normal diet apart from the
182
juices given and not to change any other lifestyle factors. Participants continued their
183
usual antihypertensive medication and their usual hypoglycaemic medications.
8
184
Placebo Production
185
The placebo juice was produced by passing beetroot juice through a column containing
186
the anion exchange resin Purolite a520e which exchanges nitrate for chloride. As it is
187
widely used in the treatment of water for human consumption, it was considered that the
188
resin may offer a methodology suitable for the depletion of nitrate in beetroot juice
189
Nitrate concentration was determined by ozone chemiluminescence as per for plasma
190
samples.
191
192
UV-vis spectrophotometry
193
Relative absorbance in the UV-vis light spectrum was determined
194
spectrophotometrically using a Varian Cary 300 spectrophotometer. Samples were
195
diluted 1/10 for analysis.
196
197
Taste test
198
A convenience sample of 10 members of staff from our institute agreed to participate in
199
a blind taste test. They were randomly assigned to consume both juices in either order
200
with a one week gap to simulate the conditions of the larger trial. Subjects were asked
201
to determine the order of the juices they received.
202
203
Sugar content
204
The predominant carbohydrate in beetroot juice is sucrose [35]( Sucrose concentration
205
was determined by Armanda Pinhoe, James White Drinks, Ashbocking UK using a
206
Bellingham and Stanley 45-03 refractometer and expressed as oBx (degrees Brix
9
207
equals g solute per 100ml). Refractometery is widely used to determine sugar content of
208
food stuffs, including beetroot juice [35; 36]
209
210
Measurement of ascorbate and dehydroascorbate
211
Ascorbate and dehydroascorbate content were measured by reverse phase HPLC
212
performed on a Dionex DX500 HPLC system (Dionex, Sunnyvale, USA). The limit of
213
detection for this assay is 20μM.
214
215
Ionic composition of juice
216
The sodium, potassium, calcium, magnesium and chloride ion concentrations of the two
217
juices were determined using ion specific electrodes (Roche modular ISE unit).
218
219
220
221
Proton NMR
222
AV600 spectrometer (Bruker corporation, Ma, USA) at Queen Mary University of
223
London. Before analysis samples were subjected to three cycles of freeze drying and
224
re-suspension in D2O. Spectra were analysed using ACD/labs ACD/NMR Processor
225
Academic Edition (Advanced Chemistry Development, Canada).
Proton NMR spectra of the placebo and active juices were obtained using a Bruker
226
227
228
All human studies took part in a temperature controlled (21.5-22.5oC) laboratory with the
229
subjects in the seated position. Patients arrived at 09.00 having completed an overnight
230
fast. The last bottle of beetroot juice was consumed with the patient’s evening meal
10
231
(typically between 18.00 and 20.00) the day prior to the study. No oral hypoglycaemic
232
agents or insulin were taken on the morning of the study. Any other regular medications
233
including antihypertensives were taken as usual.
234
235
Upon arrival at the laboratory blood samples were collected from the participants in
236
Lithium-Heparin tubes (7.5mL Monovette Lithium Heparin, Sarstedt Ltd., Leicester, UK).
237
Samples were centrifuged immediately in a pre-chilled centrifuge (4oC) at 3600 rpm for
238
10 minutes. The plasma was immediately separated into 1ml aliquots, flash frozen in
239
liquid nitrogen before transfer to a -80oC freezer.
240
241
Following blood sampling the cognitive tests were undertaken. The subject was seated
242
comfortably. Care was taken to ensure that the keyboard was within easy reach and
243
the monitor was at a distance that permitted the subject to see the screen comfortably.
244
If the subject normally used glasses they were permitted to do so for the study.
245
Subjects who were unfamiliar with a standard QWERTY keyboard were given a trial run
246
of a modified version of the study tests at the initial screening visit in order to familiarize
247
them with the keyboard. The only keys used are the “space bar” and number keys 1-4.
248
249
All cognitive tests were undertaken using E-prime, E-Studio software (Version 1.2,
250
Psychology Software Tools, Inc, Sharpsburg, PA) with five separate tests being
251
performed. The tests utilised in the present study have been adapted from the
252
Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB) [37] which have
253
been shown to be sensitive to subtle cognitive deficits in neurological and psychiatric
11
254
disorders [38]. The subject followed the on-screen instructions, moving through them at
255
their own pace. Verbal clarification of the instructions was given by the investigator if
256
requested by the participant. The cognitive tasks undertaken were as follows:
257
Reaction time
258
The subject was presented with a black screen. A white square appeared at random
259
locations on the screen at variable time intervals. The subject was instructed to press
260
the “space bar” as quickly as they could on seeing the white square. The time interval
261
between the square appearing and the subject’s acknowledgement was recorded
262
automatically. Thirty two events were analysed per visit.
263
264
Decision reaction time
265
The subject was presented with a black screen. White arrows pointing left or right
266
appeared on screen in random locations. Only one arrow was present on the screen at
267
any time. The sequence of left and right was random. The subject was instructed to
268
press the number “1” if the arrow pointed left and “2” if it pointed right. Accuracy and
269
time to depression of the key were recorded. Forty events were analysed per visit.
