Running Head: EUDAIMONIA VERSUS HEDONINA Huta, V. (2015). An overview of hedonic and eudaimonic well-being concepts. In L. Reinecke & M. B. Oliver (Eds.), Handbook of media use and well-being. Chapter 2. New York: Routledge. Manuscript accepted for publication on November 11, 2015. 1 Running Head: EUDAIMONIA VERSUS HEDONINA 2 An Overview of Hedonic and Eudaimonic Well-being Concepts This chapter provides an overview of the concepts studied in well-being research, to lay the groundwork for the research reported in this volume. The material is organized in terms of well-being “categories” and well-being “contents.” Throughout the chapter, important concepts are highlighted in bold. Well-being Categories As specified in a literature review by Huta and Waterman (2014), the well-being definitions of different researchers have fallen into four distinct categories: Orientations. Orientations represent what a person seeks in life and why, and they include the values, priorities, motives, ideals, and goals that guide the person’s chosen behaviors (e.g., seeking pleasure, pursuing personal growth). Behaviors. Behaviors represent the actual activities that the individual engages in (e.g., attending parties, writing down goals). Experiences. Experiences represent a person’s momentary or typical subjective feelings, emotions, and cognitive-affective appraisals (e.g., positive affect, feeling of meaning). Functioning. Functioning represents how well a person is doing in life and how far the person has come, in terms of their abilities, accomplishments, healthy habits, and healthy long-term functioning (e.g., prioritizing happiness, being skilled at savouring, being good at self-regulation, achieving maturity). Roughly speaking, therefore, orientation is what a person seeks, behavior is what a person does, experience is how well a person feels, and functioning is how well a person does. The first two categories – orientations and behaviors – represent ways of living, i.e., what a Running Head: EUDAIMONIA VERSUS HEDONINA 3 person chooses to do in life. The second two categories – experiences and functioning – are typically treated as well-being outcomes. Given that this book addresses media use and its effects on well-being outcomes, the remainder of this chapter focuses on experiences and functioning. Well-being Contents and the Distinction Between Hedonia and Eudaimonia Within each well-being category, researchers have studied different aspects or “contents” of well-being. For example, within the experiences category, the contents have included positive affect, negative affect, life satisfaction, meaning experience, elevation and inspiration, feelings of self-connectedness, subjective vitality, and so on. It is becoming clear that the various well-being contents which have been studied are neither a random array of highly disparate concepts, nor a set of minor variations on a single theme. The majority of researchers agree that the contents of well-being fall under two higherorder umbrellas – hedonia and eudaimonia – and that it is important to assess concepts from both umbrellas (Ryan & Deci, 2001). This chapter provides an outline of the theoretical and empirical basis of the hedoniceudaimonic distinction, followed by more detailed characterizations of the contents of wellbeing, organized into four sections: hedonic experiences, eudaimonic experiences, hedonic functioning, and eudaimonic functioning. Roughly speaking, hedonic contents involve pleasure/enjoyment/satisfaction, and comfort/painlessness/ease. These variables are associated with contents representing certain mindsets, including a focus on the self, the present moment, and the tangible, and a focus on taking and consuming what one needs and wants. Running Head: EUDAIMONIA VERSUS HEDONINA 4 Definitions of eudaimonic contents have varied more widely. Nevertheless, in a comprehensive review of psychological definitions of eudaimonia, Huta and Waterman (2014) found that four contents appeared in most or all definitions: meaning/value/relevance to a broader context, personal growth/self-realization/maturity, excellence/ethics/quality, and authenticity/autonomy/integration. These variables are associated with certain mindsets, including a balance of focusing on the self and others, a balance of focusing on the present and the future, a tendency to be guided by abstract and big-picture concepts, and a focus on cultivating and building what one values and envisions. A greater degree of eudaimonia exists if all these contents are combined (e.g., if a person ensures that their authentic self-expression is also ethical). Most researchers believe that people need both hedonia and eudaimonia to flourish, as discussed below. Hedonia and eudaimonia are not opposites, nor are they mutually exclusive – they are complementary psychological functions (Huta, 2015a). Furthermore, a person may derive a hedonic benefit but a eudaimonic loss from an activity, and vice versa. It is therefore important to assess both hedonic and eudaimonic variables when studying well-being outcomes. Evidence for the Distinction Between Hedonia and Eudaimonia A Long Theoretical History The hedonic-eudaimonic distinction has dominated discussions of well-being for at least 2500 years, beginning with philosophical texts, and continuing in the writings of humanists, psychoanalysts, and psychology researchers. The term eudaimonia was popularized by Aristotle in the 4th century BCE. He was the first to write at length about the hedonic-eudaimonic distinction, in his essay called the Nicomachean Ethics (see The Basic Works of Aristotle, 2001). Aristotle rejected hedonic Running Head: EUDAIMONIA VERSUS HEDONINA 5 pursuits, equating them with the behavior of lowly animals, and argued that a good life should be defined as eudaimonia, which is action in accordance with virtue and reason. Aristotle is often contrasted with Aristippus, who also lived in the 4th century BCE, and whose perspective was particularly far in the hedonic direction. Aristippus taught that pleasure is the only good in life and pain the only evil, regardless of how the pleasure or pain is produced. Various philosophers throughout history have sided with either the hedonic view or the eudaimonic view. Philosophers whose work was more aligned with hedonic principles included Hobbes, Bentham, and recently Onfray (2013). Philosophers whose work was more aligned with eudaimonic principles included Plato, Zeno of Citium, Marcus Aurelius, Kant, and recently Annas (1993), Kraut (2007), Norton (1976), and Tiberius (2013). In the writings of psychoanalysts, humanists, and early psychologists, we find the distinction between Freud’s (1920) pleasure principle on the one hand, and Jung’s (1933) individuation, Maslow’s (1970) self-actualization, and Allport’s (1955) maturity on the other. In psychology research, as shown in a review by Ryan and Deci (2001), conceptions of well-being can again be classified as hedonic or eudaimonic. There are some researchers who espouse only a hedonic perspective (e.g., Kahneman, 1999) or only a eudaimonic perspective (e.g., Ryff, 1989). However, most well-being researchers believe that a combination of both hedonia and eudaimonia is needed for an optimal existence, a combination which Keyes (2002) has termed “flourishing” and Seligman (2002) has termed the “full life.” Factor Analytic Evidence for the Hedonic-Eudaimonic Distinction Essential empirical support for the theoretical distinction between hedonic and eudaimonic outcomes would be provided by factor analyses of diverse well-being experiences, performed at the trait level and again at the state level. Ironically, such an analysis has yet to be Running Head: EUDAIMONIA VERSUS HEDONINA 6 published. (Note that parallel factor analyses on measures of functioning cannot be performed, as researchers have only developed measures of eudaimonic functioning, and not measures of hedonic functioning.) I have therefore reanalyzed, with Principal Components Analysis, the 7 studies of mine where I assessed at least 6 of the following 8 well-being experiences at the trait level: positive affect, negative affect, life satisfaction, carefreeness, meaning, elevation, self-connectedness, and vitality. The findings were reported in Huta (2014), and more details about the individual traitlevel studies can be found in Huta (2013a). In 6 of the 7 studies, there were 2 eigenvalues above 1; in the remaining study, 1 eigenvalue was above 1. Two-factor solutions were obtained in all the data sets, and the solutions were Varimax rotated. The pattern of loadings was fairly consistent from study to study. Mean loadings across the 7 studies are shown in the trait section of Table 1. I have similarly reanalyzed, for the purpose of this chapter, the 3 studies where I assessed at least 6 of the following 7 well-being experiences at the state level: positive affect, negative affect, carefreeness, meaning, elevation, self-connectedness, and vitality (I did not assess life satisfaction at the state level, as I considered it to be a trait-level variable). In one state level study, orientations and experiences were assessed at a given moment, multiple times per day; in a second study, they were assessed at a given bedtime as a summary of that day, for multiple days; and in the third study, they were assessed once a week as a summary of the previous week, for multiple weeks. More details about these state-level studies appear in Huta (2013a). In all 3 studies, there were 2 eigenvalues above 1, and despite differences in time span, all three studies had a similar pattern of loadings. Mean loadings across the three studies are shown in the state section of Table 1. Running Head: EUDAIMONIA VERSUS HEDONINA 7 Table 1. Mean Factor Loadings of Well-being Experiences Across 7 Past Trait Level Studies and 3 Past State Level Studies Trait Level (7 studies) State Level (3 studies) Eudaimonic Hedonic Eudaimonic Hedonic Elevation (Huta & Ryan, 2010) .08 .23 .84 .79 Meaning (Huta & Ryan, 2010) .22 .05 .79 .78 Self-connectedness (Huta, 2012) .19 .20 .76 .84 Negative Affect (Diener & Emmons, 1984) -.07 .01 -.81 -.85 Carefreeness (Huta & Ryan, 2010) .13 .34 .76 .76 Positive Affect (Diener & Emmons, 1984) .46 .55 .64 .76 Vitality (Ryan, & Frederick, 1997) .61 .58 .63 .49 Life Satisfaction (Diener, Emmons, et al., 1985) --.47 .58 Note. The references cited in his table refer to the authors who developed the measures used in the studies summarized. Primary