Special questions on lucid dreaming: What? Who? How? When? Why? Lucid dreams are those dreams in which the person realizes the fact that she or he is asleep (Van Eeden, 1913). Dreams perception and interpretation has always been a fascinating topic for scientific researches and studies. However, lucid dreams appear to be even more alluring since the beginning of the twentieth century. After conducting academic research in this paper I have tried to gather all the known information that can appear to be answers on common arising questions on lucidity and its applications; such as the mechanism that takes place in one’s brain or ways to provoke having lucid dreams. Lucidity is a scientific area that has to undergo a lot of scientific research, however, lucid dreaming has quite high frequency among population; therefore this paper may be a helpful tool in using its potential. As “being awake in your sleep” was proved to be useful in working on nightmares, self-improvement, and skills practice lucidity can be helpful in one’s lifetime improvement. Victoria Lyubykh SCI 150 1 Have you ever experienced free fall from the tower of couple of a few kilometers high? Maybe some vélociraptors were chasing you in the middle of nowhere? Unfortunately, you woke up right after the most exciting moment while being in the dinosaur’s jaws or when just about landing on the ground. The answer is most probably positive, however, you might not remember it. In most dreams we are not aware of the fact that we are dreaming, a significant exception takes place while having lucid dreams (Van Eeden, 1913) in which the dreamer attains a clear perception that he or she is dreaming while sleeping. In this paper I will try to answer the most common arising questions on lucid dreams. When the term was first introduced? Back in 1913, Frederik Van Eeden was trying to classify the kinds of dreams he was experiencing for the period of sixteen years of his lifetime. He was the first to coin the term lucid dreaming. So what makes lucid dreams different from other types? When having a lucid dream a person can experience a state of perfect awareness, can control his or her attention, and can act freely (Van Eeden, 1913). Van Eeden didn’t deny that physical activities and functions enter to a condition of dissociation while sleeping but was presuming that it was not dissociation, but, on the contrary, reintegration. He believed that reintegration is the key feature of a dream, but it was a different type of reintegration where the process of redeeming was happening in a psychical, nonspatial mode of existence. This process can interact with the full recollection of daily life, reflection and voluntary action on this reflection per se (Van Eeden, 1913). Frederik focuses not only on attempts to influence the dream flow but also on the sensations and feelings. “Yet the sleep, as I am able confidently to state, is undisturbed, deep and refreshing” (Van Eeden, 1913). The first written record of lucid dreaming by Frederik Van Eeden goes back to 1897. Although many of his findings contradict with those of current researches, his paper remains a classic. 2 Who can have those lucid dreams? According to Thomas Snyder and Jayne Gackenbach who conducted a survey among university students claim that a majority have reported experiencing at least one lucid dream during their life, moreover, approximately 20% said they experienced lucid dreaming with relative frequency (Snyder, Gackenback, 1988). However, Ursula Voss in her article in Scientific American claims that about eight out of 10 people have had a lucid dream, when they were aware of the fact they were dreaming, at least one in a lifetime (Voss, 2011). In what stages of sleep do lucid dreams occur? Currently, there are five known stages of sleep. Lucid dreaming happens at REM (rapid eye movement) stage. Most frequently, lucid dreams start from REM sleep (so called “DreamInitiated Lucid Dream” – DILD), however sometimes they can also be initiated from the waking state (“Wake-Initiated Lucid Dream” – WILD) (LaBerge, Levitan, & Dement, 1986). Consequently, following the stages from the first to fourth the brain activity gradually slows down. When descended into the fourth stage of sleep and stayed there for a short period of time (about twenty-five minutes), one goes back to stage three and then two, and only here REM sleep stage occurs. Overall, it takes approximately hour and a half to go through stage one to four and back again. It takes around five or ten minutes for each stage to pass. When another half an hour passes another REM stage occurs. As you sleep for four-five hours you don’t go lower than stage two. Vivid dreaming happens in REM stage, therefore, the more you sleep, ipso facto the more time you spend at REM stage, the more likely you may have a lucid dream (Lars Rune Foleide, 1999). 