270
Rapid processing
271
The subject was presented with a three digit sequence, for example 4, 7, and 2. They
272
are asked to remember that sequence. Numbers between 1 and 9 appear on the
273
screen and were replaced by another number 300 milliseconds later. The subject was
274
instructed to depress the space bar when the previously described sequence of
275
numbers appeared, completely, in the order described, with no other numbers in
12
276
between. The percentage of correctly identified occurrences was recorded. Three sets
277
of seven occurrences of the number pattern were analysed per visit.
278
Shape memory
279
The subject was presented with a square composed of four smaller squares coloured
280
red, blue, yellow and green. Every large square comprised one red, one blue, one
281
yellow and one green square. The positioning of the squares within the square
282
changed from trial to trial. Three seconds after it first appeared the large square was
283
then hidden from view. There followed a variable time interval of between 1 and 5
284
seconds before the subject was asked to identify which of the four options presented
285
matches the square seen previously. Accuracy and time to decision were recorded.
286
Twenty five trials were analysed per visit.
287
Spatial memory
288
Subjects were presented with a black screen. A white square would appear on the
289
screen in a random location. The white box would appear in that location for 2.5
290
seconds. This would disappear and another white box would appear in another random
291
location. This would happen once more such that a total of three boxes had appeared.
292
293
A screen inviting the subjects to move to the next screen by pressing the space bar
294
appeared. The subject was then presented with a black screen containing two squares
295
of the same size as those seen previously. One square was located in the same
296
position as one of the squares seen previously. The other was located immediately
297
adjacent. The squares are labelled “1” and “2”. The subject has to determine whether
298
square “1” or square “2” is in the position of one previously.
13
299
300
They indicated this by pressing the corresponding number key. Five sets of three are
301
performed. Accuracy and time to answer were recorded.
302
Plasma nitrate and nitrite analysis
303
304
Plasma nitrate and nitrite concentrations were determined using a Sievers nitric oxide
305
analyser (Sievers NOA 280, Analytix Ltd, Durham, UK), and both the methods and
306
results for these determinations have been described previously[34].
307
308
Statistical analysis
309
Data were tested for normality. Paired t tests were applied to data where the differences
310
in the parameter of interest between the active and the placebo conditions were
311
normally distributed with data presented as mean±SD. Wilcoxon signed rank test was
312
used for nonparametric data with data presented as median and IQR. Statistical
313
significance was accepted at P<0.05.
314
315
Results
316
Placebo production
317
Following treatment with Purolite a520e the nitrate concentration of the juice was 0.002
318
mM. The nitrate concentration of the juice in the active arm of the study was 30.75mM.
319
14
320
321
Taste test
322
Five subjects correctly guessed the order and five could not discern a difference or
323
guessed the wrong order. Kappa statistic for agreement was 0, that is no agreement,
324
with p=1. This is essential to the validity of the placebo.
325
326
Ascorbate and dehydroascorbate
327
Both the untreated and the placebo juice had ascorbate and dehydroascorbate
328
concentrations below the limits of detection for the HPLC assay (<20μM). Mean plasma
329
ascorbate concentrations in European populations are approximately 50µM[39]. Thus
330
there is unlikely to be any physiologically relevant change in ascorbate concentration
331
from consumption of beetroot juice in the present study.
332
333
The sucrose concentration determined by ascertaining Brix levels was unchanged pre
334
and post resin treatment at 10oBx. There appears to be no substantial change in the
335
concentrations of sodium, potassium or magnesium ions (table 1). The calcium ion
336
concentration appears to undergo a modest fall and the chloride concentration more
337
than doubles which is expected given the fact that nitrate concentration is lowered by
338
anion exchange
339
340
341
342
15
343
UV-Vis Spectroscopy
344
345
λmax was 487 nm of with an absorbance of 0.570 for the active and 0.416 for the placebo
346
were noted, thus the absorbance of the placebo juice is 73% of that of the active juice.
347
These peaks correspond to the absorbance spectra of betaxanthins known to be
348
present in beetroot juice[40]. Juice obtained from 2 cultivars that are used in
349
commercially available beetroot juice shots were also analysed with a peak absorbance
350
of 0.556 in the Gesar cultivar and 0.449 in the Bayer cultivar. It should be noted that.
351
λmax was at 489 nm for Gesar and 485 nm and Bayer. Thus there is a subtle variation in
352
colour between cultivars of untreated juice that is similar in nature to any variation
353
between resin treated juice and untreated juice (Figure 1)
354
355
Proton NMR
356
The spectra for active and placebo juices appear remarkably similar (Figures 2 and 3).
357
The peaks are likely be predominantly arising from protons related to sucrose, the most
358
abundant sugar in beetroot juice [35].