loadings, as well as secondary loadings that are similar in magnitude to the primary loadings, are in bold. Trait-level studies had sample sizes n = 102-2094; state-level studies had sample sizes n = 34-113. I am also currently conducting a large study which includes additional trait-level wellbeing experiences to further test the distinction between hedonia and eudaimonia. As reported in Huta (2015b), Principal Components Analysis was performed on the first 677 participants, thereby providing preliminary but large-sample findings. Four factors had eigenvalues above 1. Following Varimax rotation, the loadings were as shown in Table 2. Table 2. Factor Loadings of Well-being Experiences: Preliminary Findings (n=677) from an Ongoing Trait Level Study Eudaimonic Meaning (Kern & Butler, 2013) Accomplishment (Kern & Butler, 2013) Meaning (Steger, Frazier, Oishi, & Kaler, 2006) Meaning (Huta & Ryan, 2010) Self-connectedness (Huta, 2012) Elevation (Huta & Ryan, 2010) Interest (Vittersø, Dyrdal, & Røysamb, 2005) Engagement (Kern & Butler, 2013) Flow – single-item (Delle Fave & Massimini, 1988) Flow – multi-item (Delle Fave & Massimini, 1988) Carefreeness (Huta & Ryan, 2010) Pleasure (Vittersø, Dyrdal, & Røysamb, 2005) Positive Affect (Diener & Emmons, 1984) Positive Emotions (Kern & Butler, 2013) Positive Experience (Diener, et al., 2009) .76 .71 .70 .69 .63 .59 .57 .54 .47 .40 .15 .38 .36 .36 .25 Positive Hedonic .18 .18 .15 .31 .28 .48 .53 .20 .28 .10 .78 .74 .74 .66 .66 Low Negative Hedonic, Need Satisfaction .41 .35 .28 .12 .28 -.04 .19 .14 .01 .53 .12 .39 .37 .34 .39 Personal Expressiveness .05 .03 .12 .28 .21 .27 .17 .12 -.06 .13 .09 .17 .17 .07 .08 Running Head: EUDAIMONIA VERSUS HEDONINA 8 Emotional Well-Being (Keyes, 2002) .26 .04 .49 .38 Positive Affect (Bradburn, 1969) .23 .17 .47 .48 Negative Affect (Bradburn, 1969) -.15 -.06 -.04 -.82 Negative Affect (Diener & Emmons, 1984) -.05 -.28 .01 -.80 Negative Experience (Diener et al., 2009) -.03 -.33 .03 -.72 Competence (Gagné, 2003) .36 .21 .07 .65 Relatedness (Gagné, 2003) .32 .23 .07 .64 Relatedness (Kern & Butler, 2013) .29 -.01 .48 .48 Autonomy (Gagné, 2003) .33 .25 -.03 .48 Vitality (Ryan, & Frederick, 1997) .28 .20 .44 .59 Life Satisfaction (Diener et al., 1985) .04 .48 .38 .44 Hedonic Enjoyment (Waterman, 1993) .14 .15 .04 .94 Personal Expressiveness (Waterman, 1993) .19 .15 .01 .93 Note. Primary loadings, as well as secondary loadings that are similar in magnitude to the primary loadings, are in bold. Sample size was n = 677. Several conclusions can tentatively be drawn from Tables 1 and 2: There were clearly distinct “flavours” of well-being experience. Feelings of meaning, self-connectedness, elevation, accomplishment, and interest/engagement/flow represented one type of experience, which can be called eudaimonic. Feelings of carefreeness, positive affect, and low negative affect represented a different kind of feeling, which can be called hedonic. Note, however, that positive affect also sometimes had substantial secondary links with eudaimonic concepts. Life satisfaction and subjective vitality (feeling intensely alive) related to both hedonic and eudaimonic variables. Feelings of need satisfaction – including needs for relatedness, autonomy, and competence – formed a distinct factor from eudaimonic concepts and positive hedonic concepts, but loaded together with low negative affect. More research is needed to confirm the place of need satisfaction, but this analysis tentatively suggested a link between need thwarting and negative emotions. Running Head: EUDAIMONIA VERSUS HEDONINA 9 Personal expressiveness (a feeling of deep connection with the important activities in a person’s life) has been proposed as a eudaimonic concept, but it formed a separate factor in the analysis, along with “hedonic enjoyment.” This may be due to differences in methodology rather than content. These two measures instructed participants to first list the activities of greatest importance to them and then rate how personally expressive and hedonically enjoyable they found each activity. This differed from the other measures analyzed, which simply instructed participants to rate themselves in general. Factor analyses of well-being orientations have similarly shown that the hedonic pursuit of pleasure and comfort differs from the eudaimonic pursuit of concerns such as authenticity, meaning, excellence, and growth (Huta & Ryan, 2010; Peterson, Park, & Seligman, 2005). Note, however, that the place of engagement remains unclear. In my factor analyses of orientation measures in the first 677 participants of the large study, the pursuit of engagement and challenge was distinct from the pursuit of authenticity, meaning, excellence, and growth when analyzing individual items, but not when analyzing whole scales (Huta, 2014). Other researchers have sometimes found that an engagement orientation and a eudaimonic orientation loaded on separate factors (Peterson et al., 2005), and sometimes found that they loaded on the same factor (Anić, 2014). Proposed Biological Basis of the Hedonic-Eudaimonic Distinction Steger and Shin (2012) have proposed a biological basis for the difference between hedonia (which they termed pleasure) and eudaimonia (which they termed mattering). They asserted that pleasure processes are based on the “hot” systems of the brain, while mattering processes are based on the “cold” systems. They defined “hot” systems as the older and simpler subcortical structures that humans share with other animals, which are designed to provide Running Head: EUDAIMONIA VERSUS HEDONINA 10 immediate subjective feelings of pleasure or pain to quickly evaluate safety and welfare. According to the authors, cold systems exist in the more recently evolved cerebral cortex, which is especially well-developed in humans, and they allow a person to engage in self-reflection, deliberation, and evaluation of things from perspectives that are abstract, value-based, and longterm. Vittersø, Dyrdal, and Røysamb (2005) have proposed that hedonic processes may be associated with endogenous opioids in the brain, which are involved in pleasure and regulation of homeostatic processes, while the eudaimonic pursuit of challenge and growth may be associated with the dopamine system, which is involved in interest and novelty-seeking. Both Hedonia and Eudaimonia are Needed Increasingly, well-being researchers are assessing both hedonia and eudaimonia, for a number of reasons. First, people who pursue both hedonia and eudaimonia have higher degrees of wellbeing than people who pursue only one or the other (Anić & Tončić, 2013; Huta & Ryan, 2010; Peterson et al., 2005). And people who score high on both hedonic and eudaimonic outcomes have higher degrees of mental health than other individuals (Keyes, 2002). Second, people who pursue both hedonia and eudaimonia have more well-rounded wellbeing than people who pursue only one or the other, because hedonia and eudaimonia fill somewhat different well-being niches. The pursuit of hedonia tends to be more linked with feelings of carefreeness, positive affect (at the state level), and low negative affect (at the state level), while the pursuit of eudaimonia tends to be more linked with feelings of meaning, elevation (at the trait level), and self-connectedness (Huta, 2013a). There is also some evidence to suggest that hedonic pursuits are associated with greater immediate well-being, while Running Head: EUDAIMONIA VERSUS HEDONINA 11 eudaimonic pursuits are associated with greater long-term well-being (Huta & Ryan, 2010; Steger et al., 2008). Furthermore, the pursuit of hedonia is mainly associated with personal wellbeing (e.g., Huta & Ryan, 2010; Peterson et al., 2005), while the pursuit of eudaimonia is associated with both personal well-being and a tendency to foster well-being in others (Huta, 2012; Huta, Pelletier, Baxter, & Thompson, 2012; Pearce, Huta, & Voloaca, 2015). Third, assessing only hedonic or only eudaimonic outcomes can lead to biased conclusions. For example, assessing only hedonic outcomes and ignoring eudaimonic outcomes can give the false impression that people derive little well-being from things such as proenvironmental behavior, interdependent self-construal, honesty, or the moment when they are engaged in a eudaimonic activity (Huta, 2014). It is also reasonable to assume that people can sometimes experience a hedonic benefit at the cost of a eudaimonic loss, or vice versa, e.g., when short-term fun impairs long-term relationships, or when progress on a project leads to loss of relaxation time. Thus, unlike past theoretical scholars, most psychology researchers do not side with one camp or the other, but rather believe that both hedonia and eudaimonia are important. Both Hedonia and Eudaimonia can be Taken to Excess This chapter focuses on the healthy forms of hedonia and eudaimonia, but it should be noted that each process can have a dark side too. Hedonia might derail into addiction, chronic escapism, destructive impulsivity, selfishness, antisocial behavior, greed, excessive consumerism, and so on. Eudaimonia might derail into a workaholic lifestyle, exhaustion, excessive self-sacrifice, overthinking things, excessive theorizing and loss of practicality, losing touch with one’s body, paralyzing existential angst, and so on. I believe that hedonia strays into Running Head: EUDAIMONIA VERSUS HEDONINA 12 unhealthy territory when it is not reined in by at least a bit of eudaimonia, and vice versa, though this hypothesis has yet to be tested. The Contents of Hedonic and Eudaimonic Experiences and Functioning Below is a listing of the various contents (i.e., variables) that have been associated with hedonia or eudaimonia, organized into four sections – hedonic experiences, eudaimonic experiences, hedonic functioning, and eudaimonic functioning. The overview is meant to be telegraphic to aid the reader in quickly developing a bird’s eye view of the hedonic-eudaimonic distinction. A general term for each type of content is listed as a heading (e.g., positive affect), and this is followed by a list of associated concepts (e.g., positive affect, positive affect balance, emotional well-being, hedonic enjoyment, etc.). Only one or two key references are cited for each concept; references to empirical support focus largely on correlates of hedonic and eudaimonic orientations, the category where I think the hedonic-eudaimonic distinction is clearest (Huta, 2015a). Further comments are added only