3 What happens in my brain when I am having a lucid dream? Lucid dreaming is characterized by an elevated amount of CNS (central nervous activity) activity than normal, non-lucid REM sleep. Usually, the brain activity, some parts of it, is switched off during REM sleep. The aminergous neurons (neurons that use monoamines as a neurotransmitter; the two specific aminergic neurotransmitters are serotonin and norepinephrine) are at smallest amounts in REM sleep. The neurotransmitters which are in charge of activating aminergic neurons in the brainstem are conserved for the following day. These neurotransmitters are responsible for the attention and learning tasks. The absence, rest, or inhibition of their function perfectly fits the pattern one finds in non-lucid dreams of a non-critical consciousness, which blindly accepts bizarre events, and loss of contextual and referencing faculties. When the aminergic neurons are inhibited in REM sleep, the muscle tonus (residual muscle tension) is also inhibited or paralyzed. However, while having lucid dreams the aminergic neurons, the critical faculty, is turned on, while body muscles are still paralyzed. Only here, for some unknown cause, the “self-referencing” faculty suddenly becomes awake, do we find ourselves having a lucid dream (Lorgen, 1998). How lucid dreams can be applied while being awake? Can I control my dreams? “During a lucid dream, it's possible to act rather than simply react” (cited in Miller, 2012). However, Melanie Schädlich and Daniel Erlacher conducted a study with the goal of investigation some applications of lucid dreaming. The survey included 301 participants (luciddreamers) who filled an online questionnaire. The most frequent application was having fun (81.4%), solving problems (29.9%), getting creative ideas or insights (27.6%) and practicing skills (21.3%). Women turned out to be using lucid dreams more often than men for problem 4 solving and working on nightmares. The results have shown that lucid dreaming can be beneficial at life improving in different ways; as many people have time to improve their skills even while being asleep (Schädlich, Erlacher, 2012). Until recently, the majority of experts didn’t take lucid dreaming seriously – they considered it a way of having fun by acting freely using wishful thinking to talk to dead people and meeting pop stars. Nevertheless, lucid dreaming was tested in being useful tool in lowering nightmare frequency. Psychologists use IRT or imaginary rehearsal therapy method to help trauma victims to prevent nightmare occurrence during the sleep. Shortly, the dreamers can turn the nightmare into something that makes them feel better: instead of being chased by enormous monster they imagine a cute and fluffy puppy. As a result, VA San Diego Healthcare System patients who were participating in the course of IRT therapy showed 33% fewer nightmares after five weeks (Miller, 2012). Another recent research in Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics in October 2006 revealed that people who can increase the frequency of having lucid dreams show lower level of nightmares afterwards. Ursula Voss argues that results of this survey are not clear enough but assumes that possibly being aware of a bad dream is about to occur patients distance themselves on emotional level from the forthcoming fear. Some people can even prevent these dreams to take place by becoming versed at lucid dreaming (Voss, 2011). Not only lucidity can be a useful tool in the research of sleep state but also “offers considerable potential for a variety of practical applications, which include aiding personal- development, enhancing self-confidence, overcoming nightmares, improving mental (and perhaps, physical) health, facilitating creative problem solving, and more” (LaBerge, 1993). 5 Are there any drugs to facilitate lucid dreaming frequency? Thomas Yuschak has conducted a study to explore the effectiveness of using drugs and supplements to induce the frequency of lucid dreaming. Yuschak was using cholinergic (related to the neurotransmitter acetylcholine) substances :a) galantamine; b)choline; and sleep suppressing substances: a) caffeine; b) desmopressin (a synthetic replacement for vasopressin, the hormone that reduces urine production) and c) sulbutiamine (a synthetic derivative of thiamine (vitamin B1)). During eleven days a forty year old male was sleeping for 3.5-4.5 hours before consuming either a deep sleep suppressing substance (DSS), a cholinergic stimulating substance (CS) or both. The subject has experienced a lucid dream in five out of five trials when a combination of DSS and CS was consumed. More information on each of the substances used follows: 1. Galantamine (G) - is used for the treatment of mild to moderate Alzheimer's disease and various other memory impairments. The dosage of galantamine used for all tests supporting this paper was 8mg. 2. Huperzine A (Hup) - is a naturally occurring alkaloid found in the extracts of the firmoss Huperzia serrata species. The dosage of huperzine used for all tests supporting this paper was 200mcg. 3. Nicotine (N) – is available in different forms; the main application is smoking cessation aid. The dosage of nicotine used for all tests supporting the test was 7mg delivered via a transdermal nicotine patch. 6 4. Bupropion (Bu) – is a prescription drug that is classified as an antidepressant and smoking cessation aid as well. The dosage used was 75mg. 5. Amino acid blend (AAB) - a mix of three complementary amino acids was used: Laspartic acid, L-glutamine, and L-theanine. The dosages used for all tests incorporating this Amino Acid blend were 2000mg L-aspartic acid, 4000mg L-glutamine, 300mg Ltheanine. 