359
360
Both spectra show several well resolved doublet resonances in the region 4.8-5.5 ppm
361
which correspond to the α anomeric proton of sugars [41]. Consistent with this the
362
doublets have small coupling constants ofc J1, 2 <4Hz. The β anomeric protons of
363
sugars have larger coupling constants (J1,2~9Hz) and appear at higher fields in the
364
region 4.1-4.7 ppm [41]. Some of the signals in this region may also be due to low field
365
ring proton resonances in sugar.
16
366
367
368
All 27 subjects, 18 (66.7%) males and 9 (33.3%) females, mean age 67.2±4.9 years,
369
successfully completed the study. Though previous studies have shown a blood
370
pressure lowering effect with dietary nitrate supplementation, no such effect was evident
371
in the current cohort. Full details of participant characteristics are in table two,
372
published previously[34].
373
374
The 30 point mini mental state examination (MMSE) was performed on enrolment to
375
screen for significant cognitive impairment. MMSE score was 29.2±1.2 in the study
376
cohort.
377
378
HbA1c did not change between treatment arms at 7.61±1.12% in the placebo arm and
379
7.52±0.93 in the active arm. Subjects who monitored capillary blood glucose at home
380
reported no change. Fasting capillary blood glucose was not different between the two
381
study arms at 8.15±2.03 mmol/l in the placebo arm and 7.87±1.79 mmol/l in the active
382
arm, (mean difference 0.26±1.77 mmol/l, p=0.45, 95%CI -0.4764 to 1.044)
383
384
385
Reaction time was significantly quicker in the active arm at 327±40ms compared with
386
341.8±52.7ms in the placebo arm, mean difference 13.9±25.6 (95% CI 3.8 to 24.0 ms)
387
p=0.009 (paired t-test). This remained significant after applying the Bonferroni
388
correction for the use of multiple tests with statistical significance accepted at <0.01.No
17
389
other measure produced a significant difference between supplementation arms (table
390
3). There were no significant differences between visit 1 and visit 2 confirming the
391
absence of a training effect.
392
393
394
Plasma Nitrate and Nitrite
395
396
Two weeks of ingestion of nitrate-rich beetroot juice increased median plasma nitrate
397
and nitrite concentrations compared to placebo. As described previously for this cohort
398
of subjects, the median plasma nitrate concentration rose from 31 µM (interquartile
399
range: 19.8-41.6) in the placebo arm to 150 µM (IQR: 122.7-200.0) in the active arm
400
(p<0.001). The median plasma nitrite concentration rose from 232 nM (IQR 200-265)
401
in the placebo arm to 390 nM (median, IQR; 312-537) in the active arm, p<0.001[34].
402
403
Neither change in plasma nitrite concentration nor absolute nitrite concentration were
404
correlated with reaction time (data not shown).
405
406
Recent data have shown a possible difference in platelet activation in response to
407
inorganic nitrate supplementation between males and females[42] . In the present
408
study females had a significantly higher plasma nitrite concentration in the placebo arm
409
at 263 nM (IQR 229-351) than males, plasma nitrite concentration 225 nM (IQR 184-
410
257), p=0.045. There was no significant difference between the sexes in nitrite
411
concentration after supplementation. Plasma nitrate concentration did not differ pre or
412
post supplementation between males and females. In line with this there were no
18
413
significant differences between the sexes in performance in cognitive tests following
414
nitrate supplementation (data not shown).
415
Discussion
416
417
The placebo described in the present trial has been used in multiple trials reported
418
previously, both by our group and by others. In terms of taste test, critical to its validity
419
as a placebo for use in blinded trials, we have demonstrated that it is indistinguishable
420
from the high nitrate juice. There are small differences in the ionic compositions of the
421
two juices but set in the context of typical daily intakes are unlikely to be of physiological
422
relevance. There is some loss of pigment in the production of the placebo, however in
423
the beetroot shot and placebo produced commercially this is not apparent to the naked
424
eye. Furthermore the UV-vis spectroscopy data suggest that the extent of the
425
difference between the juice and its placebo is not dissimilar to the difference that can
426
be observed between two commercially available cultivars.
427
428
This is the first trial showing dietary nitrate supplementation may improve cognitive
429
function. With the present data it is not possible to elucidate the mechanism
430
underpinning the improvement in simple reaction time. Two broad considerations that
431
must be made are whether it is due to a genuine improvement in cognition or whether
432
cognition remains unaffected but the reaction time decreases due to more rapid muscle
433
contraction [25], or indeed both. Certainly there is growing evidence for improvements
434
in physical performance following dietary nitrate supplementation [23; 24; 25].
19
435
Regardless of the mechanism this finding may have important implications in a large
436
section of the elderly population.
437
438
439
People with T2DM have slower reaction times than age matched healthy controls to a
440
variety of stimuli [43; 44]. Reaction time appears to be predictive of overall cognitive
441
performance with ageing[45]. In an elderly population, a slower reaction time is
442
associated with an increased risk of falls[46]. As falls are associated with increased
443
mortality in elderly populations the improvement in reaction time finding may have direct
444
clinical implications[47].