where I feel they are essential. Reviews that expand in more detail on elements of hedonia and eudaimonia can be found in Haybron, 2008; Huta, 2013b, 2015a; Keyes & Annas, 2009; Ryan & Deci, 2001; Steger & Shin, 2012. One of my main interests is to develop an integrated understanding of the hedoniceudaimonic distinction, and this chapter has the most complete listing of contents I have developed to date, though it is always a work in progress. In addition, for the first time, this chapter outlines what hedonic functioning might look like, as this set of variables has been missing from the literature – the focus has been on eudaimonic functioning only. Note that it is rare for a given variable to be purely hedonic or purely eudaimonic, and that hedonia is typically accompanied by some degree of eudaimonic process, and vice versa. Running Head: EUDAIMONIA VERSUS HEDONINA 13 Also, well-being concepts vary in the degree to which they lean towards hedonia or eudaimonia, with some leaning strongly in one direction and others leaning only somewhat in one direction. Hedonic Experiences Positive affect. Associated concepts: positive affect (Diener, Suh, Lucas, & Smith, 1999); positive affect balance (Bradburn, 1969; Ryff, 1989); emotional well-being (Keyes, 2002); hedonic enjoyment (Waterman, 1993); pleasure, joyfulness, enjoyment, fun (Diener & Emmons, 1984). Positive affect is one of the most commonly used measures of well-being, the other being life satisfaction. When obtaining one-factor solutions with only the hedonic measures in the studies in Tables 1 (positive affect, negative affect, carefreeness) and with these same measures in the study in Table 2, positive affect accounted for about 75% of the variance in the common “hedonic factor” (even when life satisfaction was added to the analysis). This suggests that positive affect alone can serve as a good proxy for hedonic experience. Note that positive affect has been linked much more to a hedonic orientation at the state level, but has had similar links with hedonic and eudaimonic orientations at the trait level (Huta, 2013a; Henderson, Knight, & Richardson, 2013; Vella-Brodrick, Park, & Peterson, 2009). The factor analyses in Tables 1 and 2 suggest a similar pattern. Overall, therefore, one might say that positive affect has a primary relationship with hedonia but also a substantial secondary relationship with eudaimonia. Satisfaction. Associated concepts: satisfaction (Heathwood, 2006); feeling that personal wants and needs are met (Waterman, 1993); feeling that homeostasis is achieved (Vittersø & Søholt, 2011); replenishment, feeling well stocked up (Huta, 2015a); release of tension between how things are and how one would like them to be. Running Head: EUDAIMONIA VERSUS HEDONINA 14 Satisfaction seems to be more often understood as hedonic satisfaction – fulfilling physical/emotional needs and wants – and thus is classified in this section. Nevertheless, eudaimonic satisfaction – fulfilling cognitive/value-based goals and standards – exists as well. In addition, satisfaction is related to life satisfaction (which is more cognitive and global, and thus appears in the section on experiences that are both hedonic and eudaimonic), and to feelings of psychological need satisfaction (which are often construed as markers that important needs are met rather than ends in themselves, and thus appear in a separate section). Carefreeness. Associated concepts: carefreeness (Huta & Ryan, 2010). This feeling has been especially strongly linked to a hedonic orientation (Henderson et al., 2013; Huta, 2013a). Carefreeness likely derives from the hedonic focus on the here and now, and thus represents an emotional and cognitive disconnection from ongoing or broad concerns. Low negative affect. Associated concepts: low levels of negative affect, emotional pain, depression, anxiety, stress, strain (Diener at al., 1999; Fowers, Molica, & Procacci 2010); high levels of comfort, relaxation, ease (Huta & Ryan, 2010). When well-being has been assessed using more than one variable, researchers have most commonly used the three elements of what Diener termed subjective well-being – positive affect, life satisfaction, and low negative affect. After reviewing three decades of research on subjective well-being, Diener and colleagues (1999) concluded that negative affect should be assessed separately from positive affect, especially at the trait level where they can sometimes coexist. Running Head: EUDAIMONIA VERSUS HEDONINA 15 Negative affect has been linked much more to a hedonic orientation at the state level, but had weak or no links with hedonic and eudaimonic orientations at the trait level (Huta, 2013a; Henderson et al., 2013; Vella-Brodrick et al., 2009). Experiences Reflecting Need Satisfaction Feelings of psychological need satisfaction loaded with low negative affect in Table 2, but they are really somewhat distinct from other well-being outcomes, as these feelings are not seen as ends in themselves but rather as markers that a psychological need has been satisfied. Thus, experiences of need satisfaction appear here in a section of their own. Feeling related. Associated concepts: relatedness (Deci & Ryan, 2000); relationships (Seligman, 2011); love (Fredrickson, 2013). We are such highly social animals that an assessment of relatedness (as an experience or as functioning) appears in the majority of well-being “short-lists.” While most measures of relatedness have focused on relatedness to other people, some researchers are beginning to argue that the experience of relatedness extends beyond the social realm, and can include feelings of connectedness with animals, nature, the divine, and so on (Dambrun & Ricard, 2011; Leary, Tipsord, & Tate, 2008). Feeling autonomous. Associated concepts: autonomy, feeling free to be oneself and make one’s own choices (Deci & Ryan, 2000). Feeling competent. Associated concepts: competence (Deci & Ryan, 2000). Of the three need satisfaction experiences posited by Self-Determination Theory (Deci & Ryan, 2000) – autonomy, competence, and relatedness – all three have been linked to both hedonic and eudaimonic orientations, but the experience of competence has been more strongly linked to a eudaimonic orientation than to a hedonic one (Mack et al., 2011). Running Head: EUDAIMONIA VERSUS HEDONINA 16 Other needs. The feelings of relatedness, competence, and autonomy are the need satisfaction variables that have received the most empirical attention, but other psychological needs have also been proposed, including a need for meaning (Andersen, Chen, & Carter, 2000; Frankl, 1988), a need for safety and security (Kasser, 2009), and a need for esteem (Maslow, 1968). Note that relatedness, competence, and autonomy have been defined by some researchers as functioning that is assumed to belong specifically to eudaimonia, and thus variations on these three concepts appear again in the eudaimonic functioning section. Eudaimonic Experiences Meaning experience. Associated concepts: feelings of meaning, value, resonance, purpose, broad implications (Huta & Ryan, 2010; Huta, 2015c); feelings of meaning and purpose (Steger, 2012); meaningfulness (Delle Fave, Brdar, Freire, Vella-Brodrick, & Wissing, 2011a). A eudaimonic orientation and eudaimonic behavior have been linked with feelings of meaning and purpose (Henderson et al., 2013; Huta, 2013a; Steger et al., 2008). Most scholars would agree that, if there is a single eudaimonic outcome to be measured, it should be meaning. Meaning has repeatedly been used as a proxy for all things eudaimonic (e.g., Baumeister, Vohs, Aaker, & Garbinsky, 2013; Delle Fave et al., 2011a; King & Napa, 1998). I have argued that the assessment of well-being experience should include at minimum the following “Big Four”: positive affect, life satisfaction, low negative affect, and meaning (Huta, 2013a). When obtaining one-factor solutions with only the eudaimonic variables in the studies in Table 1 (meaning, elevation, and self-connectedness), meaning tended to account for about 70% of the variance in the common “eudaimonic factor.” This remained true when analyzing the same three measures in the study in Table 2, even when one of the other meaning Running Head: EUDAIMONIA VERSUS HEDONINA 17 measures was used instead, and even when the measure of accomplishment and any one of the measures of engagement was added to the analysis. This suggests that meaning alone can serve as a good proxy for eudaimonic experience. Elevation. Associated concepts: moral elevation (Haidt, 2000); peak experience (Maslow, 1970); elevation (Huta & Ryan, 2010); awe (Keltner & Haidt, 2003); inspiration (Thrash & Elliot, 2003); transcendence or sense of connection with a greater whole (Csikszentmihalyi, 2000); peak experience (Maslow, 1964); spiritual transcendence (Piedmont, 1999); aesthetic experience (Konečni, 2005); heightened awareness and clarity. A eudaimonic orientation has been linked with feelings of elevation (which include moral elevation, awe, inspiration, and transcendence) (Henderson et al., 2013; Huta, 2013a). Feeling integrated. Associated concepts: self-connectedness (Huta, 2012); authenticity (Schlegel, Hicks, King, & Arndt, 2011); feeling right and centered (Norton, 1976). A eudaimonic orientation has been linked with feelings of self-connectedness (Huta, 2013a). Personal expressiveness. Associated concepts: personal expressiveness, exceptional personal fit with one’s activities (Waterman, 1993). The concept of personal expressiveness is listed here as a eudaimonic experience based on theory and findings showing that self-realization values (a eudaimonic concept) are associated significantly more with personal expressiveness than with hedonic enjoyment (Waterman, 1993; Waterman, Schwartz, & Conti, 2008). Feeling of accomplishment. Associated concepts: accomplishment (Seligman, 2011); authentic pride (as opposed to hubristic pride, Tracy & Robins, 2007). Running Head: EUDAIMONIA VERSUS HEDONINA 18 Feeling of engagement. Associated concepts: interest (Vittersø & Søholt, 2011); engagement (Seligman, 2011); flow, immersion (Csikszentmihalyi, 2000). Experiences That are Both Hedonic and Eudaimonic Life satisfaction and domain satisfaction. Associated concepts: life satisfaction (Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985); domain satisfaction, e.g., satisfaction with one’s work, leisure, social relationships, family, finances (Diener et al., 1999). There is much controversy