How envy and task interdependence impact team members' pro-social behavior? Upward social comparisons (SC) often trigger malicious envy which leads to destructive interpersonal intentions & behaviors toward the envied person – e.g., belittling them, schadenfreude - i.e., taking pleasure in their suffering, deceiving them, sabotaging their outcomes and socially undermining them (Duffy et al., 2012; Gino & Pierce, 2009; Moran & Schweitzer, 2008; Salovey & Rodin, 1984; Smith & Kim, 2007; Vecchio, 1995). In the current research we test effects of envy in teams, a fundamental unit in modern organizations, in which SCs are prevalent. We focus on the understudied effects of envy on pro-social behavior: help giving (HG) and help seeking (HS), and we explore the moderating role of a key team characteristic; task interdependence (TI). Based on a cost-benefit account, we hypothesize that envy toward upward SC teammates will have detrimental effects on helping behavior, and that these will be moderated by TI. A summary of our proposed model is provided in Figure 1. Regarding HS, we posit that people will be particularly reluctant to seek help from envied (vs. non-envied) peers, since the psychological costs typically associated with HS - i.e., a threat to subjective independence, competence & self-esteem (Nadler, 1991; Nadler & Fisher, 1986; Wills & DePaulo, 1991) are augmented when the help provider is self-esteem threatening (i.e., envied). We further suggest that this reluctance will be moderated under high TI, which typically increases the recognition of the need for cooperation (Anderson & Williams, 1996), and thus increases the potential instrumental benefits of receiving help. Regarding HG, beside the typical costs, such as reduced personal resources (e.g., time) and benefits, such as improved self-esteem (e.g., Caprara & Steca, 2005), power (e.g., Flynn, 2003), perceived expertise and performance evaluations (e.g., Grant & Mayer, 2009), we offer that there are additional costs and benefits associated with providing help to and denying help from peers that are envied. Specifically, there is an added cost of helping envied peers – i.e., foregoing a specific advantage, and thereby an opportunity to decrease the disadvantageous gap, and an added benefit of denying them help – i.e., schadenfreude. Moreover, people may also ponder about the type of help to provide: “Autonomous help” which entails providing tools that enable recipients to solve problems independently, vs. “Dependent help” which consists of final solutions only, increasing recipients’ dependency (Bamberger & Levi, 2009; Nadler et al., 2009). With respect to this discrepancy, and following the cost-benefit analysis, we propose that people are likely to be particularly reluctant to providing envied peers with autonomous help. We further expect this reluctance to provide envied peers with help, especially autonomous help, to be moderated by TI. Because the intense interactions created by high TI results in a clarification of the team's helping norms (Hackman, 1992), providing help, and especially profound help to others (i.e., autonomous help), may serve as a means to comply with the team’s norms (Warburton & Terry, 2000; Grant & Patil, 2012). This is in contrast to low TI teams, where helping is less required and self-interested norms are more prevalent (Thompson, 1967; Van de Ven, Delbecq, & Koenig, 1976). We conducted 3 studies employing a 2 (envy: high vs. low, within subjects) X 2 (TI: high vs. low, between subjects) mixed factorial design. In all studies we first employed a pilot tested envy manipulation, providing participants with SC information about two virtual teammates; one superior, one neutral. We then measured participants' decisions to seek help from (study 1), and provide help to (studies 2, 3) these teammates, during a simulated team task, varying the extent to which information overlapped among team members (i.e., high vs. low TI). In study 1 (N=64), we measured HS by offering participants opportunities to ask for help on three pre-determined items. If they opted to ask, they chose between asking the envied or non-envied teammate. In study 2 (N=66), we focused on HG by presenting participants with simultaneous help requests from both teammates on three predetermined items. If they opted to comply, they chose the help recipient (the envied or non-envied teammate). In Study 3 (N=104), we focused on differentiating between the two types of HG (autonomous vs. dependent), as well as attempting to gain insight into underlying mechanisms. Thus, after deciding who to help, participants chose between providing final solutions (i.e., Dependent help), or final solutions & explaining how to solve this type of question (i.e., Autonomous help). In addition, in this study participants responded to a post task questionnaire measuring schadenfreude & other feelings and attitudes toward each teammate. Supporting our hypotheses, participants were indeed less likely to seek help