445
446
In an intervention trial where subjects with T2DM were given a balance training program
447
a statistically significant improvement in simple reaction time of 7ms was achieved [43].
448
This was associated with a significant reduction in the risk of falling as determined by
449
the long-form physiological profile assessment. Thus the 13.9ms improvement we
450
observed may be clinically significant. It seems likely in Morrison’s trial that the
451
improvement in reaction time was due to an improvement in physical or musculoskeletal
452
performance rather than having a cognitive component.
453
454
A number of trials investigating the effects of dietary nitrate supplementation assess the
455
outcome measures at around the time of the peak in plasma nitrite which typically
456
occurs at around 3 hours post nitrate supplementation[21; 29]. It is worthy of note that
457
the improvement in reaction time occurred more than 12 hours following the last dose of
20
458
nitrate. This may, if anything, lead to an underestimate of the effect size particularly if
459
cerebral blood flow is a key mediator[30].
460
461
Dietary intervention with ginseng panax has been studied to determine whether this can
462
improve cognitive function. In a trial in young healthy volunteers no change in simple
463
reaction time was seen[48]. Interestingly there was a 13.98ms difference in choice
464
reaction time between ginseng and placebo in contrast to the lack of difference in our
465
cohort. It should be noted that the Cochrane group has found no consistent effect of
466
ginseng on cognition [49].
467
468
If the fall in simple reaction time seen in our study represents a true enhancement of
469
cognitive processing speed possible underlying mechanisms include improvements in
470
cerebral perfusion [30], better neurovascular coupling [31], or other as yet unidentified
471
improvements in cellular efficiency analogous to the improved but incompletely
472
understood increase in muscle contractile efficiency seen during exercise following
473
dietary nitrate supplementation [24; 25]
474
475
Nitrite’s potential role as a vasodilator in the systemic circulation is well described [50].
476
Presley et al’s study would suggest that dietary nitrate supplementation can alter
477
cerebral blood flow [30]. This was however a small study and multiple dietary
478
constituents other than nitrate were altered. Work from our own group, however found
479
no such alteration in cerebral blood flow in a group of healthy older subjects, mean age
480
64±2.7 [51]. Conahey and colleagues work in new born lambs showed that while nitrite
21
481
crossed the blood brain barrier, CSF cGMP concentration did not change despite a
482
doubling of plasma cGMP concentration [52]. This would suggest systemic changes in
483
nitrite do not affect the tone of cerebral vasculature.
484
485
In the spatial and shape memory trials where reaction time was measured in addition to
486
the primary parameter of task accuracy the lack of difference between treatment arms
487
may be due to the greater variance in response times brought about by the increased
488
difficulty of the task. A larger study would be required to elucidate whether reaction time
489
improved on these measures.
490
491
The effect of dietary nitrate supplementation on cognitive function in healthy individuals
492
is not completely understood. Kelly’s study used three measures of cognitive function
493
(serial subtractions, rapid visual information processing, number recall) in an acute
494
supplementation trial with no difference found between active and placebo arms[51].
495
For both healthy subjects and patients with diabetes the hypothesis that dietary nitrate
496
may be behind the retardation of progression of cognitive decline with aging seen in
497
studies reporting a protective effect from green leafy vegetables [13] needs to be tested
498
with a longer term prospective intervention study.
499
500
It could be argued that two weeks of an intervention is insufficient time in which to see
501
an improvement in cognitive performance. If cognitive changes were to arise as a result
502
of improvements in cellular efficiency analogous to the improved but incompletely
503
understood increase in muscle contractile efficiency or changes in blood flow then two
22
504
weeks is more than sufficient to procure these effects. Furthermore, in a small parallel
505
groups study two weeks intensive management of glycaemic control resulted in a non
506
significant trend to improvement in reaction time and significant improvements in other
507
cognitive measures in elderly subjects with type 2 diabetes[4].
508
509
510
The assessment of cognitive function in the current study was not exhaustive. Testing
511
other cognitive domains may have produced different results. For example, in Ryan et
512
al’s [5] study where an improvement in working memory was demonstrated following
513
significant reductions in fasting plasma glucose levels following a 24 week intervention
514
with either glyburide or rosiglitazone there were no differences in reaction time, spatial
515
memory or rapid processing tests all of which were tested with a similar protocol to the
516
present study.
517
518
Though this is the largest study examining the effect of inorganic nitrate
519
supplementation in a group at high risk of developing cognitive impairment it is the
520
nevertheless small. As mentioned above for those tests with larger intra-subject
521
variance larger trials would be required. It is also likely that the study is underpowered
522
to find correlations between changes in plasma nitrite concentration and changes in
523
cognitive function tests. However the present study will inform power calculations for
524
future studies.
525
23
526
Determination of dietary nitrate’s possible effect on cognitive function in multiple
527
domains, acutely and in the longer term is worthy of further consideration and should be
528
the subject of further investigation.