regarding the place of life satisfaction, with perhaps half the research community arguing that it is a hedonic concept (e.g., Ryan & Deci, 2001; Vittersø & Søholt, 2011), the other half arguing that it is the final common outcome of both hedonic and eudaimonic considerations (e.g., Sheldon, 2013; Waterman, 2007), and a small minority arguing that it is in fact a eudaimonic concept (Sumner, 1996). I have come to believe that the degree to which a “satisfaction” scale reflects hedonia depends very much on the wording of the items. To the degree that the wording refers to “getting” what one wants and needs, satisfaction is a hedonic concept, as hedonia is about taking for oneself. To the degree that the wording refers to “behaving” in congruence with one’s values and true self, and progressing towards eudaimonic goals, life satisfaction is a eudaimonic concept. And to the degree that the wording is very global, or reflects both getting things and living and progressing in the ways one values, life satisfaction is a summary of both hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. The most commonly used measure of life satisfaction, the Satisfaction With Life Scale (Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985), has items touching on all three of the above possibilities: hedonic – “So far I have gotten the important things I want in life”; eudaimonic – “If I could live my life over, I would change almost nothing”; and global – “I am satisfied with life.” This may explain why the scale has related similarly to hedonic and eudaimonic orientations (e.g., Huta & Ryan, 2010; Running Head: EUDAIMONIA VERSUS HEDONINA 19 Henderson et al., 2013; Peterson, Ruch, Beerman, Park, & Seligman, 2007). Based on these findings, “life” satisfaction is not listed with hedonia but rather in this section for variables related to both hedonia and eudaimonia. When researchers have assessed well-being using a single concept, they have most often assessed life satisfaction. I feel that as long as a life satisfaction measure is worded globally, or has items reflecting both hedonic and eudaimonic concepts, it can be a reasonable proxy for well-being in general. When forcing one-factor solutions in the studies in Tables 1 and 2 and when including all of the variables, I found that life satisfaction accounted for about 50% of the variance represented by the single “overall well-being” factor representing both hedonia and eudaimonia, which was substantial. It is worth noting, though, that it was positive affect which tended to load highest in the one-factor solutions, and it accounted for about 70% of the variance in the overall well-being factor. Empirically, therefore, positive affect was the strongest single representative of overall well-being. Happiness. Associated concepts: subjective happiness (Lyubomirsky & Lepper, 1999). Conceptually, the term happiness tends to be used in one of two ways: as a global assessment of all aspects of well-being (feeling “happy about” things), or as a description of the hedonic state of feeling good (simply feeling “happy”). Empirically, Schueller and Seligman (2011) found that happiness related to both hedonic and eudaimonic orientations, and somewhat more to a eudaimonic orientation. Thus, on average, happiness likely deserves to be grouped with outcomes reflecting both hedonia and eudaimonia. Vitality. Associated concepts: subjective vitality, feeling alive and energized (Ryan & Frederick, 1997). Running Head: EUDAIMONIA VERSUS HEDONINA 20 Vitality was originally theorized to belong with eudaimonia (Ryan & Deci, 2001), but empirically it has related similarly to hedonic and eudaimonic orientations (Henderson et al., 2013; Huta, 2013a). Functioning as an Outcome Eudaimonic functioning has largely been championed by Ryff (1989), whose definition is referred to as psychological well-being and consists of six concepts: personal growth, purpose in life, environmental mastery, autonomy/non-conformism, positive relationships with others, and self-acceptance (see also Ryff & Singer, 2008). Additional concepts in the eudaimonic functioning category have included, among others, self-regulation, ethics, contribution, and thoughtfulness, as detailed below. Variables reflecting hedonic functioning have certainly been studied – for example, in Fordyce’s (1983) 14 fundamentals of happiness, several fundamentals are largely hedonic in nature: making happiness a high priority, taking care of oneself, not worrying needlessly, and focusing on the present. However, no explicit and coherent conception of hedonic functioning exists, one that proposes things that hedonically oriented people are especially good at. For the purpose of this chapter, and for furthering my own research, I have developed a conception of what hedonic functioning might look like. Hedonic Functioning Savouring and playfulness. Associated concepts: making happiness a high priority (Fordyce, 1983); ability to savour, which includes luxuriating, basking, marvelling, and thanksgiving (Bryant & Veroff, 2007); playfulness and humour (Ruch, Proyer, & Weber, 2010). Running Head: EUDAIMONIA VERSUS HEDONINA 21 A hedonic orientation has been linked to the character strength of playfulness/humour and to the extraversion facet of excitement-seeking (Buschor, Proyer, & Ruch, 2013; Huta, 2013a; Peterson et al., 2007; Ruch et al., 2010). Note that thanksgiving is included under hedonic functioning for now, because it was theoretically grouped with savouring by Bryant and Veroff (2007), but it may prove to have at least as strong a link with eudaimonia, given the link between eudaimonia and appreciation (Huta & Ryan, 2010). Gratitude/thanksgiving is therefore listed again in the section on functioning that reflects both hedonia and eudaimonia. Sensuality. Associated concepts: sensuality, sexuality, passion, spirit, immersion in physical experiences and activities, being in touch with one’s body, being in touch with one’s primal side. The concept of sensuality overlaps with the physical component of savouring that is referred to as luxuriating, but involves not only appreciating physicality but also expressing and identifying with physicality. Light-heartedness. Associated concepts: not worrying needlessly (Fordyce, 1983); carefreeness as an ability, light-heartedness, ability to not take things too seriously, capacity to let go and take a break. A carefree state of mind has been linked with a hedonic orientation not only at the state level but also at the trait level (Huta & Ryan, 2010; Huta 2013a). The frequent trait-level experience of carefreeness may stem from a heightened ability to let go (i.e., from carefreeness as functioning). Spontaneity and living in the here and now. Associated concepts: focusing on the present (Fordyce, 1983); present hedonistic time perspective (Zimbardo & Boyd, 1999); Running Head: EUDAIMONIA VERSUS HEDONINA 22 spontaneity, healthy impulsivity, direct doing without overthinking, ability to go with the flow of the moment, ability to live in the moment and disengage from ongoing concerns and context, ability to break away to come back with a fresh perspective. A hedonic orientation has been linked to a time perspective that is focused primarily on the present (Pearce et al., 2015). Healthy selfishness. Associated concepts: taking care of oneself (Fordyce, 1983); healthy selfishness, ability to take what one needs and wants, ability to stock up on personal energy, ability to put oneself first when warranted, assertiveness to take one’s share, knowing when to reward oneself, ability to indulge fully without guilt. A hedonic orientation has been related to primarily focusing on the self rather than on others or the surrounding world (Pearce et al., 2015). Efficacy and homeostasis. Associated concepts: efficacy (McGregor & Little, 1998); homeostasis (Vitterso & Søholt, 2011); minimizing unnecessary difficulty, effort, and discomfort. Vittersø and Søholt (2011) asserted that the hedonic feelings of satisfaction and pleasure mark the achievement of homeostasis. Also, McGregor and Little (1998) found that people whose self-identity was focused on fun/enjoyment/pleasure had personal projects that were high in efficacy (i.e., high in control over outcomes, and likelihood of success, and low in difficulty, stress, challenge, and time-pressure). Concreteness. Associated concepts: concreteness, practicality. A hedonic orientation has been positively associated with materialism, but unassociated with abstract thinking (i.e., perceiving activities in terms of their big picture implications rather Running Head: EUDAIMONIA VERSUS HEDONINA 23 than their mechanics) (Huta, 2013a; Pearce et al., 2015). An appreciation of the concrete may translate into practicality and facility in interacting with material things. Eudaimonic Functioning Personal growth. Personal growth can be separated into multiple components: personal actualization (i.e., developing potentials unique to the self), maturity and sophistication (i.e., fulfilling potentials common to all people), learning, competence and excellence, and accomplishment and progress. Concepts associated with personal actualization: self-realization, development of one’s best potentials (Waterman & Schwartz, 2013); identity achievement (Waterman, 2007); meeting one’s personal destiny (Norton, 1976); finding a calling (Wrzesniewski, McCaulay, Rozin, & Schwartz, 1997); finding one’s bliss (Campbell & Moyers, 1988); individuation (Jung, 1933); regularly using one’s signature strengths (Seligman, 2002); coming into one’s own, becoming all that one can be. A eudaimonic orientation has been linked to vocational identity achievement (Hirschi, 2011) and to having a calling (Braaten,Thompson, & Huta., 2014). Concepts associated with maturity and sophistication: personal growth (Ryff, 1989); the self-actualized person (Maslow, 1970); maturity (Allport, 1955); high level of ego development (Bauer, McAdams, & Pals, 2008; Loevinger, 1966); development of a quiet ego that is sophisticated, complex, subtle, deep, and refined (Bauer, 2008); ego integrity (Erikson, 1950); wisdom (Baltes & Staudinger, 2000). A eudaimonic orientation has been linked to the character strength of wisdom (Buschor et al., 2013; Peterson et al., 2007). Running Head: EUDAIMONIA VERSUS HEDONINA 24 Concepts associated with achieving competence and excellence: environmental mastery (Ryff, 1989); excellence in performance (Orlick, 