Iara Cury Elizabeth Ewart, Week #4 9/02/2011 Kinship, Relatedness and Solidarity The debate about the definition and applicability of the concept of kinship to non-Western cultures has given anthropologists much to think about. Yet interestingly enough, it is a commonly held view that family ties remain strong in developing countries whereas they have lost ground in developed countries. One factor wholly relevant to this perception is the trend towards smaller families that has established itself in the United States, Europe and certain Asian countries. Due to overpopulation concerns, demographers forecast the spread of this downsizing of reproduction across the world. Another issue to consider is the effect of extensive regional migration and transnational mobility, possible threats to the integrity and strength of kinship ties as traditionally conceived. We can find many analytical reasons to question the idea that “blood is thicker than water”, some of which will be presented in this essay. In support of that, we have motivating circumstances to explore notions of relatedness, trust and intimacy much beyond, and without implicit reference to, the domain of genealogical groups. Nevertheless, under the guise of kinship anthropologists have explored the connection between biology and social relations, nature and nurture, since the very beginning of the discipline. It is essential to understand this historical progression of ideas before approaching a deeper analysis of kinship theories. Anthropologists began studying non-Western peoples and cultures by focusing on what they perceived as bounded, isolated, and simpler societies than their own. One of the factors taken to be evidence as to the posited simplicity of these societies was the lack of the state, which in Western nations embodied the sociopolitical organization of society. Surveying unknown indigenous modes of organization, scholars anchored their study in the challenge of determining the most “natural” and elementary unit of society possible as well as investigating how these units interacted and organized themselves into larger social entities. Ladislav Holy, in his book, Anthropological Perspectives on Kinship, sketches some of the different models of the elementary social unit. According to Holy, M. Fortes, in addition to many other anthropologists, believed a mother and her child to be the social unit and basis for all human existence (1996, p.29). Radcliffe-Brown, on the other hand, viewed the simplest unit to be a man, his wife and their child, that is, “the elementary family” (p.32). For Levi-Strauss, however, the avunculate, or a brother, his sister, his sister’s husband and his sister’s son is the elementary structure of society (p.36). Yet the underlying reason for dealing so seriously with elementary units and secondary networks of relationships was the perception that “kinship” strongly influenced people about “whom to marry, where to live, how to raise children, which ancestors to worship, how to solve disputes, which land to cultivate, which property to inherit, whom to turn for help” (Holy, 1996, p.13). Quite early on anthropologists came to the conclusion that kinship ties, then, provided quite effective patterns of social organization. In their minds, the universality of kinship as social organizer derived from the superposition of social structures on biological/genealogical relationships (Schneider, 1972, p.33). Yet being as they were social modes of organization, these modes varied from culture to culture inasmuch as people’s imaginaries of genealogical relatedness differed widely. For researchers such as Morgan, kinship was a social approximation of the true biological facts of consanguinity and affinity (p.34), an approximation that proved to be a substantial pillar of the cultures and worldviews of non-Western peoples. From this basis sprang many complex studies involving concepts of consanguinity, descent, affinity and alliance based on the use of extensive genealogical charts, representing “a kind of ‘purified vision’ of the people from whom they were collected” (Holy, 1996, p.31). By drawing relations on paper, and classifying these diverse systems of kinship, researchers felt they were progressively elucidating the principles of social organization underlining both simple and complex societies. On the other hand, the more the study of kinship evolved, the more individual anthropologists questioned its usefulness and validity out of a concern for its convolution and mushrooming jargon. As a result, in the 1960s and 70s certain academics such as Leach and Needham embarked on a formalist endeavor, trying to leave behind confusing categories and appealing to “purely formal criteria” to delineate the logical possibilities structuring kinship (Needham, 1971, p.10). Needham, for his part, distinguished six elementary modes of descent that could be used for the transmission of rights (p.11). Yet the attempts to apply logic to the analysis of systems of social organization, while possibly quite elegant, oftentimes missed the very dynamic processes of kinship, that is, the building of as well as the emotional experience inherent in relationships. In any case, at one point anthropologists began to recognize a clash of interests, methods and theories between those interested in social patterns and those interested in cultural meanings. David Schneider, in contrast to Radcliffe Brown, considered the study of kinship to belong to the cultural arena of anthropology. Being as it were a “system of symbols and meanings”, cultures unconsciously determined the normative rules and actual practice of any given society (1972, p.40). Schneider’s strong critique was that kinship scholars kept trying to reduce the cultural level to the social organizational level, using kinship theories and methods (the genealogical chart) based on ethnocentric understandings of family and biology that by their simple application irrevocably damaged analyses (p.56). From Schneider’s perspective, “[o]ne must take the native’s own categories…and articulation of those categories and follow their definitions, their symbolic and meaningful division wherever they may