529
530
531
532
533
Acknowledgements
534
given in this paper are those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of
535
NIHR, the NHS or the Department of Health. We thank all the study volunteers. The
536
authors wish to thank Dr Tim McDonald for assistance with analysis of the ionic
537
composition of the juices, Dr Michael Page and Prof Nick Smirnoff for assistance with
538
the ascorbate/dehydroascorbate analysis, and Dr Steve Simpson for assistance with the
539
proton NMR. We are grateful to James White Drinks Ltd. for the donation of juices
540
used in the study and in particular Armanda Pinhoe at James White Drinks and David
541
Upson of Stoke Farm Orchards for assistance in the production of the placebo juice.
This project was supported by the NIHR Exeter Clinical Research Facility. The views
542
543
544
545
546
547
548
549
550
24
551
552
553
554
555
556
557
558
559
560
561
562
563
564
565
566
567
568
569
570
571
572
573
574
575
576
577
578
579
580
581
582
583
584
585
586
587
588
589
590
591
592
593
594
595
596
References
[1] S. Euser, N. Sattar, J. Witteman, E. Bollen, E. Sijbrands, A. Hofman, I. Perry, M. Breteler, R.
Westendorp, A Prospective Analysis of Elevated Fasting Glucose Levels and Cognitive
Function in Older People: Results From PROSPER and the Rotterdam Study, Diabetes
59 (2010) 1601.
[2] G.J. Biessels, S. Staekenborg, E. Brunner, C. Brayne, P. Scheltens, Risk of dementia in
diabetes mellitus: a systematic review, The Lancet Neurology 5 (2006) 64-74.
[3] Y.D. Reijmer, E. van den Berg, C. Ruis, L. Jaap Kappelle, G.J. Biessels, Cognitive
dysfunction in patients with type 2 diabetes, Diabetes/Metabolism Research and Reviews
26 (2010) 507-519.
[4] M. Naor, H.J. Steingrüber, K. Westhoff, Y. Schottenfeld-Naor, A.F. Gries, Cognitive
function in elderly non-insulin-dependent diabetic patients before and after inpatient
treatment for metabolic control, Journal of Diabetes and its Complications 11 (1997) 4046.
[5] C.M. Ryan, M.I. Freed, J.A. Rood, A.R. Cobitz, B.R. Waterhouse, M.W.J. Strachan,
Improving Metabolic Control Leads to Better Working Memory in Adults With Type 2
Diabetes, Diabetes Care 29 (2006) 345-351.
[6] L.J. Launer, M.E. Miller, J.D. Williamson, R.M. Lazar, H.C. Gerstein, A.M. Murray, M.
Sullivan, K.R. Horowitz, J. Ding, S. Marcovina, L.C. Lovato, J. Lovato, K.L. Margolis,
P. O'Connor, E.W. Lipkin, J. Hirsch, L. Coker, J. Maldjian, J.L. Sunshine, C. Truwit, C.
Davatzikos, R.N. Bryan, Effects of intensive glucose lowering on brain structure and
function in people with type 2 diabetes (ACCORD MIND): a randomised open-label
substudy, The Lancet Neurology 10 (2011) 969-977.
[7] D.S. Dede, B. Yavuz, B.B. Yavuz, M. Cankurtaran, M. Halil, Z. Ulger, E.S. Cankurtaran, K.
Aytemir, G. Kabakci, S. Ariogul, Assessment of Endothelial Function in Alzheimer's
Disease: Is Alzheimer's Disease a Vascular Disease?, Journal of the American Geriatrics
Society 55 (2007) 1613-1617.
[8] P. Kleinbongard, A. Dejam, T. Lauer, T. Jax, S. Kerber, P. Gharini, J. Balzer, R.B. Zotz, R.E.
Scharf, R. Willers, A.N. Schechter, M. Feelisch, M. Kelm, Plasma nitrite concentrations
reflect the degree of endothelial dysfunction in humans, Free Radical Biology and
Medicine 40 (2006) 295-302.
[9] L. Corzo, R. Zas, S. Rodríguez, L. Fernández-Novoa, R. Cacabelos, Decreased levels of
serum nitric oxide in different forms of dementia, Neuroscience Letters 420 (2007) 263267.
[10] M.L. Selley, Increased concentrations of homocysteine and asymmetric dimethylarginine
and decreased concentrations of nitric oxide in the plasma of patients with Alzheimer’s
disease, Neurobiology of Aging 24 (2003) 903-907.
[11] H. Vural, B. Sirin, N. Yilmaz, I. Eren, N. Delibas, The role of arginine-nitric oxide pathway
in patients with Alzheimer disease, Biological Trace Element Research 129 (2009) 58-64.
[12] P.J. Smith, J.A. Blumenthal, M.A. Babyak, A. Hinderliter, A. Sherwood, Association of
vascular health and neurocognitive performance in overweight adults with high blood
pressure, Journal of Clinical & Experimental Neuropsychology: Official Journal of the
International Neuropsychological Society 33 (2011) 559-66.