1990); competence, skill, knowledge, understanding, awareness, insight, appreciation, experience, producing quality products. Concepts associated with accomplishment and progress: accomplishment (Seligman, 2011); progress. A eudaimonic orientation has been linked to career success (Proyer et al., 2012). Achieving meaning and purpose. Associated concepts: purpose, understanding, and responsible action (Wong, 2010); meaning in life (Steger, Shin, Shim, & Fitch-Martin, 2013); will to meaning (Frankl, 1988); purpose in life (Ryff, 1989); sense of coherence (Antonovsky, 1993); having a framework for understanding life (Debats, 1998); a meaningful life of serving a higher purpose (Seligman, 2002); spirituality and religiousness (Emmons, 2003); selftranscendence (Garcia-Romeau, 2010). A eudaimonic orientation has been linked to meaning in life, having a framework for understanding life (Huta, 2013a), a meaningful life of serving a higher purpose (Pearce et al., 2015), and religiosity (Buschor et al., 2013; Peterson et al., 2007). Being authentic and integrated. Associated concepts: having autonomous motives (Deci & Ryan, 2000); coherence, congruence, and intrinsic goal content (Sheldon & Kasser, 1995); autonomy and non-conformism (Ryff, 1989); authenticity (Fordyce, 1983); integrity (McGregor & Little, 1998); constitutive goals (Fowers et al., 2010). A eudaimonic orientation has been linked to autonomous motives and intrinsic goal content (Anić & Tončić, 2013; Huta, 2013a). Ethics. Associated concepts: virtue (Fowers, 2008); high level of moral development (Kohlberg, 1984); honesty (Peterson & Seligman, 2004); high standards, doing the right thing. Running Head: EUDAIMONIA VERSUS HEDONINA 25 A eudaimonic orientation has been associated with honesty/integrity and with fairness/justice (Buschor et al., 2013; Peterson et al., 2007). Contribution. Associated concepts: social contribution (Keyes, 2002); generativity, having left something behind, having made a difference (Erikson, 1950; McAdams & de St. Aubin, 1998); symbolic immortality (Huta & Zuroff, 2007); civic engagement (Sherrod, TorneyPurta, & Flanagan, 2010); random acts of kindness (Otake, Shimai, Tanaka-Matsumi, Otsui, & Fredrickson, 2006); altruism (Batson, 1991; Kurzban, Burton-Chellew, & West, 2015); transcending self-interest (Bauer, 2008); responsibility (Frankl, 1988); standing up for what one believes in. A eudaimonic orientation has been associated with a positive impact on close friends and relatives (Huta, 2012; Huta et al., 2012), altruistic behavior, generativity, and pro-environmental behavior (Pearce et al., 2015), and kindness/generosity and leadership (Buschor et al., 2013; Peterson et al., 2007). Self-regulation. Associated concepts: perseverance and grit (Duckworth, Peterson, Matthews, & Kelly, 2007); self-regulation (Maddux, 2009); delay of gratification (Mischel, Shoda, & Rodriguez, 1989); commitment (Waterman & Schwartz, 2013); planning and organizing (Fordyce, 1983). A eudaimonic orientation has been linked to self-regulation (Buschor et al., 2013 Peterson et al., 2007), self-control (Anić & Tončić, 2013), and perseverance (Peterson et al., 2007). In addition, a genetic twin study suggested that a key ability underlying Ryff’s (1989) psychological well-being variables was self-control (Archontaki, Lewis, & Bates, 2013). Abstract thinking and thoughtfulness. Associated concepts: trait mindfulness (Baer, Smith, Hopkins, Krietemeyer, & Tonry, 2006; Ryan, Huta, & Deci, 2008); high-level action Running Head: EUDAIMONIA VERSUS HEDONINA 26 identification and abstract thinking (Vallacher & Wegner, 1987); distant temporal construal (Liberman & Trope, 1998); balanced time perspective with integration of the future and the past, future time perspective (Boniwell & Zimbardo, 2004; Zimbardo & Boyd, 1999); ego development that includes self-awareness, perspective-taking, consideration of long-term consequences, and abstract thinking (Bauer, Schwab, & McAdams, 2011); judgment (Peterson & Seligman, 2004). A eudaimonic orientation has been associated with characteristics reflecting introversion, including introspectiveness, enjoyment of solitude, and a focus on thoughts and ideas (Huta, 2013a), with abstract thinking and future time perspective (Pearce et al., 2015), and with the character strength of judgment (Buschor et al., 2013; Peterson et al., 2007). It is also worth noting that Aristotle considered contemplation to be the highest form of eudaimonia. Quality relationships. Associated concepts: positive relations with others (Ryff, 1989); social well-being (Keyes, 2002); collaborative relationships (Fowers, 2012); relatedness (Fordyce, 1983); belonging (Maslow, 1968). Resilience and constructively facing challenges. Associated concepts: resilience, hardiness, post-traumatic growth, active coping strategies (Bonanno, Galea, Bucciarelli, & Vlahov, 2007; Carver, Scheier, & Weintraub, 1989; Reich, Zautra, & Hall, 2009; Calhoun & Tedeschi, 2004); welcoming challenges (Vittersø, Oelmann, & Wang, 2009); growth mindset (Dweck, 2006); autotelic personality, psychological selection, and seeking challenges (Csikszentmihalyi, 2000; Delle Fave, Massimini, & Bassi, 2011b). A eudaimonic orientation has been linked to enjoying work (Huta, 2013a), being satisfied with one’s work (Proyer, Annen, Eggimann, Schneider, & Ruch, 2012), and love of learning (Buschor et al., 2013). Running Head: EUDAIMONIA VERSUS HEDONINA 27 Resilience is placed with eudaimonia because it often involves effort and grit, but note that resilience has links with hedonia as well. For example, positive emotions can serve as a resource that helps people recover from stressful events (Fredrickson, Tugade, Waugh, & Larkin, 2003; Tugade & Fredickson, 2004). Functioning That is Both Hedonic and Eudaimonic Finally, there are many additional functioning variables that do not clearly lean toward hedonia or eudaimonia, but rather seem related to both concepts or related to well-being in general. Some of the important ones are listed below. Positive attitude. Associated concepts: hope (Snyder, 1995); optimism (Carver & Scheier, 2002); gratitude (Emmons & McCullough, 2003); zest/enthusiasm (Peterson & Seligman, 2004); positive thinking (Fordyce, 1983); self-efficacy (Prochaska & DiClemente, 1984). Conceptually, hedonia and eudaimonia should both involve a positive attitude, as they are both about engaging with the positive things in life. Empirically, hedonic and eudaimonic orientations have both been related to hope/optimism, gratitude, and zest, though gratitude had somewhat stronger links with eudaimonia (Buschor et al., 2013; Peterson et al., 2007). Balance. Associated concepts: balance, harmony (Delle Fave et al., 2011a); work-life balance (Allen, 2013). In addition to rating personal satisfaction with different life domains, people are concerned with the balance between them. In fact, Delle Fave and colleagues (2011a) found that balance was the single most common definition that lay people gave for happiness in general. Full functioning. It is becoming increasingly recognized that optimal functioning involves appropriately mastering the full complement of abilities that humans are endowed with, Running Head: EUDAIMONIA VERSUS HEDONINA 28 including both the positive and negative ones. Ryff and Singer (1998) wrote about appropriately using both one’s positive qualities and one’s negative emotions, e.g., not avoiding the process of mourning if one has lost a loved one. More recently, Kashdan and Biswas-Diener (2014) have published a book called The Upside of Your Dark Side, reviewing research on the benefits of negative emotions and the drawbacks of being excessively focused on positivity. I have tentatively placed this concept in this general section, though other researchers, most notably Ryff and Singer (1998), place full functioning with eudaimonia. I am reluctant to place it under eudaimonia because it would further broaden an already large umbrella term, and I believe it would make the definition of eudaimonia harder to grasp – at present, when eudaimonia is defined primarily as authenticity, meaning, excellence/virtue, and growth, people get a reasonably unitary feel for eudaimonia – it’s about using the best in oneself. Personal aesthetic. I will list here an eclectic collection of concepts, but they all represent functioning on the broadest, whole-person level, which together might be called the individual’s personal aesthetic. These outcomes are subtle, but I hypothesize that they are shaped by media use and I think they deserve empirical attention. The development of a personal aesthetic is a largely unconscious process whereby a person becomes attuned to what he or she considers attractive, enjoyable, funny, tolerable enough to ignore, disturbing enough to resist; develops a sense of what proportions and composition he or she finds desirable; develops a taste for certain things; becomes sensitized to some things and desensitized to others; develops expectations about how exciting, ordinary, forceful, subtle, fun, easy, and challenging things should be; and develops schemas about how people and the world typically work. Following the use of certain media, aspects of a person’s aesthetic may shift either in less healthy directions (e.g., imitating caricatured schemas of interpersonal behavior, or developing unreasonable Running Head: EUDAIMONIA VERSUS HEDONINA 29 expectations about how easy things should be) or in more healthy directions (e.g., becoming sensitized to the damage caused by prejudice, or becoming appreciative of other cultures by developing a taste for their customs). The Difference Between Mechanisms and Markers I would like to close by situating the assessment of well-being variables in a broader context. The distinction between hedonia and eudaimonia not only influences the selection of specific well-being variables to be assessed, but also has implications for the broader issues of research design and interpretation of findings. In the hedonic view, what matters is subjective feelings – positive feelings in the moment, and frequent positive feelings in the long run (Kahneman, 1999). In the eudaimonic view, what matters is the quality of behavior – feelings are seen as markers of a life well lived rather than ends in themselves, and feelings are considered desirable only to the degree that they have meaningful sources and meaningful consequences in real life (Ryan & Huta, 2009; Waterman, 1993; Ryff, 1989). Both views are important. In support of the hedonic