from (Study 1) and provide help, especially autonomous help (Studies 2 & 3) to envied vs. non-envied peers, and this reluctance was moderated by TI (i.e., less apparent when TI was high). Moreover, in Study 3 we found that schadenfreude mediated the effect of envy on the decision to help. To conclude, consistent with a cost-benefit model, we demonstrate the understudied detrimental effects of envy on helping behaviors in teams, a fundamental organizational unit, and we propose a key team characteristic – i.e., TI as a potential moderator of these destructive effects. This research has several theoretical and practical implications. First, given that teams have become a fundamental unit in organizations, gaining insight into factors that affect teamwork has significant implications. Second, it expands the current literature on envy by focusing on its understudied effects on pro-social (rather than harming) behaviors, and particularly on its differential effects on varying types of help. Last, our results have practical managerial implications – e.g., managers might consider increasing TI, as an effective way of managing team-member envy and overcoming its potential downsides in terms of decreased cooperation between team members. Figure 1: Task Interdependence (TI) - Envy - Help Seeking (HS) Help Giving (HG) Help Giving Type (Aut. help) References 1. Anderson, S. E., & Williams, L. J. (1996). Interpersonal, job, and individual factors related to helping processes at work. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81(3), 282. 2. Bamberger, P. A., & Levi, R. (2009). Team-based reward allocation structures and the helping behaviors of outcome-interdependent team members. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 24(4), 300-327. 3. Caprara, G.V., & Steca, P. (2005). Self-efficacy beliefs as determinants of prosocial behavior conducive to life satisfaction across ages. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 24, 191–217. 4. Duffy, M. K., Scott, K. L., Shaw, J. D., Tepper, B. J., & Aquino, K. (2012). A social context model of envy and social undermining. Academy of Management Journal, 55(3), 643-666. 5. Flynn, F. J. (2003). What have you done for me lately? Temporal changes in subjective favor evaluations. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 91: 38–50. 6. Gino, F., & Pierce, L. (2009). The abundance effect: Unethical behavior in the presence of wealth. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 109(2), 142-155. 7. Grant, A. M., & Mayer, D. M. (2009). Good soldiers and good actors: Prosocial and impression management motives as interactive predictors of affiliative citizenship behaviors. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94(4), 900. 8. Grant, A. M., & Patil, S. V. (2012). Challenging the norm of self-interest: Minority influence and transitions to helping norms in work units. Academy of Management Review, 37(4), 547-568. 9. Hackman, J. R. (1992). Group influences on individuals in organizations. Consulting Psychologists Press. 10. Moran, S., & Schweitzer, M. E. (2008). When better is worse: Envy and the use of deception. Negotiation and Conflict Management Research, 1(1), 3-29. 11. Nadler, A. (1991). Help-seeking behavior: Psychological costs and instrumental benefits. Review of Personality and Social Psychology, 12, 290-311. 12. Nadler, A., & Fisher, J. D. (1986). The role of threat to self-esteem and perceived control in recipient reaction to help: Theory development and empirical validation. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 19, 81-122. 13. Nadler, A., Harpaz-Gorodeisky, G., & Ben-David, Y. (2009). Defensive helping: Threat to group identity, ingroup identification, status stability, and common group identity as determinants of intergroup help-giving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(5), 823. 14. Salovey, P., & Rodin, J. (1984). Some antecedents and consequences of socialcomparison jealousy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47(4), 780. 15. Smith, R. H., & Kim, S. H. (2007). Comprehending envy. Psychological Bulletin, 133(1), 46. 16. Thompson, J. D. (1967). Organizations in action. New York: McGraw-Hill. 17. Van de Ven, A. H., Delbecq, A. L., & Koenig Jr, R. (1976). Determinants of coordination modes within organizations. American Sociological Review, 322-338. 18. Vecchio, R. P. (1995). It’s not easy being green: jealousy and envy in the workplace. In G.R. Ferris (Ed.), Research in personnel and human resources management, 13, 201 – 244. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. 19. Warburton, J., & Terry, D. J. (2000). Volunteer decision making by older people: A test of a revised theory of planned behavior. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 22(3), 245-257. 20. Wills, T. A., & DePaulo, B. M. (1991). Interpersonal analysis of the help-seeking process. In C. R. Snyder & D. R. Forsyth (Eds.), Handbook of social and clinical psychology (pp. 350–375). Elmsford, NY: Pergamon.