[13] J.H. Kang, A. Ascherio, F. Grodstein, Fruit and vegetable consumption and cognitive
decline in aging women, Annals of Neurology 57 (2005) 713-720.
25
597
598
599
600
601
602
603
604
605
606
607
608
609
610
611
612
613
614
615
616
617
618
619
620
621
622
623
624
625
626
627
628
629
630
631
632
633
634
635
636
637
638
639
640
641
642
[14] J.H. Kang, N.R. Cook, J.E. Manson, J.E. Buring, C.M. Albert, F. Grodstein, Vitamin E,
Vitamin C, Beta carotene, and cognitive function among women with or at risk of
cardiovascular disease: The women's antioxidant and cardiovascular study, Circulation
119 (2009) 2772-2780.
[15] J.O. Lundberg, M. Feelisch, H. Bjorne, E.A. Jansson, E. Weitzberg, Cardioprotective effects
of vegetables: is nitrate the answer?, Nitric Oxide 15 (2006) 359-62.
[16] M. Gilchrist, N. Benjamin, Vegetables and Diabetes: Is nitrate the answer?, British Medical
Journal 341 (2010).
[17] T.H. Florin, G. Neale, J.H. Cummings, The effect of dietary nitrate on nitrate and nitrite
excretion in man, British Journal of Nutrition 64 (1990) 387-97.
[18] C. Duncan, H. Dougall, P. Johnston, S. Green, R. Brogan, C. Leifert, L. Smith, M. Golden,
N. Benjamin, Chemical generation of nitric oxide in the mouth from the enterosalivary
circulation of dietary nitrate., Nature Medicine 1 (1995) 546-51.
[19] N. Benjamin, F. O'Driscoll, H. Dougall, C. Duncan, L. Smith, M. Golden, H. McKenzie,
Stomach NO synthesis, Nature 368 (1994) 502.
[20] M. Gilchrist, A.C. Shore, N. Benjamin, Inorganic nitrate and nitrite and control of blood
pressure, Cardiovascular Research 89 (2011) 492-498.
[21] A.J. Webb, N. Patel, S. Loukogeorgakis, M. Okorie, Z. Aboud, S. Misra, R. Rashid, P.
Miall, J. Deanfield, N. Benjamin, R. MacAllister, A.J. Hobbs, A. Ahluwalia, Acute blood
pressure lowering, vasoprotective, and antiplatelet properties of dietary nitrate via
bioconversion to nitrite., Hypertension 51 (2008) 784-90.
[22] F.J. Larsen, B. Ekblom, K. Sahlin, J.O. Lundberg, E. Weitzberg, Effects of dietary nitrate on
blood pressure in healthy volunteers, New England Journal of Medicine 355 (2006)
2792-3.
[23] K.E. Lansley, P.G. Winyard, J. Fulford, A. Vanhatalo, S.J. Bailey, J.R. Blackwell, F.J.
DiMenna, M. Gilchrist, N. Benjamin, A.M. Jones, Dietary nitrate supplementation
reduces the O2 cost of walking and running: a placebo-controlled study, Journal of
Applied Physiology 110 (2010) 591-600.
[24] S.J. Bailey, J. Fulford, A. Vanhatalo, P.G. Winyard, J.R. Blackwell, F.J. DiMenna, D.P.
Wilkerson, N. Benjamin, A.M. Jones, Dietary nitrate supplementation enhances muscle
contractile efficiency during knee-extensor exercise in humans, Journal of Applied
Physiology 109 (2010) 135-148.
[25] A. Hernández, T.A. Schiffer, N. Ivarsson, A.J. Cheng, J.D. Bruton, J.O. Lundberg, E.
Weitzberg, H. Westerblad, Dietary nitrate increases tetanic [Ca2+]i and contractile force
in mouse fast-twitch muscle, The Journal of Physiology (2012).
[26] J. Petersson, M. Carlström, O. Schreiber, M. Phillipson, G. Christoffersson, A. Jägare, S.
Roos, E.Å. Jansson, A.E.G. Persson, J.O. Lundberg, L. Holm, Gastroprotective and blood
pressure lowering effects of dietary nitrate are abolished by an antiseptic mouthwash,
Free Radical Biology and Medicine 46 (2009) 1068-1075.
[27] A. Webb, R. Bond, P. McLean, R. Uppal, N. Benjamin, A. Ahluwalia, Reduction of nitrite
to nitric oxide during ischemia protects against myocardial ischemia-reperfusion damage,
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 101
(2004) 13683-8.
[28] C. Duncan, H. Li, R. Dykhuizen, R. Frazer, P. Johnston, G. MacKnight, L. Smith, K.
Lamza, H. McKenzie, L. Batt, D. Kelly, M. Golden, N. Benjamin, C. Leifert, Protection
against oral and gastrointestinal diseases: importance of dietary nitrate intake, oral nitrate
26
643
644
645
646
647
648
649
650
651
652
653
654
655
656
657
658
659
660
661
662
663
664
665
666
667
668
669
670
671
672
673
674
675
676
677
678
679
680
681
682
683
684
685
686
687
reduction and enterosalivary nitrate circulation, Comparative Biochemistry and
Physiology, Part A Physiology. 118 (1997) 939-48.