perspective, research has shown the many benefits of even brief positive affect, including broadened attention, a desire to build, flexible thinking, efficiency, and relief of anxiety-induced cardiovascular reactions (Fredrickson, 2001; Fredrickson, Mancuso, Branigan, & Tugade, 2000). On the other hand, the central roles of meaning and authenticity in eudaimonia bring an important insight. A concern with meaning translates into considering the context of a wellbeing outcome – its situation with respect to precursors, consequences, and effects beyond the self. A concern with authenticity translates into considering the relevance of a well-being outcome to real life. It is as if peoples’ affective-cognitive processes are “buttons” designed to be activated in certain contexts and during certain real-life processes. These buttons can be hedonic Running Head: EUDAIMONIA VERSUS HEDONINA 30 and/or eudaimonic, triggering feelings such as accomplishment, competence, pleasure, relatedness, and aliveness. However, there is a difference between simply activating a mechanism (the button), and having that mechanism be a true marker of an appropriate process. Simply triggering the mechanism does not ensure that the appropriate process has taken place, with all its complexity, subtlety, effort, vulnerability, and growth. In other words, there can be a short circuit, whereby the person derives the feelings intended to mark and reward a process, but does not undergo the full process. Also, if a well-being mechanism is triggered in a situation that is short-circuited or does not plug back into real life, the mechanism may not produce the full array of possible benefits, and it may not benefit the person in the long run. It is not easy to measure the broader implications of a well-being outcome, to see what it means in the context of a participant’s whole life. Such broad considerations tend to arise later in programs of research, once more proximal outcomes are established. But these considerations imply that it will ultimately be important to study both short-term and long-term outcomes, both experiences and overall functioning, and both outcomes in the lab and outcomes in various reallife domains. References Allen, T. D. (2013). The work-family role interface: A synthesis of the research from industrial and organizational psychology. In N. W. Schmidt, S. Highhouse, and I. B. Irving (Eds.), Handbook of Psychology, Volume 12, Industrial and Organizational Psychology, 2nd Edition (pp. 698-718). Allport, G. (1955). Becoming: Basic considerations for a psychology of personality. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Andersesen, S. M., Chen, S., & Carter, C. (2000). Fundamental human needs: Making social cognition relevant. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 269-275. doi:10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_02 Anić, P. (2014). Hedonic and eudaimonic motives for favourite leisure activities. Primenjena Psihologija, 7, 5-21. Running Head: EUDAIMONIA VERSUS HEDONINA 31 Anić, P., & Tončić, M. (2013). Orientations to happiness, subjective well-being and life goals. Psihologijske teme, 22, 135–153. Annas, J. (1993). The morality of happiness. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Antonovsky, A. (1993). The structure and properties of the sense of coherence scale. Social Science & Medicine, 36, 725–733. doi:10.1016/0277-9536(93)90033-Z Archontaki, D., Lewis, G. J., & Bates, T. C. (2013). Genetic influences on psychological well‐ being: A nationally representative twin study. Journal of Personality, 81(2), 221– 230. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2012.00787.x Aristotle. (2001). Nichomachean ethics. In R. McKeon (Ed.), The basic works of Aristotle (pp. 928–1112). New York, NY: Modern Library. (Original work published 350 B.C.E., Oxford translation 1912–1954) Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., & Tonry, L. (2006). Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment, 13, 27–45. doi:10.1177/1073191105283504 Baltes, P. B., & Staudinger, U. M. (2000). Wisdom: A metaheuristic (pragmatic) to orchestrate mind and virtue toward excellence. American Psychologist, 55, 122–136. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.122 Batson, C. D. (1991). The altruism question: Toward a social-psychological answer. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Bauer, J. J. (2008). How the ego quiets as it grows: Ego development, growth stories, and eudaimonic personality development. In H. A. Wayment & J. J. Bauer (Eds.), Transcending self-interest: Psychological explorations of the quiet ego (pp. 199–210). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Bauer, J. J., McAdams, D. P., & Pals, J. L. (2008). Narrative identity and eudaimonic well-being. Journal of Happiness Studies, 9, 81–104. doi:10.1007/s10902-006-9021-6 Bauer, J. J., Schwab, J. R., & McAdams, D. P. (2011). Self-actualizing: Where ego development finally feels good? The Humanistic Psychologist, 39, 121–136. doi:10.1080/08873267.2011.564978 Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., Aaker, J. & Garbinsky, E. N. (2013). Some key differences between a happy life and a meaningful life. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 8, 505516. doi:10.1080/17439760.2013.830764 Boniwell, I., & Zimbardo, P. G. (2004). Balancing one’s time perspective in pursuit of optimal functioning. In P. A. Linley and S. Joseph (Eds.), Positive psychology in practice (pp. 165-178). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Bonanno, G. A., Galea, S., Bucciarelli, A., & Vlahov, D. (2007). What predicts psychological resilience after disaster? The role of demographics, resources, and life stress. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75, 671-682. Braaten, A., Thompson, A., & Huta, V. (2014, July). Having a calling: Links with meaning in undergraduate studies. Paper presented at The First Congress on The Construction of Personal Meaning, Vancouver, BC. Bradburn N. M. (1969). The structure of psychological well-being. Chicago: Aldine. Buschor, C., Proyer, R. T., & Ruch, W. (2013). Self- and peer-rated character strengths: How do they relate to satisfaction with life and orientations to happiness? Journal of Positive Psychology, 8, 116–127. doi:10.1080/17439760.2012.758305 Bryant, F. B., & Veroff, J. (2007). Savoring: A new model of positive experience. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Running Head: EUDAIMONIA VERSUS HEDONINA 32 Campbell, J., & Moyers, B. D. (1988). The power of myth. New York, NY: Doubleday. Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2002). Optimism. In C. R. Snyder, & S. J. Lopez (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology (pp. 231–243). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F., & Weintraub, J. K. (1989). Assessing coping strategies: A theoretically based approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 267-283. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Beyond boredom and anxiety: experiencing flow in work and play 25th anniversary edition. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Dambrun, M., & Ricard, M. (2011). Self-centeredness and selflessness: A theory of self-based psychological functioning and its consequences for happiness. Review of General Psychology, 15, 138-157. doi:10.1037/a0023059 Debats, D. L. (1998). Measurement of personal meaning: The psychometric properties of the life regard index. In P.T.P. Wong (Ed.), The human quest for meaning: A handbook of psychological research and clinical applications (pp. 237–260). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The ‘‘what’’ and ‘‘why’’ of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 227–268. doi:10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01 Delle Fave, A., Brdar, I., Freire, T., Vella-Brodrick, D., & Wissing, M. P. (2011a). The eudaimonic and hedonic components of happiness: Qualitative and quantitative findings. Social Indicators Research, 100, 185–207. doi:10.1007/s11205-010-9632-5 Delle Fave, A., & Massimini, F. (1988). Modernization and the changing context of flow in work and leisure. In M. Csikszentmihalyi & I. Csikszentmihalyi (Eds.), Optimal experience: Psychological studies of flow in consciousness (pp. 193–213). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Delle Fave, A., Massimini, F., & Bassi, M. (2011b). Psychological selection and optimal experience across cultures: Social empowerment through personal growth. New York, NY: Springer. Diener, E., & Emmons, R. A. (1984). The independence of positive and negative affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 1105–1117. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.47.5.1105 Diener, E., Emmons, R.A., Larsen, R.J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 71–75. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13 Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., & Smith, H. L. (1999). Subjective well-being: Three decades of progress. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 276–302. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.125.2.276 Diener, E., Wirtz, D., Tov, W., Kim-Prieto, C., Choi. D., Oishi, S., & Biswas-Diener, R. (2009). New Well-Being Measures: Short Scales to Assess Flourishing and Positive and Negative Feelings. Social Indicators Research, 39, 247-266. doi:10.1007/s11205-009-9493-y Duckworth, A. L., Peterson, C., Matthews, M. D., & Kelly, D. R. (2007). Grit: Perseverance and passion for long-term goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(6), 1087– 1101. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.92.6.1087 Dweck, C. S. (2006). Mindset: The new psychology of success. New York, NY: Random House. Emmons, R. A. (2003). The psychology of ultimate concerns: Motivation and spirituality in personality. New York, NY: Guilford. Running Head: EUDAIMONIA VERSUS HEDONINA 33 Emmons, R. A., & McCullough, M. E. (2003). Counting blessings versus burdens: An experimental investigation of gratitude and subjective wellbeing in daily life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(2), 377–389. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.2.377 Erikson, E. H. (1950). Childhood and Society. New York, NY: Norton. Fordyce, M. W. (1983). A program to increase happiness: Further studies. Journal of Counselling Psychology, 30, 483–498. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.30.4.483 Fowers, B. J. (2008). From continence to virtue: Recovering goodness, character unity, and character types for positive psychology. Theory and Psychology, 18, 629–653. doi:10.1177/0959354308093399 Fowers, B. J. (2012). An Aristotelian framework for the human good. Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology, 32, 10–23. doi:10.1037/a0025820 Fowers, B. J., Molica, C. O., & Procacci, E. N. (2010). Constitutive and instrumental goal orientations and their relations with eudaimonic and hedonic well-being. Journal of Positive Psychology, 5, 139–153. doi:10.1080/17439761003630045 Frankl, V. E. (1988). The will to meaning: Foundations and applications of logotherapy. New York: Meridian. Fredrickson, B. (2013). Love 2.0: How our supreme emotion affects everything we feel, think, do, and become. Hudson Street Press. Fredrickson, B.L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden-and build theory of positive emotions. American Psychologist, 56, 218-226. Fredrickson, B. L., Mancuso, R. A., Branigan, C., Tugade, M. (2000). The undoing effect of positive emotions. Motivation and Emotion, 24, 237-258. Fredrickson, B. L., Tugade, M. M., Waugh, C. E., & Larkin, G. R. (2003). What good are positive emotions in crises?: A prospective study of resilience and emotions following the terrorist attacks on the United States in September 11, 2001. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 365-376. Freud, S. (1920) A general introduction to psychoanalysis. New York, NY: Boni and Liveright. Garcia-Romeu, A. (2010). Self-transcendence as a measurable transpersonal construct. The Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 42, 26–47. Gagné, M. (2003). The role of autonomy support and autonomy orientation in prosocial behavior engagement. Motivation and Emotion, 27, 199-223. doi:10.1023/A:1025007614869 Haidt, J. (2000). The positive emotion of elevation. Prevention and Treatment, 3. doi:10.1037/1522-3736.3.1.33c Haybron, D. (2008). Happiness, the self and human flourishing. Utilitas, 20, 21–49. doi:10.1017/S0953820807002889 Heathwood, C. (2006). Desire-satisfactionism and hedonism. Philosophical Studies, 128, 539– 563. doi:10.1007/s11098-004-7817-y Henderson, L. W., Knight, T., & Richardson, B. (2013). An exploration of the well-being benefits of hedonic and eudaimonic behaviour. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 8, 322-336. doi:10.1080/17439760.2013.803596 Hirschi, A. (2011). Effects of orientations to happiness on vocational identity achievement. Career Development Quarterly, 59, 367–378. doi:10.1002/j.2161-0045.2011.tb00075.x Huta, V. (2012). Linking peoples’ pursuit of eudaimonia and hedonia with characteristics of their parents: Parenting styles, verbally endorsed values, and role modeling. Journal of Happiness Studies, 13, 47–61. doi:10.1007/s10902-011-9249-7 Running Head: EUDAIMONIA VERSUS HEDONINA 34 Huta, V. (2013a). Pursuing eudaimonia versus hedonia: Distinctions, similarities, and relationships. In A. Waterman (Ed.), The best within us: Positive psychology perspectives on eudaimonic functioning (pp. 139–158).Washington, DC: APA Books. Huta, V. (2013b). Eudaimonia. In S. David, I. Boniwell, & A. C. Ayers (Eds.), Oxford handbook of happiness (pp. 201–213). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. Huta, V. (2014, July). How is meaning distinct from subjective well-being? Data on two key categories of well-being. Invited address at the First Congress on the Construction of Personal Meaning, Vancouver, B.C. Huta, V. (2015a). The complementary roles of eudaimonia and hedonia and how they can be pursued in practice. In S. Joseph (Ed.), Positive psychology in practice: promoting human flourishing in work, health, education, and everyday life (2nd ed., pp. 159-182) Wiley. Huta, V. (2015b, June). Eudaimonic and hedonic orientations (motives): Recent work on links with seeing beyond the self, the present, and the concrete. In V. Huta (Chair), Advances in research on eudaimonia. Symposium conducted at the Fourth World Congress on Positive Psychology, Orlando, Florida. Huta, V. Meaning as a Subjective Experience. (2015c). Journal of Constructivist Psychology: Meaning Summit Special Issue. Accepted. Huta,V., Pelletier, L., Baxter, D., & Thompson, A. (2012). How eudaimonic and hedonic motives relate to the well-being of close others. Journal of Positive Psychology, 7, 399– 404. doi:10.1080/17439760.2012.705318 Huta, V., & Ryan, R. M. (2010). Pursuing pleasure or virtue: The differential and overlapping well-being benefits of hedonic and eudaimonic motives. Journal of Happiness Studies, 11, 735–762. doi:10.1007/s10902-009-9171-4 Huta, V., & Waterman A. S. (2014). Eudaimonia and its distinction from hedonia: Developing a classification and terminology for understanding conceptual and operational definitions. Journal of Happiness Studies, 15, 1425-1456. doi:10.1007/s10902-013-9485-0 Huta, V., & Zuroff, D. C. (2007). Examining mediators of the link between generativity and well-being. Journal of Adult Development, 14, 47–52. doi:10.1007/s10804-007-9030-7 Jung, C. G. (1933). Modern man in search of a soul. New York, NY: Harcourt, Brace, & World. Kahneman, D. (1999). Objective happiness. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, and N. Schwartz (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 3-25). New York: Russell Sage. Kashdan, T., & Biswas-Diener, R. (2014). The upside of your dark side: Why being your whole self – not just your “good” self – drives success and fulfillment. New York, NY: Hudson Street Press. Kasser, T. (2009). Psychological need satisfaction, personal well-being, and ecological sustainability. Ecopsychology, 1, 175-180. doi:10.1089/eco.2009.0025 Keltner, D., & Haidt, J. (2003). Approaching awe, a moral, spiritual, and aesthetic emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 17, 297–314. doi:10.1080/02699930302297 Kern, M. L., & Butler, J. (June, 2013). The PERMA-Profiler: A brief multidimensional measure of flourishing. Poster presented at the Third World Congress on Positive Psychology, Los Angeles, California. Keyes, C. L. M. (2002). The mental health continuum: From languishing to flourishing in life. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 43, 207–222. doi:10.2307/3090197 Keyes, C. L. M, & Annas, J. (2009). Feeling good and functioning well: Distinctive concepts in ancient philosophy and contemporary science. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 4, 197–201. doi:10.1080/17439760902844228 Running Head: EUDAIMONIA VERSUS HEDONINA 35 King, L. A. & Napa, C. K. (1998). What makes a life good? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 156–165. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.75.1.156 Kohlberg, L. (1984). The psychology of moral development: The nature and validity of moral stages. San Francisco, CA: Harper & Row. Konečni, V. J. (2005). The aesthetic trinity: Awe, being moved, thrills. Bulletin of Psychology and the Arts, 5, 27–44. Kurzban, R. R., Burton-Chellew, M. N.,& West, S. A. (2015). The evolution of altruism in humans. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 575-99. Kraut, R. (2007). What is good and why: The ethics of well-being. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press. Leary, M. R., Tipsord, J. M., & Tate, E. M. (2008). Allo-inclusive identity: Incorporating the social and natural worlds into one's sense of self. In H. A. Wayment & J. J. Bauer (Eds.), Transcending self-interest: Psychological exploration of the quiet ego (pp. 137-147). Washington, DC: APA. Liberman, N., & Trope, Y. (1998). The role of feasibility and desirability considerations in near and distant future decisions: A test of temporal construal theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 5-18. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.75.1.5 Loevinger, J. (1976). Ego development. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. Lyubomirsky, S. & Lepper, H. (1999). A measure of subjective happiness: Preliminary reliability and construct validation. Social Indicators Research, 46, 137–155. doi:10.1023/A:1006824100041 Mack, D. E., Gunnell, K. E., Wilson, P. M., Gilchrist, J. D., Kowalski, K. C., Crocker, P. R. E., . . . Adachi, J. D. (2011). Physical activity in individuals living with osteopenia: Association with psychological need satisfaction and motives for well-being. The Shield Research Journal of Physical Education and Sports Sciences, 6, 26-41. Maddux, J. E. (2009). Self-regulation. In S. Lopez (Ed.), The encyclopedia of positive psychology (pp. 874–880). Chichester, England: Blackwell. Maslow, A. H. (1964). Religion, values, and peak-experiences. Columbus: Ohio State University Press. Maslow, A. (1968). Toward a psychology of being. New York, NY: Van Norstrand. Maslow, A. (1970). Motivation and personality (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Harper and Row. McAdams, D. P., & de St. Aubin, E. (1998). How and why we care for the next generation. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. McGregor, I., & Little, B. R. (1998). Personal projects, happiness, and meaning: On doing well and being yourself. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 494–512. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.2.494 Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Rodriguez, M. I. (1989). Delay of gratification in children. Science, 244, 933–938. doi:10.1126/science.2658056 Norton, D. L. (1976). Personal destinies: A philosophy of ethical individualism. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Onfray, M. (2013). Manifeste Hédoniste. France: Editions 84. Orlick, T. (1990). In pursuit of excellence: How to win in sport and life through mental training. (2nd ed.). Champaign, IL: Leisure Press. Otake, K., Shimai, S., Tanaka-Matsumi, J., Otsui, K., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2006). Happy people become happier through kindness: A counting kindnesses intervention. Journal of Happiness Studies, 7(3), 361–375. doi:10.1007/s10902-005-3650-z Running Head: EUDAIMONIA VERSUS HEDONINA 36 Pearce, K., Huta, V., & Voloaca, M. (2015). Seeing beyond the self, the present, and the concrete: A comparison of eudaimonic and hedonic approaches to life. Journal of Positive Psychology. Manuscript in preparation. Peterson, C., Park, N., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2005). Orientations to happiness and life satisfaction: The full life versus the empty life. Journal of Happiness Studies, 6, 25–41. doi:10.1007/s10902-004-1278-z Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2004). Character strengths and virtues: A handbook and classification. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Peterson, C., Ruch, W., Beermann, U., Park, N., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2007). Strengths of character, orientations to happiness, and life satisfaction. Journal of Positive Psychology, 2, 1–8. doi:10.1080/17439760701228938 Piedmont, R. L. (1999). Does spirituality represent the sixth factor of personality? Spiritual transcendence and the five-factor model. Journal of Personality, 67, 985–1013. doi:10.1111/1467-6494.00080 Prochaska, J. O., & DiClemente, C. C. (1984). The transtheoretical approach: Towards a systematic eclectic framework. Homewood, IL: Dow Jones Irwin. Proyer, R.T., Annen, H., Eggimann, N., Schneider, A., & Ruch, W. (2012). Assessing the “good life“ in a military context: How does life and work-satisfaction relate to orientations to happiness and career-success among swiss professional officers? Social Indicators Research, 106, 577-590. doi:10.1007/s11205-011-9823-8 Reich, J. W., Zautra, A. J., & Hall, J. S. (Eds.) Handbook of adult resilience. New York: Guilford Press. Ruch, W., Proyer, R. T., & Weber, M. (2010). Humor as a character strength among the elderly: Empirical findings on age-related changes and its contribution to satisfaction with life. Zeitschrift fur Gerontologie und Geriatrie, 43, 13–18. doi:10.1007/s00391-009-0090-0 Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2001). On happiness and human potentials: A review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 141–166. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.141 Ryan, R. M., & Frederick, C. (1997). On energy, personality, and health: Subjective vitality as a dynamic reflection of well‐ being. Journal of Personality, 65, 529–565. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1997.tb00326.x Ryan, R. M., & Huta, V. (2009). Wellness as healthy functioning or wellness as happiness: The importance of eudaimonic thinking. Journal of Positive Psychology, 4, 202-204. Ryan, R. M., Huta, V., & Deci, E. L. (2008). Living well: A self-determination theory perspective on eudaimonia. Journal of Happiness Studies, 9, 139–170. doi:10.1007/s10902-006-9023-4 Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological wellbeing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 1069–1081. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.57.6.1069 Ryff, C. D., & Singer, B. H. (2008). Know thyself and become what you are: A eudaimonic approach to psychological well-being. Journal of Happiness Studies, 9, 13–39. doi:10.1007/s10902-006-9019-0 Ryff, C. D., & Singer, B. H. (1998). The role of purpose in life and personal growth in positive human health. In P.S. Fry & P.T.P. Wong (Eds.), The human quest for meaning (pp. 213– 236). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Running Head: EUDAIMONIA VERSUS HEDONINA 37 Schlegel, R. J., Hicks, J. A., King, L. A., & Arndt, J. (2011). Feeling like you know who you are: Perceived true self-knowledge and meaning in life. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37, 745–756. doi:10.1177/0146167211400424 Schueller, S. M., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2010). Differential pursuit of pleasure, engagement, and meaning: Relationships to subjective and objective well-being. Journal of Positive Psychology, 5, 253-263. doi:10.1080/17439761003794130 Seligman, M. E. P. (2002). Authentic happiness. New York, NY: Free Press. Seligman, M. E. P. (2011). Flourish: A visionary new understanding of happiness and wellbeing. New York, NY: Free Press. Sheldon, K. (2013). Individual daimon, universal needs, and subjective well-being: Happiness as the natural consequence of a life well lived. In A. S. Waterman (Ed.), The best within us: Positive psychology perspectives on eudaimonia (pp. 119–138). Washington, DC: APA. Sheldon, K. M., & Kasser, T. (1995). Coherence and congruence: Two aspects of personality integration. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3, 531–543. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.68.3.531 Sherrod, L. R., Torney-Purta, J., & Flanagan, C. A., (2010). Handbook of Research on Civic Engagement in Youth. Hanboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons Inc. Snyder, C. R. (1995). Conceptualizing, measuring and nurturing hope. Journal of Counselling and Development, 73, 355–360. doi:10.1002/j.1556-6676.1995.tb01764.x Steger, M. F. (2012). Experience meaning in life: Optimal functioning at the nexus of spirituality, psychopathology, and well-being. In P. T. P. Wong (Eds.), The Human quest for meaning (2nd Ed., pp. 165-184). New York, NY: Routledge. Steger, M. F., Frazier, P., Oishi, S., & Kaler, M. (2006). The Meaning in Life Questionnaire: Assessing the presence of and search for meaning in life. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 53, 80-93.doi:10.1037/0022-0167.53.1.80 Steger, M. F., Kashdan, T. B., & Oishi, S. (2008). Being good by doing good: Daily eudaimonic activity and well-being. Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 22–42. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2007.03.004 Steger, M. F., & Shin, J. Y. (2012). Happiness and meaning in a technological age: A psychological approach. In P. Brey, A. Briggle, & E. Spence (Eds.), The good life in a technological age (pp. 92–108). New York: Routledge. Steger, M. F., Shin, J.-Y., Shim, Y., & Fitch-Martin, A. (2013). Is meaning in life a flagship indicator of well-being? In A. Waterman (Ed.), Eudaimonia (pp. 159–182). Washington, DC: APA Press. Sumner, L. (1996). Welfare, happiness and ethics. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Thrash, T. M., & Elliot, A. J. (2003). Inspiration as a psychological construct. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 871–889. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.871 Tiberius, V. (2013). Recipies of a good life: Eudaimonism and the contribution of philosophy. In A. S. Waterman (Ed.), The best within us: Positive psychology perspectives on eudaimonia (pp. 19–38). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2007). The psychological structure of pride: A tale of two facets. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 506–525. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.92.3.506 Tugade, M. M., Fredrickson, B. L. (2004). Resilient individuals use positive emotions to bounce back from negative emotional experiences. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 86, 320-333. Running Head: EUDAIMONIA VERSUS HEDONINA 38 Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. (1987). What do people think they’re doing? Action identification and human behavior. Psychological Review, 94, 3-15. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.94.1.3 Vella-Brodrick, D. A., Park, N.,& Peterson, C. (2009). Three ways to be happy: Pleasure, engagement, and meaning-findings from Australian and US samples. Social Indicators Research, 90, 165–179. doi:10.1007/s11205-008-9251-6 Vittersø, J. (2004). Subjective well-being versus self-actualization: Using the flow-simplex to promote a conceptual, clarification of subjective quality of life. Social Indicators Research, 65, 299–331. doi:10.1023/B:SOCI.0000003910.26194.ef Vittersø, J., Dyrdal, G.M., & Røysamb, E. (June, 2005). Utilities and capabilities: A psychological account of the two concepts and their relations to the idea of a good life. Paper presented at Workshop on Capabilities and Happiness, Bicocca, Italy. Vittersø, J., Oelmann, H. I., & Wang, A. L. (2009). Life satisfaction is not a balanced estimator of the good life: Evidence from reaction-time measures and self-reported emotions. Journal of Happiness Studies, 10, 1–17. doi:10.1007/s10902-007-9058-1 Vittersø, J., & Søholt, Y. (2011). Life satisfaction goes with pleasure and personal growth goes with interest: Further arguments for separating hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Journal of Positive Psychology, 6, 326–335. doi:10.1080/17439760.2011.584548 Waterman, A. S. (1993). Two conceptions of happiness: Contrasts of personal expressiveness (eudaimonia) and hedonic enjoyment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 678–691. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.64.4.678 Waterman, A. S. (2007). Doing well: The relationship of identity status to three conceptions of well-being. Identity: An International Journal of Theory and Research, 7, 289–307. doi:10.1080/15283480701600769 Waterman, A. S., & Schwartz, S. J. (2013). Eudaimonic identity theory. In A. S. Waterman (Ed.), The best within us: Positive psychology perspectives on eudaimonia (pp. 99–118). Washington, DC: APA. Waterman, A. S., Schwartz, S. J., & Conti, R. (2008). The implications of two conceptions of happiness (hedonic enjoyment and eudaimonia) for the understanding of intrinsic motivation. Journal of Happiness Studies, 9, 41–79. doi:10.1007/s10902-006-9020-7 Waterman, A. S., Schwartz, S. J., Zamboanga, B. L., Ravert, R. D., Williams, M. K., Agocha, V. B. Brent, D. M. (2010). The questionnaire for eudaimonic well-being: Psychometric properties, demographic comparisons, and evidence of validity. Journal of Positive Psychology, 5, 41–61. doi:10.1080/17439760903435208 Wong, P. T. P. (2010). Meaning therapy: An integrative and positive existential psychotherapy. Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy, 40, 85-99. doi:10.1007/s10879-009-9132-6 Wrzesniewski, A., McCauley, C. R., Rozin, P., & Schwartz, B. (1997). Jobs, careers, and callings: People’s relations to their work. Journal of Research in Personality, 31, 21– 33. doi:10.1006/jrpe.1997.2162 Zimbardo, P. G., & Boyd, J. N. (1999). Putting time in perspective: A valid, reliable individualdifferences metric. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 1271-1288. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.6.1271 Running Head: EUDAIMONIA VERSUS HEDONINA 39 Veronika Huta (Ph.D., McGill University, 2005) is Associate Professor of Psychology at the University of Ottawa. Her research focuses on well-being, the distinction between pursuing hedonia (pleasure) and eudaimonia (excellence), and the experiences of meaning and elevation. Her teaching focuses on statistics. She has received a top-poster award at an international conference, and is among the top rated instructors in her department. She is on the editorial board of three well-being journals, and is co-founder of the Canadian Positive Psychology Association.