6. Propranolol (Pr) –is a prescription drug used for treatment of hypertension, migraines, and stage fright. The dosage of propranolol used for all tests supporting the test was 40mg. To observe the results of the experiment refer to the Figures 1, 2 and 3: Figure 1: BS1 refers to sleep without consuming either DSS or CS; the figure represents the 7 failure of CS consummation to produce lucid dreams. © Substances that Facilitate Lucid Dreaming – A Case Study, Thomas Yuschak. Figure 2: Each of the DSS nights showed a strong tendency to suppress the deeper sleep stages compared to the baseline night (BN1) as shown by relative power between the delta bands and dominate alpha bands. © Substances that Facilitate Lucid Dreaming – A Case Study, Thomas Yuschak. 8 Figure 3: All five of the LDS nights (three shown above) produced Wake Initiated Lucid Dreams (WILDs). It is hypothesized that the success of this approach is due to the combination of a cholinergic stimulating substance with a deep sleep suppressing substance which allowed the subject to move from the waking state directly into a very light sleep with increased acetylcholine levels. © Substances that Facilitate Lucid Dreaming – A Case Study, Thomas Yuschak. As a result, most individual substances turned to be practical in initiating lucid dreams. Yuschak concluded from these results that the ideal approach is to use REM promoting substance together with a deep sleep suppressing substance (Yuschak, Thomas). 9 How can I learn more about lucidity? Since Van Eeden firstly introduced the term lucidity, many research institutes and individuals were particularly examining and exploring the mysteries of being “awake in your sleep”. In order to start your personal education on lucidity, its applications and usefulness, you can go through the book called “A course in lucid dreaming” By Stephen LaBerge and Lynne Levitan (Lucidity Institute, 1995). This book will be in your particular interest if you want to start practicing lucid dreaming at home. Another LaBerge’s book such as: “Exploring the world of lucid dreaming” (Ballantine, 1990) or “Lucid Dreaming” (Ballantine, 1985) can be helpful in self-education on lucid dreaming. Moreover, if you happen to be a lucid dream master, there is a chance for you to take part in a research\study conducted by Lucidity Institute (www.lucidity.com). Lucid dreaming is a mystical feature of a human’s brain. Following the research done, there is no way to trace the very first occurrence of lucid dreaming; however, the fact itself that is quite frequent supposes that lucidity is predestinated to be present nowadays. We still have much to learn on lucid dreaming. The area of research is quite wide: multiple researches have to be conducted in order to find out how to induce lucidity more safely or what techniques should be used in order to control dreaming and staying conscious. Lucidity has a big potential to become a panacea: as it has multiple advantages not only in preventing nightmares, self-development and stress release but also can help people with incurable diseases or inabilities to become healthy in their dreams. Moreover, lucidity is a skill one can attain by learning and practice; therefore, I suppose there is no limit for lucidity’s helpfulness in medicine, entertainment and research. 10 Works Cited Eeden, Frederik V. "A Study of Dreams." Proceedings of the Society for Psychical Research. Accessed April 3, 2013. www.lucidity.com. LaBerge, Stephen and others. “Lucid dreaming: Physiological correlates of consciousness during REM sleep.” Perceptual and Motor Skills, (1981). Accessed April 2, 2013. www.amsciepub.com. LaBerge, Stephen. “Lucidity Research, Past and Future.” Lucidity Institute: Nightlight. 5 (1993). Accessed March 28, 2013. www.psywww.com. LaBerge, Stephen et al. “Lucid Dreaming: Psychological correlates of consciousness during REM sleep”. Journal of Mind and Behavior, 7 (1986): 251-258, 1986. Accessed April 1, 2013. www.philpapers.org. Lorgen, Eve F. “Lucid Dreaming.” A psychophysiological Approach to an Ancient Spiritual Practice, (1998). Accessed March 29, 2013. www.bibliotecapleyades.net. Miller, Kaitlin. “Is it possible to control our dreams?” Popular Science, 1 (2012): 76. Accessed April 7 2013. web.ebscohost.com. Schädlich, Melanie and Daniel Erlacher. “Applications of lucid dreams: an online study.” International Journal of Dream Research, No 2. October 2012. www.archiv.ub.uniheidelberg.de. Snyder, Thomas J. and Jayne Gackenbach. “Individual Differences associated with Lucid Dreaming.” Conscious Mind, Sleeping Brain. 1988. Accessed April 6, 2013. www.link.springer.com. The Lucidity Institute. “Lar’s Lucid Dreaming FAQ.” February 1999. www.faqs.org. 11 Voss, Ursulla. "Unlocking the Lucid Dream." Scientific American Mind, 22.5 (2011): 33-35. Web. 7 Apr. 2013. Academic Search Complete (69655073). Yuschak, Thomas. “Substances that facilitate lucid dreaming – cade study.” Accessed April 7, 2013. www.dreamslucid.files.wordpress.com. 12