[29] M. Gilchrist, P.G. Winyard, N. Benjamin, Dietary nitrate - Good or bad?, Nitric Oxide 22
(2010) 104-109.
[30] T.D. Presley, A.R. Morgan, E. Bechtold, W. Clodfelter, R.W. Dove, J.M. Jennings, R.A.
Kraft, S. Bruce King, P.J. Laurienti, W. Jack Rejeski, J.H. Burdette, D.B. Kim-Shapiro,
G.D. Miller, Acute effect of a high nitrate diet on brain perfusion in older adults, Nitric
Oxide 24 (2011) 34-42.
[31] B. Piknova, A. Kocharyan, A.N. Schechter, A.C. Silva, The role of nitrite in neurovascular
coupling, Brain Research 1407 (2011) 62-68.
[32] D. Attwell, A.M. Buchan, S. Charpak, M. Lauritzen, B.A. MacVicar, E.A. Newman, Glial
and neuronal control of brain blood flow, Nature 468 (2010) 232-243.
[33] M. Tiso, J. Tejero, S. Basu, I. Azarov, X. Wang, V. Simplaceanu, S. Frizzell, T. Jayaraman,
L. Geary, C. Shapiro, C. Ho, S. Shiva, D.B. Kim-Shapiro, M.T. Gladwin, Human
Neuroglobin Functions as a Redox-regulated Nitrite Reductase, Journal of Biological
Chemistry 286 (2011) 18277-18289.
[34] M. Gilchrist, P.G. Winyard, K. Aizawa, C. Anning, A. Shore, N. Benjamin, Effect of dietary
nitrate on blood pressure, endothelial function, and insulin sensitivity in type 2 diabetes,
Free Radical Biology and Medicine 60 (2013) 89-97.
[35] L. Juszczak, M. Witczak, T. Fortuna, B. Solarz, Effect of Temperature and Soluble Solids
Content on the Viscosity of Beetroot (Beta vulgaris) Juice Concentrate, International
Journal of Food Properties 13 (2010) 1364-1372.
[36] N. López, E. Puértolas, S. Condón, J. Raso, Á. Ignacio, Enhancement of the solid-liquid
extraction of sucrose from sugar beet (Beta vulgaris) by pulsed electric fields, LWT Food Science and Technology 42 (2009) 1674-1680.
[37] B.J. Sahakian, A.M. Owen, Computerized assessment in neuropsychiatry using CANTAB:
Discussion paper, Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine 85 (1992) 399-402.
[38] E. Joyce, S. Blumenthal, S. Wessely, Memory, attention, and executive function in chronic
fatigue syndrome, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry 60 (1996) 495-503.
[39] B. Olmedilla, F. Granado, S. Southon, A.J.A. Wright, I. Blanco, E. Gil-Martinez, H. Van
Den Berg, B. Corridan, A.M. Roussel, M. Chopra, D.I. Thurnham, Serum concentrations
of carotenoids and vitamins A, E, and C in control subjects from five European countries,
British Journal of Nutrition 85 (2001) 227-238.
[40] L.C.P. Gonçalves, B.M. Di Genova, F.A. Dörr, E. Pinto, E.L. Bastos, Effect of dielectric
microwave heating on the color and antiradical capacity of betanin, Journal of Food
Engineering 118 (2013) 49-55.
[41] K.E. Falk, P.A. Jovall, P. Winyard, Proton NMR studies of a tetrasaccharide which is a
receptor for uropathogenic E. coli bacteria, Acta Chemica Scandinavica Series B Organic
Chemistry & Biochemistry 36 (1982) 558-60.
[42] S. Velmurugan, V. Kapil, S.M. Ghosh, S. Davies, A. McKnight, Z. Aboud, R.S. Khambata,
A.J. Webb, A. Poole, A. Ahluwalia, Antiplatelet effects of dietary nitrate in healthy
volunteers: Involvement of cGMP and influence of sex, Free Radical Biology and
Medicine 65 (2013) 1521-1532.
[43] S. Morrison, S.R. Colberg, M. Mariano, H.K. Parson, A.I. Vinik, Balance Training Reduces
Falls Risk in Older Individuals With Type 2 Diabetes, Diabetes Care 33 (2010) 748-750.
27
688
689
690
691
692
693
694
695
696
697
698
699
700
701
702
703
704
705
706
707
708
709
710
711
712
713
714
715
716
[44] S.J. Richerson, C.J. Robinson, J. Shum, A comparative study of reaction times between type
II diabetics and non-diabetics, Biomedical Engineering Online 4 (2005) 12.
[45] I.J. Deary, G. Der, Reaction time, age, and cognitive ability: Longitudinal findings from age
16 to 63 years in representative population samples, Aging, Neuropsychology, and
Cognition 12 (2005) 187-215.