lead” (p.51). Leaving behind their attachment to the “indisputable and unavoidable facts of life”, anthropologists should be able to recognize that for different societies different facts are indisputable and unavoidable, and they may or may not agree, wholly or in part, with the Western perception of nature, biology and reproduction. In seeking to explain patterns of diffuse and enduring solidarity—Schneider’s own definition of kinship (p.47)—anthropologists should only appeal to emic conceptions of social relations, which would emerge as a symbolic structure much more coherent and fluid than the disorderly kinship terminologies generated by previous work. Following Schneider’s seminal article, anthropological theory was confronted by the feminist revolution and the emergence of new reproductive technologies in the field of medicine, which challenged traditional understandings of gender and reproduction. Given that embryos fertilized in vitro from possibly anonymous parents could be implanted in surrogate mothers and passed on for adoption by other couples, the “manifest destiny” of the biological and subsequently familial bond between genitor, genetrix, and child looked uncertain and possibly unfounded. Furthermore, recent kinship studies converging on European and American societies brought to light the strong cultural basis for the conceptualization of biology, science and nature in the Western mind. Accordingly, anthropologists now recognize that not only “biology does not everywhere have the kind of foundational function it has in the West, but…the boundaries between the biological and the social…are distinctly blurred, if they are visible at all” (Carsten, 2000, p.3). While freed from a not always salutary obligation to match genealogies, kinship as a theoretical concept suffered from losing its claim to a biological framework of reference. In Holy’s analysis, without some anchoring of the term, “virtually any relationship could be classified as a kinship one if one so wished” (1996, p.169), and we risk unproductive comparative work not having criteria from which to compare like to like. In the introduction to the book, Cultures of Relatedness, Carsten’s resolution to this quandary is to try and usher a new paradigm relying on the term relatedness. For her, the breakthrough in kinship studies is to reposition research to the tune of the sharing of substance, space and food and to focus on the production of relatedness through doing and being together (2000, p.34). The hard work of maintaining relationships, the negative aspects of kinship and the termination of kinship bonds are areas of research that appear to have been neglected based on the previous conception of kinship being given rather than socially negotiated. According to Carsten, the idea of relatedness opens these and other frontiers, such as the role of material culture in kinship relations, to anthropological study. Even so, while the notion of relatedness must encompass a breadth of phenomena to be useful cross-culturally, it still risks losing itself in undefined generality. In an interesting twist to the conclusion of his book, Holy presents two possible ideas to substitute for kinship and to refine the idea of relatedness. The first, quoted from R. C. Kelly’s work, is that of “social relations predicated upon…processes by which an individual comes into being and develops into a complete (i.e. mature) social person” (1996, p.170). Tracing the people who deeply contributed to one’s physical, social and spiritual formation seems like a feasible way of defining one’s kin, once ideas of development, personhood and maturity are well understood within each culture. In parallel to but in a slightly more specific vein than Carsten, Holy himself proposes to concentrate on the particular set of relations deriving from the social perception of the sharing of bodily and spiritual substances (p.171). A step up from the traditional standard of the sharing of genetic content, a broader conception of the significance of sharing for the development of strong social ties would be of much value in terms of the already mentioned trends of migration and smaller families. Given the fluidity of human social connections, it is clear that whatever roles extensive kinship systems fulfilled in the past, these will have to be realized through new embodiments and reenactments of intimacy and trust. Overall, each time the controversial concept of kinship has been criticized it has taken social anthropology much soul-searching and theoretical refashioning to produce a reincarnation. This deeply reflective process has brought to the fore implicit and explicit understandings of family, social and psychological connection, the divide (real or constructed) between nature and culture and the origins of gender. Yet the renewed significance of meaning in the study of relatedness must not overshadow the importance of grounding it in the context of economic, political and historical factors. These are essential for analyzing the increased movement of people, ideas and goods that threaten to overthrow traditional conceptions of kinship everywhere. Confronted with such rapid, global transformations and the potential for conflict and instability, our global society depends more than ever on the sharing of responsibilities and resources and on peaceful negotiation of action. Given any interpretation of present developments, the anthropological interest in the phenomena of relatedness and concomitant solidarity— especially, in Schneider’s words, long-term patterns of diffuse, enduring solidarity (1972, p.47)—is absolutely relevant. Bibliography Carsten, J. 2000. Cultures of Relatedness Cambridge: CUP Holy, L. 1996. Anthropological perspectives on kinship. London, Chicago: Pluto Press Leach, E. R. 1961. Rethinking Anthropology London: Athlone Press Needham, R. 1971. Rethinking Kinship and Marriage London: Tavistock Radcliffe-Brown, A.R. 1953. Structure and Function in Primitive Society. London: Cohen and West. Schneider, D. 1972 ‘What is kinship all about?’ in P. Reining (ed.) Kinship Studies in the Morgan Centennial Year. Washington D.C.: Anthropological Society of Washington