[46] Y. Lajoie, S.P. Gallagher, Predicting falls within the elderly community: comparison of
postural sway, reaction time, the Berg balance scale and the Activities-specific Balance
Confidence (ABC) scale for comparing fallers and non-fallers, Archives of Gerontology
and Geriatrics 38 (2004) 11-26.
[47] J. Gribbin, R. Hubbard, C. Smith, J. Gladman, S. Lewis, Incidence and mortality of falls
amongst older people in primary care in the United Kingdom, Qjm 102 (2009) 477-83.
[48] B.R. Sünram-Lea SI, Wesnes KA, Petrini O., The effect of acute administration of 400 mg
of Panax ginseng on cognitive performance and mood in healthy young volunteers.,
Current Topics in Neutraceutical Research 3 (2005) 65-74.
[49] J. Geng, J. Dong, H. Ni, M.S. Lee, T. Wu, K. Jiang, G. Wang, A.L. Zhou, R. Malouf,
Ginseng for cognition, Cochrane database of systematic reviews (Online) 12 (2010).
[50] K. Cosby, K.S. Partovi, J.H. Crawford, R.P. Patel, C.D. Reiter, S. Martyr, B.K. Yang, M.A.
Waclawiw, G. Zalos, X. Xu, K.T. Huang, H. Shields, D.B. Kim-Shapiro, A.N. Schechter,
R.O. Cannon, 3rd, M.T. Gladwin, Nitrite reduction to nitric oxide by deoxyhemoglobin
vasodilates the human circulation., Nature Medicine 9 (2003) 1498-505.
[51] J. Kelly, J. Fulford, A. Vanhatalo, J.R. Blackwell, O. French, S.J. Bailey, M. Gilchrist, P.G.
Winyard, A.M. Jones, Effects of short-term dietary nitrate supplementation on blood
pressure, O2 uptake kinetics, and muscle and cognitive function in older adults,
American Journal of Physiology - Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology
(2012).
[52] G.R. Conahey, G.G. Power, A.O. Hopper, M.H. Terry, L.S. Kirby, A.B. Blood, Effect of
Inhaled Nitric Oxide on Cerebrospinal Fluid and Blood Nitrite Concentrations in
Newborn Lambs, Pediatr Res 64 (2008) 375-380.
717
718
719
720
721
722
723
724
28
725
Legends
726
727
Table1. Ionic composition of juice pre and post resin treatment in context of typical daily
728
intake.
729
730
Table 2:Participant characteristics reproduced with permission from [34]
731
732
Table 3: Effect of dietary nitrate supplementation on selected aspects of cognitive
733
function in subjects with T2DM. * P=0.009
734
Figure 1: UV-vis spectroscopy of A active juice with absorbance peak at 487nM of 0.570
735
and 0.416 for the placebo and B Juice from the Gesar and Bayer cultivars with
736
absorbances of 0.556 in and 0.449 respectively.
737
738
Figure 2: (A) Proton NMR spectra of active juice in the study, (B) Proton NMR spectra
739
for placebo juice used in the study. Both spectra have been normalised to the largest
740
peak at 3.7ppm.
741
742
743
744
745
746
747
29
748
Tables
749
750
Table 1.
Source
Placebo
Typical Daily
Intake
Sodium
17.7 mM
16.4 mM
150mmol
Potassium
76.6 mM
73.0 mM
71mmol
Calcium
0.48 mM
0.38 mM
20mmol
Magnesium
7.4 mM
7.7 mM
7mmol
Chloride
27.3 mM
60.5 mM
150mmol
751
752
Table 2
Age (years)
Mean
67.2
Std. Deviation
4.9
Duration of diabetes (years)
13.6
8.1
Mean office systolic blood pressure (mm Hg)
142.9
13.9
Mean office diastolic blood pressure (mm Hg)
81.1
9.2
BMI (Kg/m2)
30.8
3.2
HbA1c (%)
7.6
1.1
Serum Creatinine (mmol/l)
88.2
27.9
Total number of antihypertensives
2.1
1.1
Retinopathy
5 (18.5%)
Neuropathy
8 (29.6%)
Nephropathy
1 (3.7%)
753
30
754
Table 3:
Placebo
Active
Reaction Time (ms)
341.8±52.7
327.9±40*
Decision Reaction Time (ms)
716.6±161.3
721.3±169.4
Rapid Processing (accuracy %)
Shape Memory (accuracy %)
71.8±11.1
94.8±6.9
72±13.2
95±4.5
Spatial Memory (accuracy %)
80.4±9.4
78.8±12.4
755
756
757
758
759
760
761
762
763
764
765
766
767
768
769
770
771
772
773
31
774
Figures
775
776
Figure1:
777
Active
Placebo
778
Bayer
Gesar
779
780
781
782
Wavelength
Wavelength
783
784
785
786
787
788
789
790
791
792
793
794
795
796
32
797
798
799
Figure 2:
A
800
801
802
803
804
805
806
807
B
33
Download